Skip to main content

Construct validity and internal consistency reliability of the Loewenstein occupational therapy cognitive assessment (LOTCA)

Abstract

Background

Cognitive abilities are essential for children’s development and independence. Various cognitive assessments, standardized in Western cultures, have yet to be investigated for their multicultural suitability.

Aims

To explore the suitability of the Loewenstein Occupational Therapy Cognitive Assessment (LOTCA) for a Jordanian population.

Methods

Observed cases of 442 Jordanian children aged 6–12 were used to perform exploratory factor analyses using principal components with Varimax rotation (construct validity evidence) and to compute Cronbach’s α coefficient (internal consistency reliability).

Results

High total performance on four subscales and a slightly lower total performance on two subscales were observed. Observed performance increased with age on three subtests, whereas a more modest increase was observed on the other three subscales. The expected one-factorial solution confirming the LOTCA’s subscales homogeneity (unidimensionality) structure was found on five of six subscales. Variance explained by the subscales ranged from 39 to 82% and internal consistency reliability measured by Cronbach’s alpha ranged from .42 to .78.

Conclusions

Satisfactory construct validity and internal consistency reliability were demonstrated on two subscales applicable to Jordanian children without adaptation. With adequate cross-cultural adaptation, increasing internal consistency reliability in other subscales could make the LOTCA an effective tool for assessing cognitive abilities in this population.

Peer Review reports

Background

Cognitive abilities are skills that form the basis for every human action and enable the performance of tasks ranging from the simple to the complex. Such abilities, including perceptual discrimination, selective attention, memory, and language, are modified by the context in which they developed [1] and represent an evolving product of the interaction between people, activities, and various environmental factors, such as culture and socio-demographic status [2]. These factors determine the type of handling, care, and training given to children, the opportunities for interaction with the environment, and the objects on which they focus [1, 3, 4].

Children vary in their underlying cognitive abilities to process and use information, and these differences affect a child’s learning and academic performance [2]. Indeed, the critical importance of the early childhood years, for cognitive growth and later success, is becoming increasingly evident. Not only are some cognitive abilities, such as reading and mathematical skills, strengthened in school, but cognitive performance may also predict school readiness and scholastic success [3, 4]. Therefore, the assessment of children’s cognitive performance components may provide a basic understanding of their abilities and disabilities in school performance [1]. Early detection of high-risk and special needs children can enable the implementation of appropriate intervention during early learning years [3,4,5] and influence other aspects of adult life, including social and vocational life, and all life occupations [6,7,8].

Furthermore, each culture has its own patterns of practices that affect children’s development, behaviors, skills, and attitudes, as well as cognitive performance [3, 4, 9]. Clinicians and researchers worldwide utilize standardized Western tools with strong psychometric properties for clinical purposes, with or without appropriate cross-cultural adaptations. The use of standardized normative data for the purposes of practice with local populations, and the translation and adaptation of Western instruments for use with populations speaking other languages and from different cultures, has drawn increased attention in health-related research and practice [10,11,12]. A review of cross-cultural studies demonstrated that differences in performance on standardized developmental assessments may be attributed to cultural factors, and that standardized assessments may not be valid or reliable when used to evaluate individuals from a population different from the one in which the original instrument was standardized [9, 13,14,15].

Currently, there are several standardized assessments and screening instruments used by therapists and researchers in the area of cognitive abilities [4, 16], including the Kaufman Assessment Battery (KAB) for Children [17], the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) [18], and the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities [19]. Although these tests focus on assessing cognitive abilities, Occupational Therapists (OTs) are not eligible to administer the KAB, whereas the BRIEF is a questionnaire, which focuses solely on Executive Functions. The Loewenstein Occupational Therapy Cognitive Assessment (LOTCA), however, was developed for practice by OTs, and both clinicians and researchers routinely use it as a screening tool. One of its main advantages is its ability to assess a range of intact compared to impaired cognitive abilities, thereby enhancing the designing and planning of therapeutic interventions. In addition, the LOTCA was previously used with children in several studies [20] and specifically with Arabic speaking children [3, 4, 21]. The validity and reliability of the LOTCA was previously examined in studies where LOTCA was compared with other instruments. For example, Jang et al. [31] showed that the LOTCA is a valid measure of cognitive abilities for people with intellectual disabilities. They examined psychometric properties of the LOTCA and found correlations with the Pictorial IQ test [31]. The clinical decision to employ this instrument with Jordanian children was based on its strong psychometric properties, minimal language component (rendering translation and test adaptation easier than other Western standardized tests), as well as its extensive practical use by clinicians. For these reasons, we deemed it reasonable to use the LOTCA in the current study with Jordanian children. To date, there is limited documented information on the appropriateness of these standardized Western tests for children of different cultures, though research has suggested that the psychometric properties of a test may vary in different cultural groups [15].

