Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

The cross-sectional GRAS sample: A comprehensive phenotypical data collection of schizophrenic patients

Abstract

Background

Schizophrenia is the collective term for an exclusively clinically diagnosed, heterogeneous group of mental disorders with still obscure biological roots. Based on the assumption that valuable information about relevant genetic and environmental disease mechanisms can be obtained by association studies on patient cohorts of ≥ 1000 patients, if performed on detailed clinical datasets and quantifiable biological readouts, we generated a new schizophrenia data base, the GRAS (Göttingen Research Association for Schizophrenia) data collection. GRAS is the necessary ground to study genetic causes of the schizophrenic phenotype in a 'phenotype-based genetic association study' (PGAS). This approach is different from and complementary to the genome-wide association studies (GWAS) on schizophrenia.

Methods

For this purpose, 1085 patients were recruited between 2005 and 2010 by an invariable team of traveling investigators in a cross-sectional field study that comprised 23 German psychiatric hospitals. Additionally, chart records and discharge letters of all patients were collected.

Results

The corresponding dataset extracted and presented in form of an overview here, comprises biographic information, disease history, medication including side effects, and results of comprehensive cross-sectional psychopathological, neuropsychological, and neurological examinations. With >3000 data points per schizophrenic subject, this data base of living patients, who are also accessible for follow-up studies, provides a wide-ranging and standardized phenotype characterization of as yet unprecedented detail.

Conclusions

The GRAS data base will serve as prerequisite for PGAS, a novel approach to better understanding 'the schizophrenias' through exploring the contribution of genetic variation to the schizophrenic phenotypes.

Peer Review reports

Background

Schizophrenia is a devastating brain disease that affects approximately 1% of the population across cultures [1]. The diagnosis of schizophrenia or - perhaps more correctly - of 'the schizophrenias' is still purely clinical, requiring the coincident presence of symptoms as listed in the leading classification systems, DSM-IV and ICD-10 [2, 3].

Notably, one of the core symptoms of schizophrenia, namely cognitive deficits, from mild impairments to full-blown dementia, has not yet been considered in these classifications. Biologically, schizophrenia is a 'mixed bag' of diseases that undoubtedly have a strong genetic root. Family studies exploring relative risk of schizophrenia have led to estimates of heritability of about 64-88% [4, 5]. Monozygotic twin studies showing concordance rates of 41-65% [6, 7] indicate a considerable amount of non-genetic causes, in the following referred to as 'environmental factors'. Already in the middle of the twentieth century, schizophrenia was seen as a 'polygenetic' disease [8] and, indeed, in numerous genetic studies since, ranging from segregation or linkage analyses, genome scans and large association studies, no major 'schizophrenia gene' has been identified [9]. Even recent genome-wide association studies (GWAS) on schizophrenia confirm that several distinct loci are associated with the disease. These studies concentrated on endpoint diagnosis and found odds ratios for single markers in different genomic regions ranging from 0.68 to 6.01 [10], essentially underlining the fact that - across ethnicities - in most cases these genotypes do not contribute more to the disease than a slightly increased probability.

We hypothesize that an interplay of multiple causative factors, perhaps thousands of potential combinations of genes/genetic markers and an array of different environmental risks, leads to the development of 'the schizophrenias', as schematically illustrated in Figure 1. There may be cases with a critical genetic load already present without need of additional external co-factors, however, in most individuals, an interaction of a certain genetic predisposition with environmental co-factors is apparently required for disease onset. In fact, not too much of an overlap may exist between genetic risk factors from one schizophrenic patient to an unrelated other schizophrenic individual, explaining why it is basically impossible to find common risk genes of schizophrenia with appreciable odds ratios. One GRAS working hypothesis is that in the overwhelming majority of cases, schizophrenia is the result of a 'combination of unfortunate genotypes'.

Figure 1
figure 1

Schizophrenia is a complex multigenetic disease. Schizophrenia may be seen as the result of a multifaceted interplay between multiple causative factors, including several genetic markers and a variety of different environmental risks. Cases with a critical genetic load may not need additional external/environmental co-factors, whilst in others, the interaction of a certain genetic predisposition with environmental co-factors is required for disease onset (modified from [84]).

If along the lines of traditional human genetics all attempts to define schizophrenia as a 'classical' genetic disease have largely failed, how can we learn more about the contribution of genes/genotypes to the disease phenotype? Rather than searching by GWAS for yet other schizophrenia risk genes, we designed an alternative and widely complementary approach, termed PGAS (phenotype-based genetic association study), in order to explore the contribution of certain genes/genetic markers to the schizophrenic phenotype. To launch PGAS, we had to establish a comprehensive phenotypical data base of schizophrenic patients, the GRAS (Göttingen Research Association for Schizophrenia) data collection. Very recently, we have been able to demonstrate proof-of-concept for the PGAS approach [[11], and Grube et al: Calcium-activated potassium channels as regulators of cognitive performance in schizophrenia, submitted].

Large data bases of schizophrenic patients have been instigated for decades to perform linkage/family studies, treatment trials, genetic or epidemiological studies applying either a cross-sectional or a longitudinal design (e.g. [12–20]). However, for the above introduced PGAS approach, another type of data base is required, and only few of the existing data banks are suited for phenotypical analyses. An example is the 'Clinical Antipsychotic Trial of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE)', originally set up as a treatment study comparing a first generation antipsychotic drug with several second generation antipsychotics in a multisite randomized double-blind trial [17, 21]. The huge amount of data accumulated in the frame of this trial serves now also for GWAS and genotype-phenotype association studies [22–25]. Disadvantages may be that the CATIE data were collected by different examiners in 57 US sites and that comprehensive data for phenotypical analyses are only available for subsamples of the originally included 1493 patients. Another example of a large data base with considerable phenotypical power is the 'Australian Schizophrenia Research Bank (ASRB)' [26]. ASRB operates to collect, store and distribute linked clinical, cognitive, neuroimaging and genetic data from a large sample of patients with schizophrenia (at present nearly 500) and healthy controls (almost 300) [27, 28]).

The present paper has been designed (1) to introduce the GRAS data collection, set up as prerequisite and platform for PGAS; (2) to exemplify on some selected areas of interest the potential of phenotypical readouts derived from the GRAS data collection and their internal consistency; (3) to provide a first panel of epidemiological data as a 'side harvest' of this data base; and (4) to enable interested researchers worldwide to initiate scientific collaborations based on this data base.

