Skip to main content

Reduced P300 amplitude during retrieval on a spatial working memory task in a community sample of adolescents who report psychotic symptoms

Abstract

Background

Deficits in working memory are widely reported in schizophrenia and are considered a trait marker for the disorder. Event-related potentials (ERPs) and imaging data suggest that these differences in working memory performance may be due to aberrant functioning in the prefrontal and parietal cortices. Research suggests that many of the same risk factors for schizophrenia are shared with individuals from the general population who report psychotic symptoms.

Methods

Forty-two participants (age range 11–13 years) were divided into those who reported psychotic symptoms (N = 17) and those who reported no psychotic symptoms, i.e. the control group (N = 25). Behavioural differences in accuracy and reaction time were explored between the groups as well as electrophysiological correlates of working memory using a Spatial Working Memory Task, which was a variant of the Sternberg paradigm. Specifically, differences in the P300 component were explored across load level (low load and high load), location (positive probe i.e. in the same location as shown in the study stimulus and negative probe i.e. in a different location to the study stimulus) and between groups for the overall P300 timeframe. The effect of load was also explored at early and late timeframes of the P300 component (250-430 ms and 430-750 ms respectively).

Results

No between-group differences in the behavioural data were observed. Reduced amplitude of the P300 component was observed in the psychotic symptoms group relative to the control group at posterior electrode sites. Amplitude of the P300 component was reduced at high load for the late P300 timeframe at electrode sites Pz and POz.

Conclusions

These results identify neural correlates of neurocognitive dysfunction associated with population level psychotic symptoms and provide insights into ERP abnormalities associated with the extended psychosis phenotype.

Peer Review reports

Background

In recent years the importance of extending research in the area of psychosis to include those who report psychotic symptoms in the general population has been highlighted as a new approach to identifying individuals at increased symptomatic risk for developing a psychotic disorder [1]–[3]. Self-reports of psychotic symptomatology in adolescence have been found to be significantly associated with the development of a psychotic disorder in adulthood [4], [5], and the clinical psychosis phenotype (schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders) and non-clinical psychosis phenotype (individuals who do not have psychotic disorders but who report experiencing psychotic symptoms) have been shown to share many of the same risk factors [6]. In addition to increased risk for psychotic disorder, recent research has demonstrated that the majority of individuals in the population who report psychotic symptoms have at least one (non-psychotic) Axis-1 disorder [7], a finding which is in keeping with research in clinic-presenting young people with at risk mental states [8]–[11], who are considered to be at ‘ultra high risk’ for psychosis [12]. In fact, community-based research on young people who report psychotic symptoms has shown that this group is at high risk of multimorbidity [7] (that is, the presence of multiple co-occurring psychiatric disorders) and poorer illness course than individuals with psychopathology who do not report psychotic symptoms [13]. Individuals with psychopathology who report psychotic symptoms have also been demonstrated to be at high risk for severe suicidal behaviour [14], [15], making this group important both in terms of psychosis risk as well as in terms of risk for severe non-psychotic psychopathology.

Working memory deficits have been hypothesised as a core deficit in schizophrenia [16]. Wood et al. [17] state that working memory may represent a neurocognitive trait marker for schizophrenia, as it is considerably impaired throughout the illness and involves neural circuits deemed dysfunctional in the disorder. Aleman et al. [18] report that memory impairments do not seem to be modality specific and that clinical variables such as medication, duration of illness, patient status, severity of psychopathology and positive symptoms do not appear to influence the magnitude of memory impairment. In addition, no evidence of progressive decline in memory with duration of illness was observed in their meta-analysis, suggesting that memory impairment may be a trait rather than a state characteristic.

Both verbal and visuospatial working memory impairments have been observed in schizophrenia and first episode psychosis (FEP) patients [19]–[21] and groups defined as genetically at-risk of developing schizophrenia [22]–[25]. Similar impairments have been observed in groups considered clinically at-risk for psychosis, such as prodromal/Ultra High-Risk (UHR)/At-Risk Mental State (ARMS) groups [26]–[28], as well as groups who present putative antecedents of schizophrenia including psychotic-like experiences [29]. Visuospatial working memory deficits have also been reported in adolescent-onset schizophrenia [30], [31]. Most recently, deficits in spatial working memory have been reported in a community-based sample of adolescents reporting psychotic symptoms [32] and who met formal criteria for prodromal risk syndromes [33].