The present study represents an attempt to fill this void by assessing the internal consistency reliability and construct validity evidence of the LOTCA [22] when used with Jordanian children. The purpose of this study was to explore the suitability of the LOTCA for use with Jordanian children, ages 6 to 12, by estimating the adequacy of the psychometric properties of construct validity and internal consistency reliability when administered to Jordanian children. It was hypothesized that the observed items within each of the test’s six subscales would confirm the LOTCA’s (a) homogeneity (unidimensionality) in terms of item structure (construct validity evidence) and (b) consistency structure of the items (internal consistency reliability). The results are expected to indicate the extent to which the LOTCA is valid for use with Jordanian children or whether differentiated norms are needed to accommodate cultural factors.

Methods

Study design

This cross-sectional study employed a retrospective design and used quantitative methodology (hypotheses driven).

Participants

The participants were 442 typically developing (TD) children, aged 6-12 years, enrolled in either public or private schools. Recruitment was conducted via random distribution of 540 flyers to schoolchildren the day before data collection and testing. The flyers contained a description of the study, requested information about the child (e.g., date of birth, known disorders or disabilities), and included a parental consent form. Children with known developmental, learning, or neurological/sensory disabilities, as reported by the teacher and/or the parents, were excluded from the study. Of the 540 flyers, only 98 (18%) recipients refused or were ineligible to participate.

Approval for conducting this study was obtained from the Institutional Research Board (IRB) at King Abdullah University Hospital and the Deanship of Scientific Research at Jordan University of Science and Technology in Jordan. Written parental consent and approval from the educational authorities were received prior to carrying out the study. After receiving the signed parental consent, trained research assistants assessed the cognitive abilities of the subjects using the LOTCA. The research assistants (rehabilitation therapists) were trained by the principle investigator on how to collect data, as well as how to administer and score the LOTCA battery. In order to obtain reliability between reviewers, both the principal investigator and the research assistants assessed and scored the same group of 20 children (10 males and 10 females aged 6-12 years) until 98% compatibility of the results was achieved among them. All subjects were assessed in a quiet environment in the school and parents were not present during the testing (which was administered between 8 am - 10 am or 11 am - 1 pm (i.e., before or after school recess). Average testing time ranged between 45 and 90 min (M = 60, SD = 12.4).

Instrumentation

Overview of the LOTCA

The LOTCA is a cognitive battery that was developed and designed for use by OTs working in neurological rehabilitation settings. The LOTCA consists of 26 items in six subscales and additional scores for test administration time, attention and concentration. The subscale items ranged from a four- or five- point scale. The individual items and their composite scoring are as follows: 1) orientation, includes two items: orientation to place and to time (composite scores range from 2 to 16); 2) visual perception, includes four items: object identification, shape identification, overlapping figures and object constancy (composite scores range from 4 to 16); 3) spatial perception, includes three items: directions on client’s body, spatial relations, and spatial relations in pictures (composite scores range from 3 to 12); 4) motor praxis, includes three items: motor imitation, utilization of objects, and symbolic actions (composite scores range from 3 to 12); 5) visuo-motor organization, which examines perceptual-motor integration with spatial components, including seven items: copying geometric forms, reproduction of two- and three-dimensional models, pegboard construction, colored and plain block designs, reproduction of a puzzle, and drawing a clock (composite scores range from 7 to 28); 6) thinking operations, which examines categorization and sequencing, including seven items: pictorial classification, object classification (structured and unstructured), pictorial sequencing, and geometrical sequencing (composite scores range from 7 to 35). Results are presented as a profile along all subscales.

The test has an inter-rater reliability coefficient ranging from .82 to .97 for its six subscales. Internal consistency and construct validity were confirmed by Katz, Itzkovich, Averbuch & Elazar (1989) and Najenson et al. (1984) for perception, visuomotor organization and thinking operation [23, 24]. In their study on people with stroke and brain injury, Katz et al. (1989) found the LOTCA to have discriminant validity for all subscales aside from the identification of objects [23].