Methods

Ethics

The GRAS data collection has been approved by the ethical committee of the Georg-August-University of Göttingen (master committee) as well as by the respective local regulatories/ethical committees of all collaborating centers (Table 1). The distribution of the centers over Germany together with information on the numbers of recruited patients per center is presented in Figure 2.

Table 1 GRAS data collection manual: Table of contents
Figure 2
figure 2

Collaborating centers and patient numbers. Map of Germany displaying the locations of all 23 collaborating centers that were visited by an invariable team of traveling investigators. The table next to the map provides numbers of patients examined in each center. Some centers were visited more than once.

GRAS patients

From September 2005 to July 2008, a total of 1071 patients were examined by the GRAS team of traveling investigators after giving written informed consent, own and/or authorized legal representatives. Since then, low-rate steady state recruitment has been ongoing, among others to build up a new cohort for replicate analyses of genotype-phenotype associations. As of July 2010, 1085 patients have been entered into the data base. They were examined in different settings: 348 (32.1%) as outpatients, 474 (43.7%) as inpatients in psychiatric hospitals, 189 (17.4%) as residents in sheltered homes, 54 (5%) as patients in specific forensic units, and 20 (1.8%) as day clinic patients. Inclusion criteria were (1) confirmed or suspected diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder according to DSM-IV and (2) at least some ability to cooperate. Recruitment efficiency over the core travel/field study time from 2005 to 2008 and patient flow are shown in Figures 3a and 3b. Of the 1085 patients entered into the data base, a total of 1037 fulfilled the diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. For 48 patients the diagnosis of schizophrenia could not be ultimately confirmed upon careful re-check and follow-up. Of the schizophrenic patients, 96% completed the GRAS assessment whereas about 4% dropped out during the examination. Almost all patients agreed to be re-contacted for potential follow-up studies, only 1.5% were either lost to follow-up (present address unknown or deceased) or did not give consent to be contacted again.

Figure 3
figure 3

Patient recruitment and flow: (a) Recruitment efficiency 2005 - 2008. Cumulative numbers of recruited patients per quarter of the year are shown in bar graphs. Note that steady-state recruitment is ongoing. (b) Patient flow. Of 1085 patients examined, the diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder could not be confirmed for 48. Instead, alternative diagnoses had to be given.

Healthy control subjects

(1) For genetic analyses, control subjects, who gave written informed consent, were voluntary blood donors, recruited by the Department of Transfusion Medicine at the Georg-August-University of Göttingen according to national guidelines for blood donation. As such, they widely fulfill health criteria, ensured by a broad pre-donation screening process containing standardized questionnaires, interviews, hemoglobin, blood pressure, pulse, and body temperature determinations. Of the total of 2265 subjects, 57.5% are male (n = 1303) and 42.5% female (n = 962). The average age is 33.8 ± 12.2 years, with a range from 18 to 69 years. Participation as healthy controls for the GRAS sample was anonymous, with information restricted to age, gender, blood donor health state and ethnicity. Comparable to the patient population (Table 2), almost all control subjects were of European Caucasian descent (Caucasian 97.8%; other ethnicities 2%; unknown 0.2%). (2) For selected cognitive measures and olfactory testing, 103 additional healthy volunteers were recruited as control subjects (matched with respect to age, gender, and smoking habits). These healthy controls include 67.0% male (n = 69) and 33.0% (n = 34) female subjects with an average age of 39.02 ± 13.87 years, ranging from 18 to 71 years.

Table 2 GRAS sample description

Traveling team

The GRAS team of traveling investigators consisted of 1 trained psychiatrist and neurologist, 3 psychologists and 4 medical doctors/last year medical students. All investigators had continuous training and calibration sessions to ensure the highest possible agreement on diagnoses and other judgments as well as a low interrater variability regarding the instruments applied. Patient contacts were usually prepared by colleagues/personnel in the respective collaborating psychiatric centers (Figure 2) to make the work of the travel team as efficient as possible.

The GRAS manual

A standardized procedure for examination of the patients has been arranged with the GRAS manual, composed for the purpose of the GRAS data collection. Table 1 presents its contents, including established instruments, such as clinical interviews/ratings, questionnaires, cognitive and neurological tests [2, 29–53].

GRAS operating procedure

The GRAS data base operating procedure leading from the large set of raw data provided by the travel team to the data bank with its several-fold controlled and verified data points is illustrated in Figure 4. Already during the time when the travel team examined patients all over Germany, a team of psychologists started to work on the development of the GRAS data base, integrating the raw data to ultimately result in over 3000 phenotypic data points per patient (total of over 3.000 000 data points at present in the data collection) (Figure 5). Most importantly, the chart records/medical reports of all patients were carefully screened, missing records identified and, in numerous, sometimes extensive and repeated, telephone and written conversations, missing psychiatric discharge letters of every single patient organized. After careful study and pre-processing of raw data and chart records, the confirmation of the diagnoses, determination of age of onset of the disease and prodrome as well as other essential readouts were achieved by meticulous consensus decisions.

Figure 4
figure 4

Development of the GRAS data bank. Raw data, brought to Göttingen by the traveling team of examiners, were only entered into the data base after careful and comprehensive data re-checking, also based on patient charts and discharge letters. During the whole process, continuous calibration sessions and repeated re-checking of the entered data took place.

Figure 5
figure 5

Phenotype overview. Various different domains covered by the GRAS data collection are displayed. These domains will also deliver the basis for further sophistication of phenotypical readouts.