In a meta-analysis of the literature, Bramon et al. [34] state that the P300 waveform has been conceptualised as the physiological correlate of a working memory update of changes in the environment [35], or as an index of allocation of attentional resources [36]. According to Marchand et al. [37], many ERP studies of working memory have employed a modified version of the Sternberg task in which participants are presented with a set of digits/letters to memorise, followed by a “probe” digit. The participant must indicate if the probe was part of the original memory set (positive probe) or not (negative probe). Behaviourally, studies have shown that accuracy on working memory tasks decreases and reaction time increases as the memory load (number of items to be remembered) increases [38].

In a summary of previous research, Marchand et al. [37] state that the most consistent finding in the literature is the elicitation of a sustained parietal positivity (i.e. P300) to the probe (irrespective of whether the probe type is positive or negative) with an increase in latency as memory set size increases. Bledowski et al. [39] propose that the P3b component can be separated into two subcomponents; they propose that the early P3b subcomponent is related to stimulus evaluation processes, whereas the later P3b subcomponent reflects memory search processes in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, which access a posterior parietal storage buffer. Polich [40] proposes that the P3b originates from temporal-parietal activity associated with attention and appears to be related to subsequent memory processes.

Imaging studies of working memory have largely focused on the functioning of the prefrontal and the parietal cortices [PFC and PC; [41]–[43]. Studies have suggested a role for both the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC and vlPFC) in visuospatial working memory [43], [44]. Working memory deficits in schizophrenia patients and those genetically at-risk have been linked to abnormal brain activity in the PFC [23]. Studies have reported both hypoactivation [45], [46] and hyperactivation [47]–[50] of areas of the PFC in patients with schizophrenia. Keshavan et al. [51] reported decreased activity in the dlPFC and inferior PC in offspring of individuals with schizophrenia, while Broome et al. [52] reported decreased activity in the medial PFC and right precuneus in a group of ARMS participants during spatial working memory tasks.

A reduction of the P300 component in schizophrenia patients is one of the most consistent findings in the literature [34]. Galletly et al. [53] used a Two-In-A-Row (TIAR) auditory oddball task which they argue necessitates the repeated updating of target identity, thus requiring stimuli to be maintained in working memory. They reported reduced P300 amplitude in schizophrenia participants relative to controls for both non-target and target tones in their task. A reduction of the amplitude of the P3b component during encoding and retrieval on a visual working memory task has been reported in patients with early-onset (adolescent) schizophrenia [53]. Haenschel et al. [54] propose that the early posterior P3b peak observed in their study may reflect stimulus evaluation while the later P3b peak may reflect consolidation during encoding and template matching during retrieval [39], [53], [55].

To date, no electroencephalography (EEG) studies of spatial working memory have been carried out with adolescents who report psychotic symptoms. The present paper investigates the electrophysiological correlates of spatial working memory in a group of adolescents who reported psychotic symptoms compared to a matched group of controls. This was achieved using a Spatial Working Memory Task developed for use with an adolescent population based on the classic Sternberg working memory paradigm [38].

We hypothesised that there would be differences in ERP amplitude between those participants who reported psychotic symptoms and the control group for the P300 component in line with amplitude reductions reported in schizophrenia patients and adolescent-onset schizophrenia [53], [54]. While increases in latency and sustained amplitude of the P300 component were predicted for all participants as memory load increased, it was hypothesised that P300 amplitudes would be reduced in the psychotic symptoms group compared to the control group.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from the two most senior classes in primary schools in north Co. Dublin and counties Kildare and Meath. Details of the recruitment procedure have been previously reported [56]. Forty-two participants (21 male; age range 11–13 years; mean age = 12.14 years) completed the Spatial Working Memory Task after completing a clinical interview using the Kiddie- Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School Aged Children (6–18 Years) – Present and Lifetime Version [K-SADS-PL; [57]. Seventeen participants reported psychotic symptoms (7 male; age range 11–13 years; mean age = 12 years; 1 left handed). Twenty-five participants reported no psychotic symptoms (14 male; age range 11–13 years; mean age = 12.24 years; 0 left handed). Participants gave written informed assent, and parental consent was obtained before participation in the study. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no previous neurological disorders or brain injuries or family history of psychotic illness. This research was approved by the Ethics Committees of Beaumont Hospital and the National University of Ireland, Maynooth.

