Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Regional gray matter volumetric changes in autism associated with social and repetitive behavior symptoms

Abstract

Background

Although differences in brain anatomy in autism have been difficult to replicate using manual tracing methods, automated whole brain analyses have begun to find consistent differences in regions of the brain associated with the social cognitive processes that are often impaired in autism. We attempted to replicate these whole brain studies and to correlate regional volume changes with several autism symptom measures.

Methods

We performed MRI scans on 24 individuals diagnosed with DSM-IV autistic disorder and compared those to scans from 23 healthy comparison subjects matched on age. All participants were male. Whole brain, voxel-wise analyses of regional gray matter volume were conducted using voxel-based morphometry (VBM).

Results

Controlling for age and total gray matter volume, the volumes of the medial frontal gyri, left pre-central gyrus, right post-central gyrus, right fusiform gyrus, caudate nuclei and the left hippocampus were larger in the autism group relative to controls. Regions exhibiting smaller volumes in the autism group were observed exclusively in the cerebellum. Significant partial correlations were found between the volumes of the caudate nuclei, multiple frontal and temporal regions, the cerebellum and a measure of repetitive behaviors, controlling for total gray matter volume. Social and communication deficits in autism were also associated with caudate, cerebellar, and precuneus volumes, as well as with frontal and temporal lobe regional volumes.

Conclusion

Gray matter enlargement was observed in areas that have been functionally identified as important in social-cognitive processes, such as the medial frontal gyri, sensorimotor cortex and middle temporal gyrus. Additionally, we have shown that VBM is sensitive to associations between social and repetitive behaviors and regional brain volumes in autism.

Peer Review reports

Background

Studies of volumetric quantification of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data in autism have largely been non-replicative. To illustrate with a single example of considerable theoretical importance to autism, interest in hippocampal volume has been high since the neuropathology studies of Bauman and Kemper [1] reported evidence of increased cell packing density and reduced cell size in the hippocampus. However, as pointed out in two recent articles [2, 3], there is little agreement between studies measuring hippocampal volume in individuals with autism. To date, there are 4 studies reporting no differences [4–7], 3 reports of decreased volume in autism relative to controls [8, 9], and 3 reports of increased hippocampal volume in autism [2, 3, 10]. The differences among the studies are considerable, including different sample characteristics, MRI scanners, methods for assessing hippocampal volume and statistical approaches for analyzing data (reviewed in part in Rojas et al. [2]; see also [11]), and no single, clear candidate emerges to account for the varying findings for the hippocampus.

There are a variety of potentially confounding factors in autism research that challenge the scientific community that are not MRI specific, such as age, gender, IQ, and the inherent heterogeneity of the disorder separate from these other factors (reviewed in Palmen and van Engeland [12]). For example, Palmen and van Engeland [12] accept the argument made by Piven and Arndt [13] that IQ must be strictly matched or statistically factored out in autism studies, and their conclusions on cerebellar studies in autism are based on dismissal of the various studies that do not adhere to this recommendation (e.g., [14–17]). In contrast, Yeung-Courchesne and Courchesne [18] argued strongly that in attempting to "control" for a domain non-specific construct such as IQ, variability truly associated with autism could be discarded as "non-specific". Indeed, few MRI studies critically examine the effects of using IQ matched, but etiologically heterogeneous samples of developmentally disordered controls. The alternate use of statistical covariates for IQ is also usually performed without consideration for the assumptions underlying such analyses (e.g., correlation between the covariate and dependent measure in the sample being measured).

In commenting on differences in major findings between MRI studies, methodological differences are often appealing candidates. Differences in MRI scanner hardware and acquisition hardware can lead to differences across sites (e.g., see Patwardhan et al. [19]) that are difficult to assess. Compounding this problem, many, if not most of the MRI-based morphometric literature in autism have employed manual tracing methods. Manual tracing methods suffer from several problems that negatively impact their reliability and validity. First, they rely on sulcal/gyral boundary determinations that are relatively arbitrarily determined and are highly variable from individual brain to brain. Second, those sulcal/gyral boundaries are often not well correlated with histoanatomically defined areas [e.g., [20–22]]. Third, they are susceptible to rater judgments, which necessarily introduce inter-rater, and therefore intra- and inter-laboratory error variance into the comparisons. Individual laboratories develop highly reliable protocols for such boundary determination, but these protocols are not standardized and are challenging to adapt between sites.

Despite the difficulties inherent with manually traced structural MRI studies, a small number of findings appear to replicate across laboratories. The most consistent finding has been that brain volume appears to be larger in autism [23–26], although there is emerging consensus that this finding is restricted to young children with autism [27, 28]. Differences in cerebellar volume are reported in many studies [15–17, 29–34], but the directionality of the finding in autism (larger or smaller relative to comparison group) and region within the cerebellum (vermis or hemisphere) differ greatly between studies. Most studies of the cerebellum, however, report reduced vermal volume in autism, unless IQ is matched or covaried in the analysis (e.g., see [12] for a review). Some studies have reported enlarged caudate nuclei [35–37], although this finding does not always survive correction for overall brain volume [e.g., [35]] and another study failed to observe enlargement with or without correction for total brain volume [38]. Enlargement of the caudate nucleus has been correlated with repetitive/stereotyped behaviors in two studies [35, 36].