Statistical analyses

For the establishment of the data base and for basic statistical analyses of the data, SPSS for Windows version 17.0 [54] was used. Comparisons of men and women in terms of sociodemographic and clinical picture as well as neurological examination were assessed using either Mann-Whitney-U or Chi-square test. Prior to correlation and regression analyses, selected metric phenotypic variables were standardized by Blom transformation [55]. The Blom transformation is a probate transformation into ranks and the resulting standardized values are normally distributed with zero mean and variance one. Comparisons of men and women in terms of cognitive performance were assessed by analyses of covariance, using age, duration of disease, years of education and chlorpromazine equivalents as covariates. For all intercorrelation patterns, correlations of the particular target variables were assessed using Pearson product-moment correlation. Cronbach's alpha coefficient was determined for estimation of internal consistency of the target variables within a defined intercorrelation pattern. Multiple regression analyses using the enter method were conducted to evaluate the contribution of selected disease related variables (duration of disease, positive symptoms, negative symptoms, catatonic signs and chlorpromazine equivalents) to 3 dependent variables: basic cognition/fine motor functions, cognitive functions and global functioning (GAF) [2]. The dependent variables basic cognition/fine motor functions and cognitive functions are both composite score variables. The basic cognition/fine motor function score comprises alertness (TAP), dotting and tapping (Cronbach's alpha = .801) [39, 46] and the cognition score consists of reasoning (LPS3), 2 processing speed measures (TMT-A and digit-symbol test, ZST), executive functions (TMT-B), working memory (BZT), verbal learning & memory (VLMT) and divided attention (TAP) [37, 38, 41, 44–46] (Cronbach's alpha = .869). For both scores, a Cronbach's alpha >.80 indicates a high internal consistency as prerequisite for integrating several distinct items into one score. Multiple regression analyses were conducted for the total sample and separated for men and women.

Results

Biographic and clinical data

The GRAS data collection comprises presently (as of August 2010) 1037 patients with confirmed diagnosis of schizophrenia (82.2%) or schizoaffective disorder (17.8%). A total of 693 men and 344 women fulfilled the respective diagnostic requirements of DSM-IV. Table 2 provides a sample description, both total and separated for male and female patients, with respect to sociodemographic data and clinical picture. There are some differences between genders in the GRAS sample: Women are older, less single, have more years of education, more diagnoses of schizoaffective disorders, longer duration of disease, later age of onset of first psychotic episode and lower doses of antipsychotics. However, regarding determinants of the clinical picture, e.g. PANSS scores [30], genders do not differ significantly. An intercorrelation pattern of selected clinical readouts, obtained by (1) clinical ratings and (2) self-ratings of the patients and complemented by (3) 'objective data', in this case medication and hospitalization, is presented in Figure 6. The Cronbach's alpha of .753 suggests that items derived from the 3 different perspectives harmonize well. Whereas patient ratings of quality of life and state anxiety (STAI) [32] are only weakly correlated with professional clinical ratings and objective data, the patients' self-estimated symptom burden as measured with the BSI [33] shows moderate to good correlation.

Figure 6
figure 6

Clinical intercorrelation pattern. Correlations between measures of the clinical picture derived from different approaches: Patient self-ratings, clinical rater judgement and 'objective data'. Thickness of the lines represents the strength of correlation between two measures; only significant correlations are displayed. Note the strong internal consistency expressed by a Cronbach's alpha of .753.

Cognition

For the ongoing/planned genetic analyses, not only the clinical picture with its schizophrenia-typical positive and negative symptoms, but particularly cognition plays an important role. The cognitive tests applied in the GRAS data collection show an intercorrelation pattern that further underlines quality and internal consistency of the data obtained by the invariable team of investigators (Figure 7). Table 3 represents the cognitive performance data of the complete GRAS sample in the respective domains. In addition, the performance level of men and women is given as well as - for comparison - available normative data of healthy individuals. Since for dotting and tapping [39], no normative data were available in the literature, the values shown in Table 3 were obtained from the healthy GRAS control population for cognitive measures (n = 103; see patients and methods).

Figure 7
figure 7

Cognitive intercorrelation pattern. Shown are all neuropsychological tests performed, together with their respective cognitive domain. Thickness of the lines represents the strength of correlation between two tests; only significant correlations are displayed. Tests for higher cognitive functions are labelled in orange; tests for basic (mainly basic cognition/fine motor dependent) functions in grey. Measures of higher cognitive functions as well as measures of basic cognition/fine motor functions show powerful internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha of .819 and .801 respectively).

Table 3 Cognitive performance of GRAS patients. For comparison, normative data are presented wherever available2.

Comparing cognitive performance of schizophrenic men and women, analyses of covariance have been conducted, with age, duration of disease, years of education and chlorpromazine equivalents as covariates, which revealed significant gender differences in discrete cognitive domains. Men performed better in reasoning (F = 17.62, p <.001), alertness (F = 28.30, p <.001 for reaction time and F = 10.39, p = .001 for lapses), and divided attention (F = 14.07 p <.001 for reaction time and F = 22.12, p <.001 for lapses). In contrast, female schizophrenic patients were superior in verbal memory tasks (F = 12.38, p <.001) and digit symbol test (F = 19.24, p <.001). With respect to normative data obtained from healthy controls, cognitive data of all schizophrenic patients are in the lower normal range (percentile rank = 16 for digit symbol test) or even below (percentile ranks 10 for verbal memory, TMT-A, TMT-B, alertness and divided attention). Only for reasoning (LPS) [37] and premorbid intelligence (MWT-B) [36], schizophrenic subjects lie in the average range (percentile ranks of 31 and 43.5 respectively).

Antipsychotic medication and side effects

Another important feature of schizophrenic patients that may influence their every-day functioning and performance, and result in a considerable number of side effects, is their antipsychotic medication. The GRAS data collection contains information on type, dose, duration of medication and drugs prescribed over the years. The mean dose of present antipsychotic medication of the whole GRAS population, expressed as chlorpromazine equivalents [56] amounts to 687.36 (± 696.85). Chlorpromazine equivalents in male are significantly higher as compared to female patients (Table 2). An overview of self-reported side effects of current antipsychotic medication in the GRAS sample, again sorted by gender, is given in Table 4. Of the 1037 patients with confirmed diagnosis of schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder, 24 were presently not on antipsychotic drugs, whilst for 1 patient the current medication was unknown. Of the remaining 1012 patients who currently receive antipsychotic medication (16.5% first generation antipsychotics, 54.1% second generation antipsychotics and 29.4% mixed) and were all explicitly interviewed regarding medication side effects, only 423 reported any. The discrepancy between side effects measured versus side effects based on patients' reports becomes obvious when considering for instance the number of patients with clear extrapyramidal symptoms: A total of 335 subjects measured by Simpson-Angus Scale (mean score >.3) [50] contrasts only 117 patients self-reporting extrapyramidal complaints. External rating of extrapyramidal side effects in the GRAS population was comprehensively performed, utilizing a number of respective instruments which all showed significant intercorrelation (Figure 8) [47, 49–52, 57]. A composite score of the 6 Blom transformed scales, used for testing potential gender effects, yielded no significant differences in extrapyramidal symptoms in men versus women (Z = -0.022, p = 0.982).