Although the LOTCA was standardized in Western populations and has not yet been standardized for use in other populations, it has the potential for evaluating individuals from non-Western populations [20]. As mentioned above, several studies have used the LOTCA to study the cognitive abilities of Arab children [3, 4, 21]. The results of these studies suggest that culture may explain the differences in performance and scores between cultural groups and the lack of cultural adaptation may have affected the construct validity and internal consistency reliability of the LOTCA. However, these studies were performed without prior testing as to whether the LOTCA’s normative data required cross-cultural adaptations.

A composite score for each of the LOTCA’s six subscales was calculated by totaling the scores of the relevant items. The maximum score on the test is 123, and the minimum score is 27. A higher score indicates better cognitive performance. Raw scores were used in this study, as the LOTCA battery has not yet been standardized for Jordanian children.

The English version of LOTCA, including the instruction manual, was translated into Arabic using a backward-forward translation process [21]. Discrepancies in the translation of specific items were discussed until a consensus was reached. A pilot study with 20 children was carried out in order to examine the readability of the initial versions of the translated scale; the items were revised accordingly and then were piloted to 20 other children. At that point, researchers unanimously agreed that no more amendments were needed. This translated Arabic version of the instrument was then retranslated into English by a bilingual speaker who was unfamiliar with the original versions of the instrument. This process of backward translation was evaluated by ten bilingual professors working at Arabic universities.

The scores of the instrument in evaluating the translation, ranged from 0 (not similar) to 1 (similar) and a cut off score of at least 0.80 was identified in order to assess the adequacy of the Arabic translated version, implying that 80% or more of the evaluators agreed that the backward-translated item had the same meaning as the original item. A score below 0.80 suggested a possible problem with the translation. After the translation stage was completed and modifications were made, all translated items achieved the cutoff score of 0.80.

Data analysis and management

Descriptive statistics (M and SD) were computed to describe the distributions of the test’s six subscales and socio-demographic data. Exploratory factor analyses (EFA) using principal components extraction model with Varimax rotation were performed on the six subscales.

The Kaiser’s criterion, used to determine the number of factors to retain, suggests retaining all factors that are above the eigenvalue of 1 [25, 26]. All 26 items were found to have enough variance and were maintained in the factor analysis and Cronbach’s alpha computations. The minimum Eigenvalue for rotated factors was established at one [27] and the criterion for determining whether an item loaded substantially on a subscale was established at .50 [27]. Cronbach’s alpha was computed for the items within each of the six subscales. Cronbach’s alpha [28] indicated 0.7 to be a satisfactory reliability coefficient; however, lower thresholds are occasionally used in the literature. There are no widely accepted criteria for defining a strong vs. moderate association. Portney and Watkins (2015) suggest values of r ranging from .50 to .75 to represent moderate to good relationship and those ranging from .75 and above to be good to excellent relationship [29].

EFA generated an expected one-factor solution showing the expected unidimensionality factor structure for five of the six LOTCA subscales. The EFA yielded a two-factor solution showing multidimensionality for the subscale Thinking Operation. An EFA without Pictorial B (the cross loading item) generated a one-factor solution with a slightly increased amount of explained variance and Cronbach’s α, confirming the expected homogeneity (unidimensionality) structure of this LOTCA subscale and was used in the analysis. Significance was set at the 0.05 probability level. All operations were carried out using SPSS Version 20 (SPSS, 2012).

Results

The 442 Typically Developing (TD) children who participated in this study ranged in age from 6 to 12 years, with a mean of 9.12 ± 1.9 years. The sample was almost equally divided between males and females (51.4% males), urban vs. rural areas (48.6% urban), and public vs. private schools (49% in public schools). The majority (96.2%) of the subjects lived with both parents and 46.4% of the parents had an average yearly family income of JD 6000 ($ 8600).

Associations between cognitive functioning and independent demographic variables (i.e.; child’s age, area of residence, hours of sleep, parent’s occupation etc.) were analyzed using a multivariate linear regression model. This description is provided in our previous publication [21].