Assessment of psychotic symptoms and general functioning

The psychosis section from the K-SADS-PL [57] was used to probe psychotic symptoms such as hallucinations and delusions. The most commonly reported psychotic symptoms in the K-SADS interview were auditory hallucinations, which were often accompanied by some form of delusional beliefs. Auditory hallucinations that were deemed as clinically significant included voices commenting on behaviour, a voice giving commands, voices conversing, whispering voices and voices at varying volumes where the words cannot clearly be distinguished by the individual. Clinically significant delusions included bizarre ideas of being watched (e.g., by police using hidden security cameras in the home), unfounded ideas that others are saying negative things about the individual (which are distinguished from normal teenage self-consciousness), and a belief that ghosts are communicating directly with the individual. Extensive notes were recorded during the clinical assessment and symptoms were rated as psychotic based on a subsequent consensus meeting between three mental health professionals, blind to all information other than the psychosis section of the K-SADS-PL. Following the clinical interview a Children’s Global Assessment Scale score [C-GAS; [58] was assigned to assess each participants’ current level of functioning. The Wide Range Achievement Test–4 [WRAT-4; [59] was administered as a measure of scholastic ability.

Spatial working memory task

A Spatial Working Memory Task developed for use with an adolescent population based on the classic Sternberg working memory paradigm [28]. The task was written and presented in E-Prime E-Studio®. Participants were presented with a study array showing a number of target stimuli in fixed placeholders. The task consisted of 128 trials in total. Each block contained 32 test trials in which participants made a button press response, via a mouse with their index or middle finger, to indicate whether a stimulus was in a correct (positive probe) or incorrect (negative probe) location, respectively.

Stimuli were either fish in fishbowls or birds in bird cages generated in Microsoft Word using ClipArt (see Figure 1 for an example of a trial). For each block, participants were instructed to remember which placeholders (i.e. fishbowls or bird cages) contained target stimuli (i.e. fish or birds). Subsequently a target stimulus would appear in one of the placeholders and the participants would have to remember whether they had seen a stimulus in that placeholder.

Figure 1
figure 1

Example from block 1 (low load) of the working memory task displaying a fish bowl placeholder array, followed by the study array, followed by another placeholder array and a test stimulus in a correct location.

Specifically, participants were initially presented with the blank placeholder array for 1000 ms (which displayed the empty placeholders for that block). Participants were then presented with a study array which displayed a number of target stimuli in a subset of the placeholders. This study array was presented for 3500 ms. The presentation of the study array was then followed by another presentation of the blank placeholder array for a further 1000 ms. Finally, participants were presented with the test stimulus (one target stimulus presented in the placeholder array), which required the participant to make a button press response with their right hand using a mouse which terminated each trial to indicate whether the test stimulus was in a correct (left button) or incorrect (right button) placeholder. For the first half of each block participants viewed one study array, following which 16 probe stimuli using the fish/fishbowl stimuli were presented as test stimuli. The second half of each block contained one study array using the bird/birdcage stimuli, followed by a further 16 probe stimuli.

The task included 4 blocks (2 × low memory load and 2 × high memory load). Blocks 1 and 2 were classed as low memory load, while Blocks 3 and 4 were classed as high memory load. In Block 1, 4 out of 9 placeholders in the study array contained a target stimulus (as in Figure 1). In Block 2, 5 out of 12 placeholders in the study array contained a target stimulus. In Block 3, 6 out of 15 placeholders in the study array contained a target stimulus, while in Block 4, 7 out of 18 placeholders in the study array contained a target stimulus.

Figure 2
figure 2

Top - ERP waveforms showing group differences in mean amplitude for correct responses at Pz and POz. Bottom - ERP waveforms and corresponding scalp topographic maps (control group) showing group differences in mean amplitude for correct responses at Low Load and High Load at POz.