Table 4 Self-reported medication side effects of patients (N = 423)* according to treatment type
Figure 8
figure 8

Extrapyramidal intercorrelation pattern. Shown are correlations between different neurological tests for measuring extrapyramidal symptoms. Thickness of the lines represents the strength of correlation between two tests; only significant correlations are displayed. Cronbach's alpha of .675 shows that these measures have a decent internal consistency.

Neurological symptoms

Similar to cognitive readouts, evaluation of inherent neurological symptoms in the schizophrenic patient population are of tremendous interest, not only for understanding the contribution of particular genes/genetic markers and/or environmental factors to the schizophrenic phenotype but also for estimating the impact of potential neurological comorbidities. Table 5 provides an overview of neurological symptoms based on the Cambridge Neurological Inventory (CNI) [47]. Only in the subscale 'Failure to suppress inappropriate response', significant differences between men and women (Z = -3.175, p = 0.001) became evident. Women were less able to hold respective responses back, e.g. to blink with one eye, leaving the other eye open, or to perform saccadic eye movements without moving the head.

Table 5 Cambridge Neurological Inventory (CNI)a subscale sum scores (N = 893-942)

Prediction of functioning

In order to delineate the influence of disease on functioning in the GRAS sample, multiple regression analyses have been employed. These procedures assessed the contribution of 5 disease-related variables, i.e. duration of disease, PANSS positive and negative scores [30], catatonic signs [47], and dose of antipsychotic medication, to 3 dependent performance variables: (a) basic cognition/fine motor functions, (b) cognitive performance and (c) global functioning (Table 6). Regarding basic cognition/fine motor function, multiple regression analysis revealed a significant model accounting for 32.4% of variance in the total sample. In fact, duration of disease, negative symptoms, catatonic signs, and medication (chlorpromazine equivalents) contributed significantly to basic cognition/fine motor function, whereas positive symptoms did not (β = -.006, p = .856). According to the standardized regression coefficients, duration of disease and negative symptoms are the best predictors of basic cognition/fine motor function (β = -.346, p < .001 and β = -.334, p < .001). For higher cognitive functions, the set of disease-related variables explained 33% of variance in the total sample. Again, duration of disease and negative symptoms are the best predictors of higher cognitive functions (β = -.335, p < .001 and β = -.351, p < .001). Positive symptoms did not reach significance (β = - .015, p = .658). With respect to global functioning, all chosen disease-related factors accounted for 59.6% of variance in the total sample. Only duration of disease per se did not reach significance (β = -.028, p = .198). Positive and negative symptoms were the strongest predictors of global functioning (β = - .441, p < .001 and β = -.380, p < .001).

Table 6 Multiple regression analyses predicting a) basic cognition/fine motor functions, b) cognitive performance, c) global functioning

Discussion

The present paper provides an overview of the GRAS data collection, including (1) study logistics and procedures, (2) sample description regarding sociodemographic data, disease-related variables, cognitive performance and neurological symptoms, paying particular attention to gender differences, and (3) a first presentation of intercorrelation patterns for selected areas of interest to phenotype studies. (4) In addition, disease-related factors influencing important criteria of daily functioning are evaluated in the >1000 GRAS patients. Overall, the GRAS sample represents a typical schizophrenic population in contact with the health system and is - last not least due to its homogeneous data acquisition - ideally suited for the ongoing and planned phenotype-based genetic association studies (PGAS) (e.g. [[11], and Grube et al: Calcium-activated potassium channels as regulators of cognitive performance in schizophrenia, submitted]).

The GRAS data collection has several remarkable advantages, two of which are of major importance for its ultimate goal, PGAS: (i) Different from other studies dealing with the establishment of a schizophrenia data base, all data for GRAS were collected by one and the same traveling team of examiners, who frequently performed calibrating sessions and rater trainings. This effort has clearly paid off in terms of reliability and quality of the data, considering the internal consistencies of the GRAS phenotypes, as exemplified in the displayed correlation patterns. (ii) Even though the GRAS study has been implemented as a cross-sectional investigation, the GRAS data collection also includes solid longitudinal information derived from the almost complete psychiatric chart records/discharge letters of all schizophrenic patients. This longitudinal set of data has been essential to e.g. reliably estimate prodrome versus disease onset, i.e. occurrence of the first psychotic episode.

Comparable to other schizophrenia samples, the GRAS sample comprises around two thirds of male and one third of female patients [17, 58]. Assuming that the gender ratio in schizophrenia were 1:1 as claimed in text books, but recently also questioned [59, 60], then two principal reasons may account for the gender distribution observed here: (1) Schizophrenic women generally seem to have less contact with the health system due to being better socially settled (later age of onset of disease) and protected within their families [61]; (2) A certain (smaller) recruitment bias may be explained by the fact that the traveling team of examiners visited some institutions with an overrepresentation of males, e.g. specialized forensic units or a hospital for psychotic patients with co-morbid substance use disorders.

With the purposeful strategy to visit several different facilities of psychiatric health care covering inpatients, outpatients, residents of sheltered homes and forensic patients, the GRAS approach tried to avoid biases inherent to pure inpatient samples [58]. Nevertheless, patients who are not in contact with the health care system are unlikely to be integrated in any comparable data bases. For instance, only 4 of the 1085 examined patients are currently homeless, whereas among homeless people a considerable proportion suffers from schizophrenia [62]. To reach them as well, different and more cost intensive recruitment strategies would be required [13]. On the other hand, the schizophrenic phenotype required for the GRAS-PGAS studies pursued here, might be veiled in this severely affected subsample of patients that is additionally characterized by other specific problems, e.g. a highly elevated incidence of multiple substance use disorders and severe downstream medical comorbidities [63, 64].

Gender differences in schizophrenia as obvious from the present data collection have been known for a long time [65]. In agreement with the literature, men and women in the GRAS sample differ by diagnosis, with women having a higher rate of schizoaffective disorders [66, 67]. With respect to age of onset, education, indicators of social integration (e.g. marital status, living situation) and medication, the present results are also in perfect agreement with previous findings: Male patients are younger when the first psychotic episode occurs, are more frequently single, more often dependent on supported living conditions (e.g. residential homes) and show lower educational status [61, 67, 68]. Among the explanations for the observed gender differences in schizophrenia are the protective role of female hormones [69] and social aspects like earlier marriage of young women leading to a more protected environment at disease onset [13]. In line with these considerations is the work of Häfner and colleagues [12]. In a prospective design he could show that 'the social course (of schizophrenia) is determined by individual stage at illness onset and by early illness course' [70].