Several automated approaches to MRI morphometry have been developed over the past decade, including template-based, region of interest (ROI) labeling procedures (e.g., see [39–42]), cortical thickness approaches [43, 44] and voxel based morphometry (VBM [45–47]). Some of these approaches have been applied to studies of autism spectrum disorders [44, 48–55], particularly using VBM methods. In the first VBM study of 15 people with autism, Abell et al. [48] identified abnormalities in the frontal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, fusiform gyrus, occipital-temporal junction, and the cerebellum [48]. Waiter et al. [54] obtained significant findings in similar regions, although the directionality of the findings (increase or decrease) for some structures (e.g., left inferior frontal gyrus) was opposite to Abell et al. [48]. McAlonen et al. [51] identified reductions in fronto-striatal regions. Two additional VBM studies have limited their analyses to regions associated with social cognition. Boddaert et al. [49] reported bilateral gray matter (GM) decreases in the superior temporal sulcus, a region whose activity has been reported to be linked with biological motion perception and theory of mind [56]. Salmond et al. [55] identified abnormalities in the amygdala and hippocampal region, the orbital frontal cortex, the superior temporal gyri and the cerebellum. Using a related technique, but focusing on GM thickness rather than volume, Hadjikhani et al. [57] found GM thinning in autism in regions associated with the so-called mirror-neuron system, including pre- and post-central gyri, inferior frontal gyrus, medial frontal gyrus and middle temporal gyrus, all of which were significantly correlated with social and communication deficits in the autism participants. In summary, studies using automated volumetric and/or thickness measurements suggest that autism may be associated with GM differences in distributed cortical and subcortical regions that are important in social cognitive and/or motor processes.

VBM studies in autism have not, however, capitalized on the ability of the method to do whole-brain correlations between GM and symptom measures. In the current study, we applied VBM methods to a well defined sample of patients with autistic disorder with the goal of replicating prior whole brain findings in the literature reported for both gray and white matter. In addition, we examined correlations between GM volume, IQ and autism symptom measures derived from diagnostic interviews. We hypothesized that caudate nucleus volume would correlate with repetitive behaviors in autism, replicating previous hand tracing results. Finally, we hypothesized that social communication deficits would correlate with volumes of structures known to participate in social cognitive processes such as the amygdala, medial frontal gyri and superior temporal sulcus.

Methods

Participants and behavioral measures

Twenty-four males with autism between the ages of 7 and 47 years (mean: 22.60 ± 11.61 years) were recruited to participate in the study. Individuals with autism were included in that group if they met clinical criteria for DSM-IV autistic disorder [58], as well as criteria for autism on both the Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADI: Lord et al. [59]) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS: Lord et al. [60]). The autism participants were tested, either by karyotype or DNA testing, for fragile X syndrome, and were all negative. For comparison, 23 male participants with no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders were recruited from the Denver metropolitan region. Comparison subjects were first screened using the Structured Clinical Interview, DSM-IV (SCID) Screen Patient Questionnaire – Extended [61], and subjects whose responses needed further clarification were administered that portion of the full SCID interview [62]still in question. Participants in the comparison group had no personal history of neurological or Axis I psychiatric illness and met Research Diagnostic Criteria for never mentally-ill [63]. In addition, comparison subjects had no reported family history of neurological or psychiatric illness among first-degree relatives.

All participants were administered either a full Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd edition (WAIS-III; [64]) or Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edition (WISC-III; [65]). Demographic and cognitive data on participants are shown in Table 1. All subjects signed informed consent to participate in the experiment consistent with the guidelines of the Colorado Multiple Institution Review Board.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics and whole brain volume measures for the study groups (mean ± SD).

Image acquisition

Spoiled gradient recalled (SPGR) T1-weighted (TR = 40 ms, TE = 5 ms, 40 degree flip angle, NEX = 1) images were acquired on a 1.5 T General Electric Signa system (G.E. Medical Systems, Inc., Milwaukee, WI). One-hundred and twenty-four contiguous, 1.7-mm thick coronal slices with a 192 × 256 (reconstructed to 2562) matrix in a 240 mm field of view resulted in voxel dimensions of .9375 by .9375 by 1.7 mm. Prior to the coronal scan sequence, the coronal slice axis was aligned perpendicular to a straight line connecting the anterior tip of the genu of the corpus callosum with the posterior tip of the splenium of the corpus callosum in a sagittal scout image series. Participants were not sedated for the scans.

Image processing

Image processing and statistical analyses were conducted using the SPM2 software package [66]. To account for potential differences between our local sample and the templates provided in SPM2, an "optimized VBM" protocol was used, as described in Good et al. [67], which results in the use of a study-specific anatomical template for spatial normalization and tissue segmentation, as described below.

To create the customized template, all scans were first segmented into gray, white and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) compartments in their native space using the standard SPM2 T1 template (MNI305 [68]). The spatial normalization parameters for individual subjects were then estimated based on the individual GM compartments (7 × 9 × 7 discrete cosine basis functions, 16 non-linear iterations, .01 regularization) and then applied to the original native T1 data. These normalized images were segmented again using the MNI305 template and spatial priors, and averages of the 51 T1 images, as well as corresponding GM, white matter and CSF averages from the segmentation output. These four averages were then smoothed with an isotropic 8 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel to produce a new study specific template, as well as spatial priors for gray, white and CSF compartments.

The original, native T1 images were then segmented using the new template and priors, and the spatial normalization parameters were estimated using the GM compartment and applied to the native images to produce new spatially normalized images. These were segmented again in normalized space and multiplied (i.e., modulated) by the Jacobian determinants from the normalization step to preserve the volume information in each voxel [67]. The modulated GM images were then smoothed with a 12 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel for statistical analyses.

Statistical analyses

To examine differences in regional GM volume between groups, an ANCOVA model was employed with two covariates, total GM volume and age. Total GM volume was employed so that inferences could be made about regional differences in volume. Age was employed as a covariate in consideration of both the wide age range of the sample, the possible age-dependency of structural findings in autism [e.g., [27]], and preliminary examination of age correlations with the hypothesized regions of interest (see below). Based on the results of the age correlation analysis (see below), the linear effect of age was entered as a covariate to account for the confounding effects of age on regional GM volume.