The total observed mean performance score of four of the six domains is located toward the higher end of the possible composite range score, indicating high observed performance on these domains [orientation [M = 13.32 (SD = 3.32)- composite scores ranging from 2 to 16; visual perception, M = 14.53 (SD = 1.36)- composite scores ranging from 4 to 16; spatial perception, M = 11.30 (SD = 1.33) - composite scores ranging from 3 to 12; and motor praxis, M = 11.42 (SD = .99), composite scores ranging from 3 to 12]. While the total mean score of the other two subscales is slightly lower, performance on these subscales is still quite high: [visuo-motor organization, M = 22.35 (SD = 4.23) - composite scores ranging from 7 to 28; thinking operations, M = 24.71 (SD = 4.36- composite scores ranging from 7 to 31] (see Table 1).

Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations of the LOTCA Six subscales for the Seven Age Groups (N = 442)

As shown in Table 2, the observed mean performances on each of the six LOTCA subscales were significantly correlated with one another. Of the 15 Pearson correlation coefficients, there were 12 significant small correlations (ranging from r = .22; p ≤ .001 to r = .44; p ≤ .001) and three moderate to good coefficients ranging from (r = .53; p ≤ .001 to r =. 69; p ≤ .001). These correlations suggest that all subscales measure the same construct and provide satisfactory levels and evidence of construct validity.

Table 2 Pearson Zero Order Correlation Coefficients for Correlations between the LOTCA Subscales (N = 442)

Results of the exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and Cronbach’s α for the subscale orientation generated the expected one-factor solution with an eigenvalue of 1.64, confirming the LOTCA’s homogeneity (unidimensionality) factor structure and thus providing construct validity evidence. Accounting for approximately 82% of the variance, with a Cronbach’s α of .76, results show a satisfactory level of internal consistency reliability. Therefore, this subscale may be used with Jordanian children ages 6 to 12 without cross-cultural adaptation (see Table 3). Results for the visomotor organization subscale yielded an expected one-factor solution with an eigenvalue of 3.06, confirming the LOTCA’s homogeneity (unidimensionality) factor structure and providing adequate construct validity evidence. Accounting for approximately 44% of the variance with a Cronbach’s α .78 this subscale exhibits a satisfactory level of internal consistency reliability. Consequently, this subscale may be used with Jordanian children ages 6 to 12, without cross-cultural adaptation (see Table 3).

Table 3 LOTCA Subscales (Not Requiring Cultural Adaptation) Factor Loadings, Eigenvalues, Communalities, Percent of Explained Variance and Cronbach’s Alpha (N = 442)

EFA for the subscale visual perception yielded an expected one-factor solution with an eigenvalue of 1.58, confirming the LOTCA’s homogeneity (unidimensionality) factor structure and providing an adequate level of construct validity accounting for approximately 40% of the variance. The Cronbach’s α of .47 does not provide a satisfactory level of internal consistency reliability. This subscale may not be used with Jordanian children ages 6 to 12 without adequate cross-cultural adaptation to increase its internal consistency reliability (see Table 4).

Table 4 LOTCA Subscales (Requiring Cultural Adaptation) Factor Loadings, Eigenvalues, Communalities, Percent of Explained Variance and Cronbach’s Alpha (N = 442)

EFA for the spatial perception subscale generated an expected one-factor solution with an eigenvalue of 1.75, confirming the LOTCA’s homogeneity (unidimensionality) factor structure for this subscale, demonstrating a satisfactory level of construct validity evidence. This solution accounted for approximately 58% of the variance. Cronbach’s α was .63, indicating a questionable level of internal consistency reliability. Consequently, this subscale may not be used with Jordanian children ages 6 to 12 without cross-cultural adaptation to increase its internal consistency reliability (see Table 4).

EFA for the motor praxis subscale generated an expected one-factor solution with an eigenvalue of 1.54, confirming the LOTCA’s homogeneity (unidimensionality) factor structure demonstrating a satisfactory level of construct validity. This solution accounted for approximately 52% of the variance. Cronbach’s α was .47, exhibiting an unacceptable level of internal consistency reliability evidence. Consequently, this subscale may be not be used with Jordanian children ages 6 to 12 without cross-cultural adaptation to increase its internal consistency reliability evidence (see Table 4).