With respect to psychopathology and premorbid functioning, the GRAS sample may be slightly different from other schizophrenia samples reported in the literature [67]. Several studies published in this area show that men exhibit more negative symptoms, even in a geriatric sample [71, 72], and that females have poorer premorbid cognitive functioning than males [73]. In the GRAS patients, there are no gender differences regarding psychopathology and premorbid cognition. Importantly, clear support for a comparable severity of psychopathology in men and women of the GRAS sample is provided by the lack of gender differences in numbers of hospitalizations, clinical severity ratings, including global functioning (CGI, GAF [2, 31]), and self-ratings of symptom severity and anxiety. One potential explanation for the discrepancies between the GRAS sample and other studies regarding psychopathology may be that patient numbers in some of the other studies have been too low to give conclusive results. In the assessment of premorbid cognitive functioning of the GRAS sample, a methodological limitation could be the retrospective determination of premorbid intelligence using a so-called 'hold' measure, i.e. a multiple choice vocabulary test [35]. Even though this is an accepted and valid instrument to retrospectively estimate premorbid intelligence [74], a prospective procedure might be more accurate. In fact, Weiser and colleagues had the opportunity to base their assessments on cognitive testing performed on adolescents before starting their military service [73], potentially explaining the deviating results.

Gender differences regarding current cognitive performance are similar within the GRAS sample (even though at a lower functioning level [75]) compared to healthy controls [76] after considering age of onset, duration of disease, education and medication as covariates. Men perform better in reasoning, alertness and divided attention but worse in verbal memory, confirming reports on first-episode as well as chronically ill schizophrenic patients [77].

Women in the GRAS study receive significantly lower doses of chlorpromazine equivalents, confirming that they require less medication to achieve a reasonable treatment effect [78]. Importantly, regarding medication side effects, there were no gender differences in extrapyramidal symptoms. There were also no differences in the overall proportion of men and women who self-reported side effects, but the pattern of complaints was slightly different. For instance, women mentioned more often hormonal dysfunction and vertigo (or related symptoms like hypotonia), whilst men complained mainly about sexual dysfunction. Altogether, it is worth pointing out that the percentage of patients self-reporting side effects is low when compared to that with objectively measured side effects, e.g. extrapyramidal symptoms (11.3% versus 32.3%).

Explicit studies on gender differences in antipsychotic medication side effects found a somewhat different distribution of complaints, e.g. more weight gain, diabetes and specific cardiovascular diseases in females [78, 79]. Here, one reason is certainly the still preliminary data set of the GRAS collection evaluated, based at this point exclusively on cross-sectional patient reports. For a more appropriate coverage of medication side effects, all charts/discharge letters of every GRAS patient (also of those patients who did/could not report them), will have to be screened and entered into the data base. Comprehensive information on antipsychotic (and other) drugs and their side effects in the GRAS sample has been collected and is waiting for analyses to support e.g. future pharmacogenomic approaches, perhaps also in collaboration with industry partners.

In line with the findings of a recent meta-analysis [80], positive symptoms of the GRAS patients do not influence higher cognitive function or basic cognition/fine motor performance, whilst negative symptoms, catatonic signs, duration of disease and antipsychotic medication have a significant effect on both. The clinical ratings of global functioning, however, strongly rely on positive as well as negative symptoms, medication and catatonic signs [81–83].

Conclusion

GRAS enables a novel phenotype-based approach to understand the molecular-genetic architecture of schizophrenia. The GRAS data collection encompasses a large sample of comprehensively phenotyped, moderately to severely affected schizophrenic patients. Proof-of-principle for the suitability of the GRAS data collection for PGAS has already been demonstrated [[11], and Grube et al: Calcium-activated potassium channels as regulators of cognitive performance in schizophrenia, submitted]. Further extensive analyses of the accumulated information on every single patient are ongoing.

Abbreviations

GRAS:

Göttingen Research Association for Schizophrenia

GWAS:

genome-wide association study

PGAS:

phenotype-based genetic association study

CATIE:

clinical antipsychotic trials of intervention effectiveness

CNI:

Cambridge Neurological Inventory

ASRB:

Australian Schizophrenia Research Bank

FGA:

first generation antipsychotics

SGA:

second generation antipsychotics

CPZ:

chlorpromazine.

References

  1. Jablensky A, Sartorius N, Ernberg G, Anker M, Korten A, Cooper JE, Day R, Bertelsen A: Schizophrenia: manifestations, incidence and course in different cultures. A World Health Organization ten-country study. Psychol Med Monogr Suppl. 1992, 20: 1-97. 10.1017/S0264180100000904.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, (DSM-IV). 1994, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 4

    Google Scholar 

  3. World Health Organization: The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. 1992, Geneva: WHO

    Google Scholar 

  4. Cardno AG, Gottesman II: Twin studies of schizophrenia: from bow-and-arrow concordances to starwars Mx and functional genomics. Am J Med Genet. 2000, 97 (1): 12-17. 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(200021)97:1<12::AID-AJMG3>3.0.CO;2-U.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Lichtenstein P, Yip BH, Björk C, Pawitan Y, Cannon TD, Sullivan PF, Hultman CM: Common genetic determinants of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in Swedish families: a population-based study. The Lancet. 2009, 373 (9659): 234-239. 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60072-6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Franzek E, Beckmann H: Different Genetic Background of Schizophrenia Spectrum Psychoses: A Twin Study. Am J Psychiatry. 1998, 155 (1): 76-83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Cardno AG, Marshall EJ, Coid B, Macdonald AM, Ribchester TR, Davies NJ, Venturi P, Jones LA, Lewis SW, Sham PC, Gottesman II, Farmer AE, McGuffin P, Reveley AM, Murray RM: Heritability Estimates for Psychotic Disorders: The Maudsley Twin Psychosis Series. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999, 56 (2): 162-168. 10.1001/archpsyc.56.2.162.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Gottesman I I, Shields J: A polygenic theory of schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1967, 58 (1): 199-205. 10.1073/pnas.58.1.199.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Riley B, Asherson PJ, McGuffin P: Genetics and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia. Edited by: Hirsch SR, Weinberger DR. 2003, Massachusetts: Blackwell Publishing