Based on previous findings in autism, we decided on a list of structures to use as a priori regions of interest (ROI) for our analyses to increase the sensitivity of our group comparisons. For each ROI, if a cluster of 25 or more contiguous voxels exceeded an uncorrected p < .01 threshold at the local maximum voxel, we applied a small volume correction (SVC: Worseley et al. [69]) at p < .05 on a 6 mm diameter sphere centered on the cluster maximum. Non-hypothesized group differences (i.e., outside of regions of interest) were only considered if they reached significance after correcting for proportion of false positive results using a false discovery rate (FDR) threshold of p < .05. Whether a cluster was considered part of an ROI or not was determined from whether the local maximum voxel was contained within the label of the same name in the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) atlas [70]. Our hypothesized bilateral ROIs, based on regions associated with motor behavior and social cognition, were: 1) medial frontal gyrus, 2) inferior frontal gyrus, 3) middle and superior temporal gyrus (thus containing superior temporal sulcus), 4) fusiform gyrus, 5) cerebellum, 6) posterior cingulate gyrus (i.e., precuneus/retrosplenial cortex), 7) pre-central gyrus, 8) post-central gyrus, 9) amygdala, 10) caudate nucleus and 11) hippocampus. The hippocampus was included because there is significant overlap (N = 15 with autism) between this sample and that reported by Rojas et al. [2], who reported significantly larger hippocampi in adults with autism.

For partial correlations with symptom domains, a multiple regression analysis with the ADI-R, Social and Communication Total score, ADI-R Repetitive and Stereotyped Patterns score and a measure of severity for the ADOS, along with total gray matter volume, was computed in SPM2. The ADOS severity measure [71] was calculated by summing the Social and Communication scores and dividing by the number of items from which the total is derived (i.e., to account for differences in the ADOS modules, which differ by developmental level). Symptom correlation analyses were restricted to regions contained within a mask including the structures listed for the group comparison above and were evaluated for significance using the small volume correction (p < .05). Age and FSIQ were also evaluated for significance using the ROI mask, but because there were no a priori hypotheses, we used a more conservative multiple comparison correction of FDR < .05.

Results

Sample characteristics

Although the autism group had significantly lower full scale (t(45) = 4.92, p < .05), verbal (t(45) = 4.48, p < .05) and performance (t(45) = 4.57, p < .05) IQ scores than the control group, no differences in age, education or handedness were observed between groups (all p > .10, see Table 1). Verbal, Performance, and Full scale IQ, however, were not significantly correlated with the volumes of any of the voxels within the structural ROIs in the study, even at an uncorrected p < .01, in partial correlation analyses controlling for total GM volume.

A preliminary voxel-wise partial correlation analysis of age (with linear, quadratic and cubic polynomial age expansions to account for linear and non-linear age effects) controlling for total GM volume, revealed that significant correlations existed with some of our structures of interest (Figure 1). There were significant linear effects, but no significant quadratic or cubic effects (Table 2).

Figure 1
figure 1

Correlations between regional gray matter volume and age. Positive (top row) and negative (bottom row) partial correlations, corrected for total gray matter volume, are shown in glass brain projection. SPM(t) map display threshold is FDR < .05. L = left hemisphere. The data in this figure correspond to the linear effects reported in Table 2.

Figure 2
figure 2

Gray matter volume differences between groups, overlaid onto average T1 image from study. Slice numbers (relative to AC-PC origin plane) are shown in upper left corner for each slice. Relative increases (autism > control) are shown in red-orange color scale corresponding to upper right color bar and relative decreases (control > autism) are shown in blue-violet color scale corresponding to lower right color bar. The display threshold is p < .01, uncorrected, but only clusters surviving small volume correction, p < .05, are illustrated (see Table 1 for coordinates and label descriptions).

Table 2 Significant partial correlations between regional GM and age, controlling for total gray matter volume

Total gray matter, white matter, and brain/intracranial ratios

No differences were observed between groups for total GM, total white matter or brain (gray + white) to intracranial (gray + white + csf) volume ratio, regardless of whether age was used as a covariate or not (Table 1).

Regional gray matter results

Table 3 lists structures for which significant results were obtained after small volume correction in the ROIs for the study. Regional increases in GM volume in the autism group relative to control subjects, controlling for age and total GM, were observed in the left and right medial frontal gyri, left middle temporal and pre-central gyri, right fusiform gyrus, right post-central gyrus, left hippocampus, left and right caudate nuclei. Regional decreases in GM volume in the autism group were observed only in the cerebellum (Figure 2). No regions were identified outside of the a priori ROIs after correction for multiple comparisons at FDR < .05.

Table 3 Significant differences in GM volume between groups

Demographic and symptom correlations

No significant correlations were observed between full scale IQ and regional GM volumes within the regions of interest in the study, even at p < .001 uncorrected for multiple comparisons. However, there were several regions that correlated significantly with age, although only the linear and quadratic components were significant (see Figure 1 and Table 2).