Factor analysis for the modified thinking organization subscale generated a one-factor solution exhibiting the expected unidimensionality with and eigenvalue of 2.645. The LOTCA’s homogeneity (unidimensionality) factor structure of this modified subscale was confirmed, demonstrating unacceptable level of construct validity. This solution accounted for approximately 44% of the total variance. Cronbach’s α was .74 exhibiting a satisfactory level of internal consistency reliability. Although construct validity and internal consistency reliability evidence were established, this modified subscale may not be used with Jordanian children ages 6 to 12 without a significant cross-cultural adaptation designed to further study the possible consequences of eliminating the item Pictorial B from the subscale (see Table 4).

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to explore the suitability of the LOTCA for use with Jordanian children. The findings show that overall, the translation and use of the LOTCA with Jordanian children ages 6 to 12 demonstrated sufficient additional cross-cultural adaptation efforts, the LOTCA could be an effective “culture-free” tool for assessing the cognitive abilities of Arabic speaking Jordanian children ages 6 to 12.

Construct validity - LOTCA internal subscale structure

The computation of Pearson correlations provided additional validation for estimating the construct evidence of the LOTCA’s test structure. The six LOTCA subscales are presumed to assess different aspects of cognitive ability and as such, are expected to be positively related to one another. Indeed, the observed correlations among the six LOTCA subscales were all significant. While a small-observed association was found between Motor Praxis and Orientation, the rest of the correlations represented mostly moderate associations. Therefore, while supporting the test structure proposed by the LOTCA between the subscales, the mostly modest correlations also suggest that each subscale item represents a distinct construct of cognitive ability contained in the cognition and visual perception functions inherent in the developmental theory being tested in the LOTCA. Thus, in order for the LOTCA to maintain its effectiveness in assessing cognitive abilities in this population, all six subscales should be retained, though additional cross–cultural research is needed for four of the six subscales.

Construct validity – LOTCA within subscales Unidimensionality structure

The EFA generated five one-factor solutions and one two-factor solution, confirming the expected homogeneity (unidimensionality) factor structure of the items within five of the six LOTCA subscales. All solutions had eigenvalues that satisfied the Kaiser’s criterion used to determine the number of retaining factors. The initial EFA for the thinking operations subscale yielded a two-factor solution and the homogeneity (unidimensionality) factor structure of the LOTCA subscales was not confirmed.

The thinking operations subscale involves higher mental processes such as executive function, problem solving, abstraction, and logical operation [22] and socio-cultural background is known to affect such thinking patterns [30]. Therefore, this subscale may be sensitive in identifying differences between children from different cultures. This claim is supported by Jang, Chern & Lin (2009) who showed that the LOTCA is a valid measure of cognitive abilities among people with intellectual disabilities. In examining known group validity, thinking operations was the most sensitive subscale for identifying differences between the ID and the healthy groups [31].

The item Pictorial Sequence B was loaded above the cutoff point on both factors, becoming a cross loading item [26, 32]. A possible explanation is that each factor defines a distinct cluster of interrelated items, and items in cross-loading situations are usually eliminated from the analysis [26, 32]. In the present study, Pictorial Sequencing B can be eliminated, as the skills measured by this item are measured by the other items in the subscale.

The pictorial sequence has various underlying components, such as cognitive abilities of judgment, sequence, organization, and comprehension, which may already be measured by other items in the thinking operations subscale. A cross-cultural adaptation process can help assess whether the subscale contains enough information to adequately cover what it is intended to measure without this specific item.

In the present study, the criterion for determining whether an item loaded substantially on a subscale was established at .50 [27]. In twenty-four of the 26 items contained in the LOTCA four of the six subscales, namely, Orientation, Spatial Perception, Motor Praxis and Thinking Operations subscales loaded above the established cutoff point, ranging from .61 to .91. It is important to note that the loadings of these items demonstrated a satisfactory construct validity evidence for these subscales [26]. The relatively high loadings of these items provide evidence of the strength of the correlations between them and of the meaningful contribution of each item within each of the four subscales [33].

The loadings of the item Object Identification (.47), part of the Visual Perception subscale and Two Dimensional Model (.48), part of the Visomotor Organization subscale, were slightly below the cut-off point. These relatively low loadings show that their contribution to each subscale is less meaningful [33]. These findings also indicate that the dimensions of the Visual Perception and Visu-Motor Organization subscales are not sufficiently accounted for by their items and thus do not demonstrate satisfactory evidence for construct validity for these subscales [26]. Object Identification and Two Dimensional Model are simple items, originally prepared for people with brain injuries. Thus, it is not surprising that TD children completed these items easily and obtained high scores on them, creating a ceiling effect.