    Google Scholar 

  10. Duan J, Sanders AR, Gejman PV: Genome-wide approaches to schizophrenia. Brain Research Bulletin.

  11. Begemann M, Grube S, Papiol S, Malzahn D, Krampe H, Ribbe K, Friedrichs H, Radyushkin KA, El-Kordi A, Benseler F, Hannke K, Sperling S, Schwerdtfeger D, Thanhauser I, Gerchen MF, Ghorbani M, Gutwinski S, Hilmes C, Leppert R, Ronnenberg A, Sowislo J, Stawicki S, Stodtke M, Szuszies C, Reim K, Riggert J, Eckstein F, Falkai P, Bickeboller H, Nave KA, et al: Modification of cognitive performance in schizophrenia by complexin 2 gene polymorphisms. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010, 67 (9): 879-888. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.107.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Häfner H, Maurer K, Löffler W, an der Heiden W, Munk-Jørgensen P, Hambrecht M, Riecher-Rössler A: The ABC schizophrenia study: a preliminary overview of the results. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 1998, 33 (8): 380-386. 10.1007/s001270050069.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Jablensky A, McGrath J, Herrman H, Castle D, Gureje O, Evans M, Carr V, Morgan V, Korten A, Harvey C: Psychotic disorders in urban areas: an overview of the Study on Low Prevalence Disorders. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2000, 34 (2): 221-236.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Haro JM, Edgell ET, Jones PB, Alonso J, Gavart S, Gregor KJ, Wright P, Knapp M: The European Schizophrenia Outpatient Health Outcomes (SOHO) study: rationale, methods and recruitment. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2003, 107 (3): 222-232. 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.00064.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Karayiorgou M, Torrington M, Abecasis GR, Pretorius H, Robertson B, Kaliski S, Lay S, Sobin C, Möller N, Lundy SL, Blundell ML, Gogos JA, Roos JL: Phenotypic characterization and genealogical tracing in an Afrikaner schizophrenia database. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2004, 124B (1): 20-28. 10.1002/ajmg.b.20090.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Fleischhacker WW, Keet IP, Kahn RS: The European First Episode Schizophrenia Trial (EUFEST): rationale and de sign of the trial. Schizophr Res. 2005, 78 (2-3): 147-156. 10.1016/j.schres.2005.06.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Lieberman JA, Stroup TS, McEvoy JP, Swartz MS, Rosenheck RA, Perkins DO, Keefe RS, Davis SM, Davis CE, Lebowitz BD, Severe J, Hsiao JK: Effectiveness of antipsychotic drugs in patients with chronic schizophrenia. N Engl J Med. 2005, 353 (12): 1209-1223. 10.1056/NEJMoa051688.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Price GW, Michie PT, Johnston J, Innes-Brown H, Kent A, Clissa P, Jablensky AV: A Multivariate Electrophy siological Endophenotype, from a Unitary Cohort, Shows Greater Research Utility than Any Single Feature in the Western Australian Family Study of Schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 2006, 60 (1): 1-10. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, Cadenhead KS, Olincy A, Freedman R, Green MF, Greenwood TA, Gur RE, Gur RC, Light GA, Mintz J, Nuechterlein KH, Radant AD, Schork NJ, Seidman LJ, Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Swerdlow NR, Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Turetsky BI, Braff DL: The Consortium on the Genetics of Endophenotypes in Schizophrenia: Model Recruitment, Assessment, and Endophenotyping Methods for a Multisite Collaboration. Schizophr Bull. 2007, 33 (1): 33-48. 10.1093/schbul/sbl044.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Allen NC, Bagade S, McQueen MB, Ioannidis JPA, Kavvoura FK, Khoury MJ, Tanzi RE, Bertram L: Systematic meta-analy ses and field synopsis of genetic association studies in schizophrenia: the SzGene database. Nat Genet. 2008, 40 (7): 827-834. 10.1038/ng.171.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Stroup TS, McEvoy JP, Swartz MS, Byerly MJ, Glick ID, Canive JM, McGee MF, Simpson GM, Stevens MC, Lieberman JA: The National Institute of Mental Health Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) project: schizophrenia trial design and protocol development. Schizophr Bull. 2003, 29 (1): 15-31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Poyastro Pinheiro A, Keefe RSE, Skelly T, Olarte M, Leviel K, Lange LA, Lange EM, Scott Stroup T, Lieberman J, Sullivan PF: AKT1 and neurocognition in schizophrenia. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2009, 41 (2): 169-177. 10.1080/00048670601109956.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Sullivan PF, Keefe RSE, Lange LA, Lange EM, Stroup TS, Lieberman J, Maness PF: NCAM1 and Neurocognition in Schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 2007, 61 (7): 902-910. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.036.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Crowley JJ, Keefe RSE, Perkins DO, Stroup TS, Lieberman JA, Sullivan PF: The neuregulin 1 promoter polymorphism rs6994992 is not associated with chronic schizophrenia or neurocognition. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2008, 147B (7): 1298-1300. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30727.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Sullivan PF, Lin D, Tzeng JY, van den Oord E, Perkins D, Stroup TS, Wagner M, Lee S, Wright FA, Zou F, Liu W, Downing AM, Lieberman J, Close SL: Genomewide association for schizophrenia in the CATIE study: results of stage 1. Mol Psychiatry. 2008, 13 (6): 570-584. 10.1038/mp.2008.25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Australian Schizophrenia Research Bank. [http://www.schizophreniaresearch.org.au/bank/index.php]

  27. Loughland CM, Carr VJ, Lewin TJ: The NISAD Schizophrenia Research Register: why do we need a database of schizophrenia volunteers?. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2001, 35 (5): 660-667. 10.1080/0004867010060516.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Loughland C, Draganic D, McCabe K, Richards J, Nasir A, Allen J, Catts S, Jablensky A, Henskens F, Michie P, Mowry B, Pantelis C, Schall U, Rodney S, Tooney P, Carr V: The Australian Schizophrenia Research Bank: A database of comprehensive clinical, endophenotypic and genetic data for etiological studies of schizophrenia. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry.