Table 4 lists regional partial correlations, controlling for total GM volume and correlations with other behavioral measures, for each of the symptom measures used in the study, after small volume correction. Regions with significant positive partial correlations with the ADI-R Repetitive and Stereotyped Behavior Domain, after small volume correction, include the left inferior frontal gyrus, caudate nuclei and right amygdala. Regions exhibiting negative correlations (i.e., smaller volumes associated with worse scores) included the superior temporal gyri, left post-central gyrus and cerebellar regions. For the ADI-R Social and Communication total score, positive correlations were present with the volumes of and caudate nucleus bilaterally, the left post-central gyrus, superior temporal gyri and cerebellar regions. Significant negative correlations were present with the left precuneus, left medial frontal and inferior frontal gyri, and left pre-central gyrus. The ADOS-G Severity measure exhibited significant negative correlations with the volumes of the left cerebellum (lobule IX) and right superior temporal gyrus. Figures 3 and 4 illustrate the ADI-R Repetitive and Stereotyped Behavior and Social/Communication Total symptom correlations with regional GM volume, respectively.

Table 4 Significant partial correlations between regional GM and symptom severity measures in the autism group.
Figure 3
figure 3

Correlations between regional gray matter volume and ADI-R Repetitive and Stereotyped Behavior Domain in the autism participants. Positive (top row) and negative (bottom row) partial correlations, corrected for total gray matter volume, are shown in glass brain projection. SPM(t) map display threshold is p < .01, uncorrected (Table 4 lists structures surviving small volume correction). L = left hemisphere.

Figure 4
figure 4

Correlations between regional gray matter volume and ADI-R Social and Communication Domain total scores in the autism participants. Positive (top row) and negative (bottom row) partial correlations, corrected for total gray matter volume, are shown in glass brain projection. SPM(t) map display threshold is p < .01, uncorrected (Table 4 lists structures surviving small volume correction). L = left hemisphere.

Discussion and conclusion

Most, if not all, of the regions in which we report differences between the autism and control groups have been previously associated with social cognitive processes in functional imaging and lesion studies. For example, functional MRI studies of Theory of Mind (TOM: i.e., the ability to make mental state inferences about others) have consistently reported activation of the medial aspect of the superior frontal gyrus, anterior to the region commonly referred to as the pre-supplementary motor cortex, despite using a wide variety of TOM tasks [72–76]. The precuneus region of the medial parietal lobe is also observed to be active in many TOM experiments, may be particularly important in self awareness and is heavily interconnected to the medial prefrontal regions and the dorsal striatum [77]. The posterior middle temporal gyrus/sulcus region has a role in biologically relevant motion perception, intentionality of eye gaze direction and inferring intentionality from stories, and is often active in functional imaging of TOM tasks [72, 75, 78–80]. Many of these same regions have been reported to be activated differently in people with autism and their first degree relatives while performing TOM tasks, relative to the activation seen in matched controls [72, 81].

We found no significant correlations between full scale IQ and regional GM volume in our sample. Although this was a study of GM volume, rather than thickness, our findings our consistent with Hadjikhani et al. [57], who used the latter methodology in a recent study on autism. Taken together, these findings suggest that caution is warranted in using IQ as a covariate in anatomical studies of autism (e.g., see [18]). However, age was highly correlated with the volume of the hippocampus and surrounding region in our sample, which is consistent with the finding of Good et al. [67], who first described the optimized VBM approach in the literature and found positive correlations between age and medial temporal lobe volume in large sample of adults when controlling for total brain volume. In our sample, this might be driven by the lower end of the age range. However, only the linear component for age was significant. A study including a larger number of older adults might be able to demonstrate an asymptote on aging effects on the hippocampal region (i.e., a significant quadratic term).

The increased left hippocampal volume in the autism group, relative to the control group, is consistent with the findings reported in Rojas et al. [2]. This is expected given that almost 60 percent of the autism subjects from this study also participated in that paper, which used hand-tracing methods to assess the hippocampus. As discussed in the introduction, however, both increases and decreases in hippocampal volume have been reported in autism by hand tracing methods. To our knowledge, this is the first report of hippocampal volume change, increase or decrease, in autism using VBM methodology. A recent paper by Testa et al. [82] comparing hand-tracing to VBM for detecting hippocampal atrophy in Alzheimer's Disease provided data supporting the comparability of the two methods for hippocampal volumetry.

To our knowledge, this is the first VBM study in autism to utilize symptom measures from the ADI-R and ADOS-G to examine correlations with both cortical and subcortical GM. Hadjikhani et al. [57] performed such correlations, but only with regional cortical thickness, not with subcortical structural measures. Our significant positive correlation between caudate nucleus volume and the ADI-R Repetitive and Stereotyped Behavior domain is supportive of the same association reported in the manual tracing study of Hollander et al. [36]. Caudate enlargement has also been reported in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder [OCD: 83]. Interestingly, caudate nucleus activity in OCD has been reported to be higher in both resting and symptom provocation conditions in functional neuroimaging studies [84, 85], and treatment with either serotonin agonist medications or behavior therapy have been shown to reduce this over-activity [86, 87].

An alternative, but not necessarily mutually exclusive, interpretation of the caudate nucleus correlation is that it simply reflects disorder severity generally. In support of this notion is that the caudate nucleus volume was significantly associated with both with the ADI-R Social and Communication total score and with the Repetitive and Stereotyped Behaviors score. Arguing against this interpretation is the lack of correlation observed in these same structures with the ADOS-G Severity measure. These other structures, along with the superior temporal gyrus and cerebellum, have been heavily implicated in social cognitive and motor abilities. Taken together, the GM findings and the symptom correlations suggest that the regional GM differences are highly relevant to the behavioral phenotype in autism.

Some caution is warranted with respect to the caudate nucleus findings, however. Three of our autism participants had a past history of neuroleptic medication treatment, use of which has been previously associated with caudate volume increases in schizophrenia [e.g., [88, 89]]. However, all of three of these were taking atypical antipsychotic agents and discontinued use of the drugs at least 2 years prior to participation in the study. Studies have suggested that caudate volume increases to neuroleptic treatment are associated more strongly with typical neuroleptics and that volumes decrease rapidly after neuroleptic treatment is withdrawn [90, 91]. It therefore seems unlikely that the caudate volume enlargement in our sample could be explained by treatment by neuroleptic medications.