These findings affect the ability of Arabic speaking Jordanian clinicians to interpret the composite score of these subscales as a reflection of all the subscale items [34]. Thus, for the LOTCA to be an effective tool for assessing cognitive abilities with Jordanian children there is a need for cultural adaptation in order to achieve satisfactory levels of construct validity.

Internal consistency reliability – LOTCA subscales

In the present study, the estimates of internal consistency reliability were obtained using Cronbach’s alpha. As the coefficient alpha is affected by test length and subject to inflation, it has been suggested that alpha should be calculated for each of the subscales rather than for the entire test [28, 35, 36]. Therefore, in this study, estimates of internal consistency validation were calculated for each of the six subscales individually. The calculated coefficients generated demonstrated different levels of internal reliability evidence among the six subscales. Satisfactory levels of internal consistency reliability evidence (α = .76) were found for Orientation, Visomotor and Thinking operations subscales. For example, Visomotor and Thinking operations were significantly correlated at the .69 level. Hence, the skills needed to perform the Visomotor subscale are also the basis of the Thinking operation subscale and indicate a satisfactory degree to which the items comprising each subscale measure the same construct [34] in the present study, regardless of culture. For example, although the item ‘pegboard construction’ is under Visomotor organization, its underlying components include a categorization component, such as which color the subject should choose. Such item homogeneity directly affects the ability of a Jordanian clinician to interpret the LOTCA composite clinical score as a reflection of all the measures comprising each of these subscales. The items on these subscales are correlated with each other regardless of culture differences.

A questionable level of internal consistency reliability evidence (α = .63) was found for the subscale Spatial Perception, indicating an insufficient ability to measure the same construct. In a previous study (Josman et al., 2010) which compared the cognitive functioning of Israeli and Palestinian children using the LOTCA, the spatial perception subscale was included in different factors in each group. In the Israeli group, spatial perception was included in a factor named Visuo-motor organization and sequence abilities and loaded .62 while in the Palestinian group it was included in a factor named temporal and spatial perception and loaded .45. The study authors noted that these findings were due to cultural differences such as age of entry to the educational system and engagement in different cognitive tasks.

Unacceptable levels of internal consistency reliability evidence (α = .47) were found for the Visual Perception and Motor Praxis subscales, clearly indicating that the measures comprising these subscales do not jointly measure the same construct and lack item homogeneity. Since the subscale’s measures are linearly combined into a composite score, the concern for item homogeneity directly affects the ability of clinicians to interpret composite scores on these subscales. Since the evidence of measures of homogeneity was absent, these subscales require cultural adaptation before they can be of utility to Arabic speaking Jordanian clinicians.

The value of Cronbach’s alpha for the Orientation, Visu-Motor and Thinking subscales was within satisfactory level of internal reliability (76), indicating a relatively low level of error variance for the measures contained in each of these subscales; thus they can be considered reliable as a single construct even without cultural adaptation. The Cronbach’s alpha for the other three subscales, Spatial Perception, Visual Perception and Motor Praxis, was below the accepted level of internal reliability, indicating a high level of error variance for the measures contained in these subscales to be considered as a single construct. This might be due to the high dimensionality in the measures, more often associated with low values for Cronbach’s alpha while not necessarily indicating that the measures are not adequate or reliable [37]. The extent to which the measures in each subscale exhibit high dimensionality should be further researched, and the high level of error variance in the measures needs to be addressed. The high fraction of error variance associated with the measures contained in these three subscales limits their clinical utility and thus the ability of a clinician to perform a reliable assessment.

Limitations

Although there are several important implications for the current study, some limitations should be addressed. First, future psychometric research on the LOTCA should include other aspects of validity and reliability not undertaken in the present study. There is also a need to assess the association between test performance and academic achievement. Second, children with known disabilities were excluded from the current study. While including those children might have added to the discriminating value of the LOTCA, it is recommended to address this issue in future studies. Third, the administration and especially scoring of the LOTCA requires some training of personnel, because not all items are non-verbal. Therefore, some items are not suitable for people with hearing/speech problems. In addition, the test is time-consuming. Therefore, it might be that some aspects of the results are attributable to instrument administration.