  29. Wittchen H-U, Zaudig M, Fydrich T: SKID-I (Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV; Achse I: Psychische Störungen). 1997, Göttingen: Hogrefe

    Google Scholar 

  30. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA: The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987, 13 (2): 261-276.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Guy W: Clinical Global Impression (CGI). ECDEU Assessment manual for psychopharmacology, revised National Institue of Mental Health. Rockville, MD. 1976

    Google Scholar 

  32. Laux L, Glanzmann P, Schaffner P, Spielberger CD: Das State-Trait-Angstinventar (STAI). 1981, Weinheim: Beltz

    Google Scholar 

  33. Franke GH: Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI). 2000, Goettingen: Beltz

    Google Scholar 

  34. Kupfer J, Brosig B, Braehler E: Toronto Alexithymie-Skala-26 (TAS-26). 2001, Goettingen: Hogrefe

    Google Scholar 

  35. Lehrl S, Triebig G, Fischer B: Multiple choice vocabulary test MWT as a valid and short test to estimate premorbid intelligence. Acta Neurol Scand. 1995, 91 (5): 335-345. 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1995.tb07018.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Lehrl S: Mehrfach-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest MWT-B. 1999, Balingen: Spitta Verlag

    Google Scholar 

  37. Horn W: Leistungsprüfsystem (LPS). 1983, Goettingen: Hogrefe, 2

    Google Scholar 

  38. Gold JM, Carpenter C, Randolph C, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR: Auditory working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997, 54 (2): 159-165.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Chapman RL: The MacQuarrie test for mechanical ability. Psychometrika. 1948, 13 (3): 175-179. 10.1007/BF02289259.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. War-Department: Army Individual Test Battery. Manual of directions and scoring. 1944, Washington, D.C.: War Department, Adjutant General's Office

    Google Scholar 

  41. Perianez JA, Rios-Lago M, Rodriguez-Sanchez JM, Adrover-Roig D, Sanchez-Cubillo I, Crespo-Facorro B, Quemada JI, Barcelo F: Trail Making Test in traumatic brain injury, schizophrenia, and normal ageing: sample comparisons and normative data. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2007, 22 (4): 433-447. 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.022.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Kessler J, Denzler P, Markowitsch HJ: Demenz-Test (DT). 1999, Göttingen: Hogrefe

    Google Scholar 

  43. Aschenbrenner S, Tucha O, Lange KW: Der Regensburger Wortflüssigkeits-Test (RWT). 2000, Göttingen: Hogrefe

    Google Scholar 

  44. Tewes U: Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest fuer Erwachsene (HAWIE-R). 1991, Bern: Huber

    Google Scholar 

  45. Helmstaedter C, Lendt M, Lux S: Verbaler Lern- und Merkfähigkeitstest (VLMT). 2001, Goettingen: Beltz

    Google Scholar 

  46. Zimmermann P, Fimm B: Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung (TAP). Version 1.02c. 1993, Herzogenrath: PSYTEST

    Google Scholar 

  47. Chen EY, Shapleske J, Luque R, McKenna PJ, Hodges JR, Calloway SP, Hymas NF, Dening TR, Berrios GE: The Cambridge Neurological Inventory: a clinical instrument for assessment of soft neurological signs in psychiatric patients. Psychiatry Res. 1995, 56 (2): 183-204. 10.1016/0165-1781(95)02535-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Bartels C, Mertens N, Hofer S, Merboldt KD, Dietrich J, Frahm J, Ehrenreich H: Callosal dysfunction in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis correlates with diffusion tensor imaging of the central motor system. Neuromuscul Disord. 2008, 18 (5): 398-407. 10.1016/j.nmd.2008.02.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Barnes TR: The Barnes Akathisia Rating Scale - revisited. J Psychopharmacol. 2003, 17 (4): 365-370. 10.1177/0269881103174013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Simpson GM, Angus JW: A rating scale for extrapyramidal side effects. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1970, 212: 11-19. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1970.tb02066.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Simpson GM, Lee JH, Zoubok B, Gardos G: A rating scale for tardive dyskinesia. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1979, 64 (2): 171-179. 10.1007/BF00496058.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. Guy W: Abnormal involuntary movement scale (AIMS). ECDEU Assessment manual for psychopharmacology, revised National Institute of Mental Health. Rockville, MD. 1976

    Google Scholar 

  53. Krampe H, Bartels C, Victorson D, Enders CK, Beaumont J, Cella D, Ehrenreich H: The influence of personality factors on disease progression and health-related quality of life in people with ALS. Amyotroph Lateral Scler. 2008, 9 (2): 99-107. 10.1080/17482960701875805.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. SPSS Inc. [http://www.spss.com/]

  55. Blom G: Statistical Estimates and Transformed Beta Variables. 1958, New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc

    Google Scholar 

  56. Davis JM: Comparative doses and costs of antipsychotic medication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1976, 33 (7): 858-861.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Barnes TR: A rating scale for drug-induced akathisia. Br J Psychiatry. 1989, 154: 672-676. 10.1192/bjp.154.5.672.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Loughland CM, Carr VJ, Lewin TJ, Barnard RE, Chapman JL, Walton JM: Potential sampling and recruitment source impacts in schizophrenia research. Psychiatry Res. 2004, 125 (2): 117-127. 10.1016/j.psychres.2003.11.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Aleman A, Kahn RS, Selten J-P: Sex differences in the risk of schizophrenia: evidence from meta-analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003, 60 (6): 565-571. 10.1001/archpsyc.60.6.565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. McGrath J, Saha S, Chant D, Welham J: Schizophrenia: a concise overview of incidence, prevalence, and mortality. Epidemiol Rev. 2008, 30: 67-76. 10.1093/epirev/mxn001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Riecher-Rossler A, Hafner H: Gender aspects in schizophrenia: bridging the border between social and biological psychiatry. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 2000, 58-62. 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2000.00011.x. 407

  62. Henry J-M, Boyer L, Belzeaux R, Baumstarck-Barrau K, Samuelian J-C: Mental Disorders Among Homeless People Admitted to a French Psychiatric Emergency Service. Psychiatr Serv. 2010, 61 (3): 264-271. 10.1176/appi.ps.61.3.264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Toro PA, Bellavia CW, Daeschler CV, Owens BJ, Wall DD, Passero JM, Thomas DM: Distinguishing homelessness from poverty: A comparative study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1995, 63 (2): 280-289. 10.1037/0022-006X.63.2.280.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Israel N, Toro P, Ouellette N: Changes in the Composition of the Homeless Population: 1992-2002. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2010, 46 (1-2): 49-59. 10.1007/s10464-010-9326-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Kraepelin E: Psychiatrie. Ein Lehrbuch für Studierende und Ärzte. Sechste, vollständig umgearbeitete Auflage. 1899, Leipzig: Johann Ambrosius Barth