There are several limitations to the results of this study to consider. Among them, the non-isotropic voxel dimensions of the MRI scans may have introduced partial volume effects that influenced the tissue segmentation step of the VBM procedure. Additionally, the large age range of participants could have introduced variability that reduced the effect sizes for some structures we hypothesized would differ between groups (e.g., the amygdala), but for which we did not find group differences. We attempted to control for age effects statistically, by treating the effects of age as covariates in the main analysis. This may not be the most optimal design, however, and further studies that more tightly control the age range of the samples are warranted. Third, segmentation algorithms such as the one employed here on T1 weighted MRI data generally do not discriminate well between deep cerebellar nuclei and the surrounding cerebellar white matter (most of the nuclei are classified as white matter). Fourth, the lack of inclusion of female participants, while advantageous in terms of sample homogeneity, limits the generalizability to males with autism. Finally, the IQ of the control participants (mean = 119), while not strictly a limitation, should be mentioned, because it is worth noting that previous population-based studies have indicated a higher mean IQ level in the Denver metropolitan region than in the national standardization sample for the Wechsler scales [92].

References

  1. Bauman M, Kemper TL: Histoanatomic observations of the brain in early infantile autism. Neurology. 1985, 35 (6): 866-874.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Rojas DC, Smith JA, Benkers TL, Camou SL, Reite ML, Rogers SJ: Hippocampus and amygdala volumes in parents of children with autistic disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2004, 161 (11): 2038-2044. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.11.2038.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Schumann CM, Hamstra J, Goodlin-Jones BL, Lotspeich LJ, Kwon H, Buonocore MH, Lammers CR, Reiss AL, Amaral DG: The amygdala is enlarged in children but not adolescents with autism; the hippocampus is enlarged at all ages. J Neurosci. 2004, 24 (28): 6392-6401. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1297-04.2004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Haznedar MM, Buchsbaum MS, Wei TC, Hof PR, Cartwright C, Bienstock CA, Hollander E: Limbic circuitry in patients with autism spectrum disorders studied with positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000, 157 (12): 1994-2001. 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.12.1994.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Howard MA, Cowell PE, Boucher J, Broks P, Mayes A, Farrant A, Roberts N: Convergent neuroanatomical and behavioural evidence of an amygdala hypothesis of autism. NeuroReport. 2000, 11 (13): 2931-2935. 10.1097/00001756-200009110-00020.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Piven J, Bailey J, Ranson BJ, Arndt S: No difference in hippocampus volume detected on magnetic resonance imaging in autistic individuals [published erratum appears in J Autism Dev Disord 1998 Jun;28(3):271]. J Autism Dev Disord. 1998, 28 (2): 105-110. 10.1023/A:1026084430649.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Saitoh O, Courchesne E, Egaas B, Lincoln AJ, Schreibman L: Cross-sectional area of the posterior hippocampus in autistic patients with cerebellar and corpus callosum abnormalities. Neurology. 1995, 45 (2): 317-324.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Aylward EH, Minshew NJ, Goldstein G, Honeycutt NA, Augustine AM, Yates KO, Barta PE, Pearlson GD: MRI volumes of amygdala and hippocampus in non-mentally retarded autistic adolescents and adults. Neurology. 1999, 53 (9): 2145-2150.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Saitoh O, Karns CM, Courchesne E: Development of the hippocampal formation from 2 to 42 years: MRI evidence of smaller area dentata in autism. Brain. 2001, 124 (Pt 7): 1317-1324. 10.1093/brain/124.7.1317.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Sparks BF, Friedman SD, Shaw DW, Aylward EH, Echelard D, Artru AA, Maravilla KR, Giedd JN, Munson J, Dawson G, Dager SR: Brain structural abnormalities in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Neurology. 2002, 59 (2): 184-192.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Geuze E, Vermetten E, Bremner JD: MR-based in vivo hippocampal volumetrics: 1. Review of methodologies currently employed. Mol Psychiatry. 2005, 10 (2): 147-159. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001580.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Palmen SJ, van Engeland H: Review on structural neuroimaging findings in autism. J Neural Transm. 2004, 111 (7): 903-929. 10.1007/s00702-003-0068-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Piven J, Arndt S: The cerebellum and autism. Neurology. 1995, 45 (2): 398-402.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Courchesne E, Townsend J, Saitoh O: The brain in infantile autism: posterior fossa structures are abnormal [see comments]. Neurology. 1994, 44 (2): 214-223.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Hashimoto T, Tayama M, Miyazaki M, Murakawa K, Kuroda Y: Brainstem and cerebellar vermis involvement in autistic children. J Child Neurol. 1993, 8 (2): 149-153.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Gaffney GR, Tsai LY, Kuperman S, Minchin S: Cerebellar structure in autism. Am J Dis Child. 1987, 141 (12): 1330-1332.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Courchesne E, Yeung-Courchesne R, Press GA, Hesselink JR, Jernigan TL: Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules VI and VII in autism. N Engl J Med. 1988, 318 (21): 1349-1354.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Yeung-Courchesne R, Courchesne E: From impasse to insight in autism research: from behavioral symptoms to biological explanations. Dev Psychopathol. 1997, 9 (2): 389-419.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Patwardhan AJ, Eliez S, Warsofsky IS, Glover GH, White CD, Giedd JN, Peterson BS, Rojas DC, Reiss AL: Effects of image orientation on the comparability of pediatric brain volumes using three-dimensional MR data. J Comput Assist Tomogr. 2001, 25 (3): 452-457. 10.1097/00004728-200105000-00020.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Amunts K, Malikovic A, Mohlberg H, Schormann T, Zilles K: Brodmann's areas 17 and 18 brought into stereotaxic space-where and how variable?. Neuroimage. 2000, 11 (1): 66-84. 10.1006/nimg.1999.0516.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Morosan P, Rademacher J, Schleicher A, Amunts K, Schormann T, Zilles K: Human primary auditory cortex: cytoarchitectonic subdivisions and mapping into a spatial reference system. Neuroimage. 2001, 13 (4): 684-701. 10.1006/nimg.2000.0715.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Brett M, Johnsrude IS, Owen AM: The problem of functional localization in the human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002, 3 (3): 243-249. 10.1038/nrn756.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Piven J, Arndt S, Bailey J, Andreasen N: Regional brain enlargement in autism: a magnetic resonance imaging study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996, 35 (4): 530-536. 10.1097/00004583-199604000-00020.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Aylward EH, Minshew NJ, Field K, Sparks BF, Singh N: Effects of age on brain volume and head circumference in autism. Neurology. 2002, 59 (2): 175-183.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Courchesne E, Karns CM, Davis HR, Ziccardi R, Carper RA, Tigue ZD, Chisum HJ, Moses P, Pierce K, Lord C, Lincoln AJ, Pizzo S, Schreibman L, Haas RH, Akshoomoff NA, Courchesne RY: Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic disorder: an MRI study. Neurology. 2001, 57 (2): 245-254.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Palmen SJ, Hulshoff Pol HE, Kemner C, Schnack HG, Durston S, Lahuis BE, Kahn RS, Van Engeland H: Increased gray-matter volume in medication-naive high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder. Psychol Med. 2005, 35 (4): 561-570. 10.1017/S0033291704003496.