Conclusions

The translation and use of the LOTCA demonstrated satisfactory evidence of construct validity and internal consistency reliability in two subscales, Orientation and Visu-motor organization. The one-factor solutions obtained for each subscale confirmed the LOTCA subscale homogeneity (unidimensionality) structure and the above-satisfactory levels of Cronbach’s alpha attested to the consistency of the measures within these subscales and hence, to satisfactory internal consistency reliability evidence. These findings suggest that these two subscales were found to be free of cultural bias and could be used with Jordanian children without the need for cultural adaptation. With adequate cross-cultural adaptation efforts to adapt the other four subscales, the LOTCA may be used as an effective tool for assessing cognitive abilities with Jordanian children ages 6 to 12. Future research should include additional representative samples. Future psychometric research on the LOTCA to include other aspects of validity and reliability not undertaken in the present study is also needed, as well as assessing the association of test performance with academic achievements.

Abbreviations

BRIEF:

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function

EFA:

Exploratory Factor Analyses

KAB:

Kaufman Assessment Battery

LOTCA:

The Loewenstein Occupational Therapy Cognitive Assessment

TD:

Typically developing

References

  1. Bjorklund DF. Children’s thinking: development function and individual differences. 5th ed. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Messerer J, Hunt E, Meyers G, Lerner I. Feuerstein’s instrumental enrichment: a new approach for activating intellectual potential in learning disabled youth. J Learn Disabil. 1984;17:322–5.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Josman N, Abdallah TM, Engel-Yeger B. Using the LOTCA to measure cultural and sociodemographic effects on cognitive skills in two groups of children. Am J Occup Ther. 2011;65:29–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Josman N, Abdallah TM, Engel-Yeger B. Cultural factors affecting the differential performance of Israeli and Palestinian children on the Loewenstein occupational therapy cognitive assessment. Res Dev Disabil. 2010;31(3):656–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Uynik M, Aki E, Ger T, Bum G, Kayiha H. Cognition in 4± 11 year old children in Turkey. Pediatric Rehabilitation. 1999;3:119–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Al-Mekhlafi HM, Mahdy MA, Sallam AA, Ariffin WA, Al-Mekhlafi AM, Amran AA, Surin J. Nutritional and socio-economic determinants of cognitive function and educational achievement of aboriginal schoolchildren in rural Malaysia. Br J Nutr. 2011;106(7):1100–6.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Brands, B., Egan, B., Györei, E., López-Robles, J.C., Gage, H., Campoy, C.,. .. Raats, M.M. (2012). A qualitative interview study on effects of diet on children's mental state and performance. Evaluation of perceptions, attitudes and beliefs of parents in four European countries. Appetite, 58(2), 739–746.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Kobrosly RW, Seplaki CL, Jones CM, Van Wijngaarden E. Physiologic dysfunction scores and cognitive function test performance in U.S. adults. Psychosom Med. 2012;74(1):81–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Byrne B, Watkins D. The issue of measurement invariance revisited. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology. 2003;34:155–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Diekmann A. Methodological Artefacts, data manipulation and fraud in economics and social Sciences. In: Lucius & Lucius; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Shaikh W, Patel M, Singh S. Sleep deprivation predisposes adolescents to obesity. Indian J Community Med. 2009;34(3):192–4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Yu H, Kaufman D. Cognitive evaluation of four online search engines for answering definitional questions posed by physicians. Pac Symp Biocomput. 2007;12:328–39.