    Google Scholar 

  66. Castle DJ, Sham PC, Wessely S, Murray RM: The subtyping of schizophrenia in men and women: a latent class analysis. Psychol Med. 1994, 24 (1): 41-51. 10.1017/S0033291700026817.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Leung A, Chue P: Sex differences in schizophrenia, a review of the literature. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 2000, 401: 3-38. 10.1111/j.0065-1591.2000.0ap25.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Larsen TK, McGlashan TH, Johannessen JO, Vibe-Hansen L: First-episode schizophrenia: II. Premorbid patterns by gender. Schizophr Bull. 1996, 22 (2): 257-269.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Seeman MV, Lang M: The role of estrogens in schizophrenia gender differences. Schizophr Bull. 1990, 16 (2): 185-194.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Häfner H: Onset and early course as determinants of the further course of schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 2000, 44-48. 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2000.00008.x. 407

  71. Moriarty PJ, Lieber D, Bennett A, White L, Parrella M, Harvey PD, Davis KL: Gender differences in poor outcome patients with lifelong schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2001, 27 (1): 103-113.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Thorup A, Petersen L, Jeppesen P, Ohlenschlaeger J, Christensen T, Krarup G, Jorgensen P, Nordentoft M: Gender differences in young adults with first-episode schizophrenia spectrum disorders at baseline in the Danish OPUS study. Nerv Ment Dis. 2007, 195 (5): 396-405.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Weiser M, Reichenberg A, Rabinowitz J, Kaplan Z, Mark M, Nahon D, Davidson M: Gender differences in premorbid cognitive performance in a national cohort of schizophrenic patients. Schizophr Res. 2000, 45 (3): 185-190. 10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00190-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Green RE, Melo B, Christensen B, Ngo LA, Monette G, Bradbury C: Measuring premorbid IQ in traumatic brain injury: an examination of the validity of the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR). J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2008, 30 (2): 163-172. 10.1080/13803390701300524.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Aylward E, Walker E, Bettes B: Intelligence in schizophrenia: meta-analysis of the research. Schizophr Bull. 1984, 10 (3): 430-459.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Kern RS, Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Baade LE, Fenton WS, Gold JM, Keefe RS, Mesholam-Gately R, Mintz J, Seidman LJ, Stover E, Marder SR: The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 2: co-norming and standardization. Am J Psychiatry. 2008, 165 (2): 214-220. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07010043.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Hoff AL, Wieneke M, Faustman WO, Horon R, Sakuma M, Blankfeld H, Espinoza S, DeLisi LE: Sex differences in neurops ychological functioning of first-episode and chronically ill schizophrenic patients. Am J Psychiatry. 1998, 155 (10): 1437-1439.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Seeman MV: Gender differences in the prescribing of antipsychotic drugs. Am J Psychiatry. 2004, 161 (8): 1324-1333. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.8.1324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Seeman MV: Secondary effects of antipsychotics: women at greater risk than men. Schizophr Bull. 2009, 35 (5): 937-948. 10.1093/schbul/sbn023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Dominguez Mde G, Viechtbauer W, Simons CJ, van Os J, Krabbendam L: Are psychotic psychopathology and neurocognition orthogonal? A systematic review of their associations. Psychol Bull. 2009, 135 (1): 157-171. 10.1037/a0014415.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Puig O, Penades R, Gasto C, Catalan R, Torres A, Salamero M: Verbal memory, negative symptomatology and prediction psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2008, 158 (1): 11-17. 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.04.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Schennach-Wolff R, Jager M, Seemuller F, Obermeier M, Messer T, Laux G, Pfeiffer H, Naber D, Schmidt LG, Gaebel W, Huff W, Heuser I, Maier W, Lemke MR, Ruther E, Buchkremer G, Gastpar M, Moller HJ, Riedel M: Defining and predicting functional outcome in schizophrenia and schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr Res. 2009, 113 (2-3): 210-217. 10.1016/j.schres.2009.05.032.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Gaite L, Vazquez-Barquero JL, Herran A, Thornicroft G, Becker T, Sierra-Biddle D, Ruggeri M, Schene A, Knapp M, Vazquez-Bourgon J: Main determinants of Global Assessment of Functioning score in schizophrenia: a European multicenter study. Compr Psychiatry. 2005, 46 (6): 440-446. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2005.03.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Ehrenreich H, Siren AL: Neuroprotection--what does it mean?--What means do w e have?. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2001, 251 (4): 149-151. 10.1007/s004060170034.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the Max Planck Society and the DFG-Research Center for Molecular Physiology of the Brain (CMBP). We are indebted to all patients for their participation in the GRAS (Göttingen Research Association for Schizophrenia) study and to all colleagues in the collaborating centers who contributed to the GRAS data collection.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hannelore Ehrenreich.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

MB coordinated and supervised the traveling team of investigators and had a considerable impact on design and establishment of the data collection. KR and HFr were part of the traveling team of investigators, conducted statistical analyses of the clinical data, assisted in manuscript writing, and supervised data entry, substantially performed by SG, SP, AK, MFG, VA, ATa, ATr, and MF. Of the collaborating centers, LA, JBA, MBE, TB, AC, MD, HFo, RF, RG, SH, DH, GK, HK, MFr, FL, WM, AM, RMI, CO, FGP, TP, US, HJS and UHR enabled the work of the traveling team of examiners, by pre-selecting and preparing patients and organizing respective facilities and working conditions. HE, KAN, NB, PF, WS, and JF developed the concept of GRAS (Göttingen Research Association for Schizophrenia, founded in 2004), and guided the project, data analysis, and paper writing, hereby supported by BKH. All authors discussed the results, commented on the paper draft and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Katja Ribbe, Heidi Friedrichs, Martin Begemann contributed equally to this work.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ribbe, K., Friedrichs, H., Begemann, M. et al. The cross-sectional GRAS sample: A comprehensive phenotypical data collection of schizophrenic patients . BMC Psychiatry 10, 91 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-10-91

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-10-91

Keywords