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Redcay E, Courchesne E: When is the brain enlarged in autism? A meta-analysis of all brain size reports. Biol Psychiatry. 2005, 58 (1): 1-9. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Herbert MR: Large brains in autism: the challenge of pervasive abnormality. Neuroscientist. 2005, 11 (5): 417-440. 10.1177/0091270005278866.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Rumsey JM, Creasey H, Stepanek JS, Dorwart R, Patronas N, Hamburger SD, Duara R: Hemispheric asymmetries, fourth ventricular size, and cerebellar morphology in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1988, 18 (1): 127-137. 10.1007/BF02211823.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Murakami JW, Courchesne E, Press GA, Yeung-Courchesne R, Hesselink JR: Reduced cerebellar hemisphere size and its relationship to vermal hypoplasia in autism. Arch Neurol. 1989, 46 (6): 689-694.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Piven J, Nehme E, Simon J, Barta P, Pearlson G, Folstein SE: Magnetic resonance imaging in autism: measurement of the cerebellum, pons, and fourth ventricle. Biol Psychiatry. 1992, 31 (5): 491-504. 10.1016/0006-3223(92)90260-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Courchesne E, Saitoh O, Yeung-Courchesne R, Press GA, Lincoln AJ, Haas RH, Schreibman L: Abnormality of cerebellar vermian lobules VI and VII in patients with infantile autism: identification of hypoplastic and hyperplastic subgroups with MR imaging. AJR Am J Roentgenol. 1994, 162 (1): 123-130.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Courchesne E: Brainstem, cerebellar and limbic neuroanatomical abnormalities in autism. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1997, 7 (2): 269-278. 10.1016/S0959-4388(97)80016-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Levitt JG, Blanton R, Capetillo-Cunliffe L, Guthrie D, Toga A, McCracken JT: Cerebellar vermis lobules VIII-X in autism. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 1999, 23 (4): 625-633. 10.1016/S0278-5846(99)00021-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Sears LL, Vest C, Mohamed S, Bailey J, Ranson BJ, Piven J: An MRI study of the basal ganglia in autism. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 1999, 23 (4): 613-624. 10.1016/S0278-5846(99)00020-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Hollander E, Anagnostou E, Chaplin W, Esposito K, Haznedar MM, Licalzi E, Wasserman S, Soorya L, Buchsbaum M: Striatal volume on magnetic resonance imaging and repetitive behaviors in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2005, 58 (3): 226-232. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Herbert MR, Ziegler DA, Deutsch CK, O'Brien LM, Lange N, Bakardjiev A, Hodgson J, Adrien KT, Steele S, Makris N, Kennedy D, Harris GJ, Caviness VS: Dissociations of cerebral cortex, subcortical and cerebral white matter volumes in autistic boys. Brain. 2003, 126 (Pt 5): 1182-1192. 10.1093/brain/awg110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Hardan AY, Kilpatrick M, Keshavan MS, Minshew NJ: Motor performance and anatomic magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of the basal ganglia in autism. J Child Neurol. 2003, 18 (5): 317-324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, van der Kouwe A, Killiany R, Kennedy D, Klaveness S, Montillo A, Makris N, Rosen B, Dale AM: Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002, 33 (3): 341-355. 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Fischl B, van der Kouwe A, Destrieux C, Halgren E, Segonne F, Salat DH, Busa E, Seidman LJ, Goldstein J, Kennedy D, Caviness V, Makris N, Rosen B, Dale AM: Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2004, 14 (1): 11-22. 10.1093/cercor/bhg087.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Caviness VS, Kennedy DN, Richelme C, Rademacher J, Filipek PA: The human brain age 7-11 years: a volumetric analysis based on magnetic resonance images. Cereb Cortex. 1996, 6 (5): 726-736.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Kennedy DN, Lange N, Makris N, Bates J, Meyer J, Caviness VS: Gyri of the human neocortex: an MRI-based analysis of volume and variance. Cereb Cortex. 1998, 8 (4): 372-384. 10.1093/cercor/8.4.372.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Fischl B, Dale AM: Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000, 97 (20): 11050-11055. 10.1073/pnas.200033797.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Chung MK, Robbins SM, Dalton KM, Davidson RJ, Alexander AL, Evans AC: Cortical thickness analysis in autism with heat kernel smoothing. Neuroimage. 2005, 25 (4): 1256-1265. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.052.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Ashburner J, Friston KJ: Voxel-based morphometry--the methods. Neuroimage. 2000, 11 (6 Pt 1): 805-821. 10.1006/nimg.2000.0582.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Davatzikos C, Genc A, Xu D, Resnick SM: Voxel-based morphometry using the RAVENS maps: methods and validation using simulated longitudinal atrophy. Neuroimage. 2001, 14 (6): 1361-1369. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0937.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Mechelli A, Price CJ, Friston KJ, Ashburner J: Voxel-based morphometry of the human brain: Methods and applications. Current Medical Imaging Reviews. 2005, 1: 105-113. 10.2174/1573405054038726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Abell F, Krams M, Ashburner J, Passingham R, Friston K, Frackowiak R, Happe F, Frith C, Frith U: The neuroanatomy of autism: a voxel-based whole brain analysis of structural scans. Neuroreport. 1999, 10 (8): 1647-1651. 10.1097/00001756-199906030-00005.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Boddaert N, Chabane N, Gervais H, Good CD, Bourgeois M, Plumet MH, Barthelemy C, Mouren MC, Artiges E, Samson Y, Brunelle F, Frackowiak RS, Zilbovicius M: Superior temporal sulcus anatomical abnormalities in childhood autism: a voxel-based morphometry MRI study. Neuroimage. 2004, 23 (1): 364-369. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.016.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Waiter GD, Williams JH, Murray AD, Gilchrist A, Perrett DI, Whiten A: Structural white matter deficits in high-functioning individuals with autistic spectrum disorder: a voxel-based investigation. Neuroimage. 2005, 24 (2): 455-461. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.049.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. McAlonan GM, Cheung V, Cheung C, Suckling J, Lam GY, Tai KS, Yip L, Murphy DG, Chua SE: Mapping the brain in autism. A voxel-based MRI study of volumetric differences and intercorrelations in autism. Brain. 2005, 128 (Pt 2): 268-276.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Kwon H, Ow AW, Pedatella KE, Lotspeich LJ, Reiss AL: Voxel-based morphometry elucidates structural neuroanatomy of high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2004, 46 (11): 760-764. 10.1017/S0012162204001306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Chung MK, Dalton KM, Alexander AL, Davidson RJ: Less white matter concentration in autism: 2D voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage. 2004, 23 (1): 242-251. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Waiter GD, Williams JH, Murray AD, Gilchrist A, Perrett DI, Whiten A: A voxel-based investigation of brain structure in male adolescents with autistic spectrum disorder. Neuroimage. 2004, 22 (2): 619-625. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.02.029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Salmond CH, de Haan M, Friston KJ, Gadian DG, Vargha-Khadem F: Investigating individual differences in brain abnormalities in autism. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003, 358 (1430): 405-413. 10.1098/rstb.2002.1210.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Blakemore SJ, Decety J: From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001, 2 (8): 561-567. 10.1038/35085047.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Hadjikhani N, Joseph RM, Snyder J, Tager-Flusberg H: Anatomical Differences in the Mirror Neuron System and Social Cognition Network in Autism. Cereb Cortex. 2005