  13. Avi-Itzhak T, Obler DR, Abdallah TM, Engel-Yeger B, Josman N. Testing the cross-cultural clinical utility of the VMI for Palestinian, Israeli and American typically developing kindergarten children. Child Adolescent Behavior Journal. 2014;2:136–43.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Engel-Yeger B. Validating the adolescent/adult sensory profile and examining its ability to screen sensory processing difficulties among Israeli people. British Journal of Occupational. Therapy. 2012;75(7):321–9.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Liao HF, Yao G, Wang TM. Concurrent validity in Taiwan of the comprehensive developmental inventory for infants and toddlers who were full term infants. Percept Mot Skills. 2008;107(1):29–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Tzuriel D, Kaufman R. Mediated learning and cognitive modifiability: dynamic assessment of young Ethiopian immigrant children to Israel. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 1999;30:359–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Kaufman assessment battery for children. 2nd ed. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior rating of executive function. Lutz: FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Woodcock R, Mather N, McGrew K. Woodcock-Johnson III test of cognitive abilities Examiner’s manual. Itasca: Riverside; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Katz N, Kizony R, Parush S. Visuo-motor organization and thinking operations performance of school age Ethiopian, Bedouin and main stream Israeli children. The Occupational Therapy Journal of Research. 2002;22:34–43.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Almomani F, Josman N, Almomani MO, Malkawi S, Almomani F, Mahdawi K, Nazzal M. Factors related to cognitive function among elementary school children. The Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2014;21:191–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Itzkovich M, Elazar B, Averbuch S, Katz N. Loewenstein occupational therapy cognitive assessment (LOTCA) battery. Manual and test kit. 2nd ed. Pequannock, NJ: Maddak; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Katz N, Itzkovich M, Averbuch S, Elazar B. Lowenstein occupational therapy cognitive assessment (LOTCA) battery for brain-injured patients: reliability and validity. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy. 1989;43:184–92.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Najenson T, Rahmani L, Elazar B, Averbuch S. An elementary cognitive assessment and treatment of the craniocerebrally injured patient. In: Edelstein BA, Couture ET, editors. Behavioral assessment and rehabilitation of the traumatically brain damaged. New York: Plenum; 1984. p. 313–38.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  25. Kaiser HF. The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educ Psychol Meas. 1960;20:141–51. https://doi.org/10.1177/001316446002000116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Yong AG, Pearce S. A beginner’s guide to factor analysis: focusing on exploratory factor analysis. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology. 2013;9(2):79–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Munro BH. Statistical methods for health care research. 5th ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Nunnally JC. Psychometric theory. 2nd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Portney G, Watkins P. Foundations of Clin Res : Applications to.Practice.3rd Edition. In: Davis Company; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Nisbett R, Peng K, Choi I, Norenzayan A. Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychol Rev. 2001;108(2):291–310.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Jang Y, Chern JS, Lin KC. Validity of the Lowenstein occupational therapy assessment in people with intellectual disabilities. Am J Occup Ther. 2009;63:414–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Costello B, Osborne W. Best Practices in Exploratory Factor Analysis: Four.Recommendations for Getting the Most FromYour Analysis. Practical Assessment. Research & Evaluation. 2005;10(7):1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Kline P. An easy guide to factor analysis. Abingdon-on-Thames: Routledge; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Henson RK. Understanding internal consistency reliability estimates: a conceptual primer on coefficient alpha. Meas Eval Couns Dev. 2001;34:177–89.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Tavakol M, Dennick R. Making sense of Cronbach’s alpha. International Journal of Medical Education. 2011a;2:53–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Tavakol M, Dennick R. Post-examination analysis of objective tests. Medical Teacher. 2011b;33:447–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Grau E. Using factor analysis and Cronbach’s alpha to ascertain relationships between questions of a dietary behavior questionnaire. Proceedings of the Survey Research Methods Section: American Statistical Association; 2007. http://ww2.amstat.org/sections/srms/proceedings/y2007f.html.

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was approved and funded by Deanship of Research at Jordan University of Science and Technology (Grant # 20120151) and Institutional Review Board of King Abdullah University Hospital/Irbid/Jordan.

Funding

This study was funded by Deanship of Research at Jordan University of Science and Technology (Grant # 20120151)/Irbid/Jordan. This institution provided funding for the process of data collection, entry and analysis.

Availability of data and materials

Supporting data can be accessed by emailing the corresponding author at falmomani@just.eu.jo.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors including FA, TAI, ND, MA and NJ made substantial contributions to conception and design, acquisition of data, analysis and interpretation of data, were involved in drafting the manuscript and revising it critically for important intellectual content, and have given final approval of this version to be published.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Fidaa Almomani.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Written parental consent form was obtained which needed to be signed if the child was eligible and the parents voluntarily agreed that their child would participate. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of King Abdullah University Hospital (Reference # 151) (i.e. ethics committee) and Deanship of Research of Jordan University of Science and Technology (Grant # 20120151).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Almomani, F., Avi-Itzhak, T., Demeter, N. et al. Construct validity and internal consistency reliability of the Loewenstein occupational therapy cognitive assessment (LOTCA). BMC Psychiatry 18, 184 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-018-1776-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-018-1776-x

Keywords