    Google Scholar 

  58. APA: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 1994, Washington, D. C. , American Psychiatric Association, 4th

    Google Scholar 

  59. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A: Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994, 24 (5): 659-685. 10.1007/BF02172145.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A, Rutter M: The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000, 30 (3): 205-223. 10.1023/A:1005592401947.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. First MB, Gibbon M, Williams JBW, Spitzer RL: SCID Screen Patient Questionnaire - Extended. 1999, North Tonawanda, NY , Multi Health Systems, 2nd

    Google Scholar 

  62. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for Axis I DSM-IV Disorders: Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). 1994, New York , Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute

    Google Scholar 

  63. Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Robins E: Research diagnostic criteria: rationale and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1978, 35 (6): 773-782.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III manual. 1997, San Antonio, TX , Psychological Corporation

    Google Scholar 

  65. Wechsler D: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III manual. 1991, San Antonio, TX , Psychological Corporation

    Google Scholar 

  66. Penny WD, Ashburner J, Kiebel S, Henson R, Glaser DE, Phillips C, Friston K: Statistical parametric mapping: An annotated bibliography. [http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/doc/spmbib.html]

  67. Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS: A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage. 2001, 14 (1 Pt 1): 21-36. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Evans AC, Collins DL, Mills SR, Brown ED, Kelly RL, Peters TM: 3D statistical neuroanatomical models from 305 MRI volumes. Edited by: Klaisner L. 1993, IEEE, 1813-1817.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Worsley KJ, Marrett S, Neelin P, Vandal AC, Friston KJ, Evans AC: A unified statistical approach for determining significant voxels in images of cerebral activation. Human Brain Mapping. 1996, 4: 58-73. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:1<58::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-O.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, Mazoyer B, Joliot M: Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002, 15 (1): 273-289. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Bailey A, Parr JR, Baird G, Le Couteur A, Rutter M, IMGSAC: Phenotypic characteristics of autistic regression in an international multiplex sample: Boston, MA. 2005,

    Google Scholar 

  72. Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Wheelwright S, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Simmons A, Williams SC: Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci. 1999, 11 (6): 1891-1898. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00621.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Heberlein AS, Saxe RR: Dissociation between emotion and personality judgments: convergent evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuroimage. 2005, 28 (4): 770-777. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.064.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Vollm BA, Taylor AN, Richardson P, Corcoran R, Stirling J, McKie S, Deakin JF, Elliott R: Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. Neuroimage. 2006, 29 (1): 90-98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. den Ouden HE, Frith U, Frith C, Blakemore SJ: Thinking about intentions. Neuroimage. 2005, 28 (4): 787-796. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Vogeley K, Bussfeld P, Newen A, Herrmann S, Happe F, Falkai P, Maier W, Shah NJ, Fink GR, Zilles K: Mind reading: neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. Neuroimage. 2001, 14 (1 Pt 1): 170-181. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0789.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR: The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. 2006, 129 (Pt 3): 564-583. 10.1093/brain/awl004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Morris JP, Pelphrey KA, McCarthy G: Regional brain activation evoked when approaching a virtual human on a virtual walk. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005, 17 (11): 1744-1752. 10.1162/089892905774589253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Pelphrey KA, Morris JP, Michelich CR, Allison T, McCarthy G: Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: an FMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements. Cereb Cortex. 2005, 15 (12): 1866-1876. 10.1093/cercor/bhi064.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Pelphrey KA, Morris JP, McCarthy G: Grasping the intentions of others: the perceived intentionality of an action influences activity in the superior temporal sulcus during social perception. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004, 16 (10): 1706-1716. 10.1162/0898929042947900.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Baron-Cohen S, Ring H, Chitnis X, Wheelwright S, Gregory L, Williams S, Brammer M, Bullmore E: fMRI of parents of children with Asperger Syndrome: A pilot study. Brain Cogn. 2006

    Google Scholar 

  82. Testa C, Laakso MP, Sabattoli F, Rossi R, Beltramello A, Soininen H, Frisoni GB: A comparison between the accuracy of voxel-based morphometry and hippocampal volumetry in Alzheimer's disease. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2004, 19 (3): 274-282. 10.1002/jmri.20001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Scarone S, Colombo C, Livian S, Abbruzzese M, Ronchi P, Locatelli M, Scotti G, Smeraldi E: Increased right caudate nucleus size in obsessive-compulsive disorder: detection with magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry Res. 1992, 45 (2): 115-121.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Breiter HC, Rauch SL, Kwong KK, Baker JR, Weisskoff RM, Kennedy DN, Kendrick AD, Davis TL, Jiang A, Cohen MS, Stern CE, Belliveau JW, Baer L, O'Sullivan RL, Savage CR, Jenike MA, Rosen BR: Functional magnetic resonance imaging of symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996, 53 (7): 595-606.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Baxter LR, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC, Guze BH, Schwartz JM, Selin CE: Local cerebral glucose metabolic rates in obsessive-compulsive disorder. A comparison with rates in unipolar depression and in normal controls. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987, 44 (3): 211-218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Baxter LR, Schwartz JM, Bergman KS, Szuba MP, Guze BH, Mazziotta JC, Alazraki A, Selin CE, Ferng HK, Munford P, et al: Caudate glucose metabolic rate changes with both drug and behavior therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992, 49 (9): 681-689.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Schwartz JM, Stoessel PW, Baxter LR, Martin KM, Phelps ME: Systematic changes in cerebral glucose metabolic rate after successful behavior modification treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996, 53 (2): 109-113.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Chakos MH, Lieberman JA, Bilder RM, Borenstein M, Lerner G, Bogerts B, Wu H, Kinon B, Ashtari M: Increase in caudate nuclei volumes of first-episode schizophrenic patients taking antipsychotic drugs. Am J Psychiatry. 1994, 151 (10): 1430-1436.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Keshavan MS, Bagwell WW, Haas GL, Sweeney JA, Schooler NR, Pettegrew JW: Changes in caudate volume with neuroleptic treatment. Lancet. 1994, 344 (8934): 1434-10.1016/S0140-6736(94)90599-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Chakos MH, Lieberman JA, Alvir J, Bilder R, Ashtari M: Caudate nuclei volumes in schizophrenic patients treated with typical antipsychotics or clozapine. Lancet. 1995, 345 (8947): 456-457. 10.1016/S0140-6736(95)90441-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Scheepers FE, de Wied CC, Hulshoff Pol HE, van de Flier W, van der Linden JA, Kahn RS: The effect of clozapine on caudate nucleus volume in schizophrenic patients previously treated with typical antipsychotics. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001, 24 (1): 47-54. 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00172-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Boada R, Ogline JS, Tunick RA, Chhabildas NA, Olson RK: A comparison of the cognitive deficits in reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001, 110 (1): 157-172. 10.1037/0021-843X.110.1.157.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

Funding for this study was provided by Public Health Service grant P01 HD35468 (SJ Rogers) and the National Alliance for Autism Research (DC Rojas).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Donald C Rojas.

Additional information

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

DR conceived of the study, made the main contribution to the design and analysis, performed all VBM analyses and drafted the manuscript. EP, EW, MR and JT participated in the design of the study. SR made a substantial contribution to the selection and use of the clinical measures reported in the study.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rojas, D.C., Peterson, E., Winterrowd, E. et al. Regional gray matter volumetric changes in autism associated with social and repetitive behavior symptoms. BMC Psychiatry 6, 56 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-6-56

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-6-56

Keywords