Procedure

E-Prime© logged response times for each participant and sent Transistor-Transistor Logic (TTL) triggers to the EEG acquisition PC to allow stimulus presentations (stimulus type) and responses to be logged in real time on the EEG recording. Response times were measured as the time between presentation of the test stimulus and the response, and were recorded for all trials. Response latencies and accuracy were calculated automatically by E-Prime© and average response times were collated in E-Prime© for each block.

EEG data were recorded in microvolts (μV) from 62 scalp sites positioned according to the extended international 10–20 system of electrode placement. The nasion was used as a reference. All impedances were below 10 kΩ. Vertical eye movements (VEOG) were recorded from electrodes located above and below the left eye and horizontal eye movements (HEOG) were recorded from the electrodes positioned at the outer canthus of each eye. Blinks were averaged off-line and a blink reduction algorithm was applied to the data. This algorithm involved automatic artifact correction [60], [61]. Stimulus-locked average ERPs were obtained by averaging the EEG using the test stimulus presentation as the trigger. Scalp sites chosen for further analysis were based on visual inspection of the scalp topographies for the task. ERP waveforms were maximal at midline recording sites for the task, therefore sites Pz and POz were chosen for further analysis of the P300 ERP component. ERP component time windows were chosen based on previous literature and visual inspection of grand averaged waveforms. Stimulus-locked epochs were defined as -100 ms pre-stimulus presentation until 1000 ms after stimulus presentation. Data were averaged for each participant and grand averages for each block were compiled. Only epochs in which participants made correct responses were included for group comparisons of ERP mean amplitude and latency analysis.

Statistical analyses

Statistical Analyses were carried out using SPSS Statistics version 20 for Windows. Demographic variables were compared using independent-samples t-tests and chi-square analyses. Separate 2×2 repeated measures ANOVA were used to examine differences in mean accuracy scores and mean reaction times (defined in milliseconds - ms) between high and low loads for the overall group. Independent-samples t-tests were used to test between-group differences in mean accuracy and mean reaction times. A 2×4 repeated measures ANOVA was used to compare the number of accepted trials for each group across each block of the task and no between-groups effect was observed [F(1, 38) = 0.2, p = 0.65]. Separate 2×2×2 mixed factorial ANOVAs were utilised to examine mean amplitude and mean latency differences respectively, with Load Level (low, high) and Location (positive probe, negative probe) as within-subjects variables, and Group (psychotic symptoms group, control) as the between-subjects variable to test for differences at each of the electrode sites chosen for further analysis (Fz, Pz, POz). Independent-samples t-tests were then used to further examine observed between-groups differences. Separate 2×2×2 mixed factorial ANOVAs were used to further examine mean amplitude difference at Pz and POz with Load Level (low, high) and Time (250 ms to 430 ms, 430 ms to 750 ms) as within subjects variables and Group (psychotic symptoms group, control) as the between-subjects variable. Latency was defined as the most positive data point within the specified component timeframe (Early P300 – 250-430 ms) and a 2×2 mixed factorial ANOVA was used to examine the effect of Load (low, high) across Groups (psychotic symptoms group, control). Independent-samples t-tests were then used to further examine observed between-group differences. For each ANOVA, an alpha value of 0.05 was used for main and interaction effects. Greenhouse-Geisser correction was employed where the assumption of sphericity was violated.

Results

Demographic comparisons

Groups were compared on age, gender, overall current C-GAS scores from the K-SADS-PL interview schedule [56] and scores on the WRAT-4 [59]. No between-group differences in mean age [t(40) = 0.97, p = 0.34], gender [χ 2(1) = 0.14, p = 0.71], handedness (χ 2(1) = 0.04, p = 0.844) or WRAT-4 scores [t(34) = −0.35, p = 0.73] – see Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic and general functioning details for the control and psychotic symptoms groups

Behavioural data

No significant differences in overall mean accuracy scores were observed between the control and psychotic symptoms groups [F(1, 39) = 0.07, p = 0.8]. No interaction effect was observed between load and group. A main effect of load was observed [F(1, 39) = 36.73, p < 0.0005] with accuracy higher for low load than at high load.

No significant differences in overall mean reaction time scores for correct responses were observed between the control and psychotic symptoms groups [F(1, 39) = 0.94, p = 0.34]. No interaction effect was observed between load and group. The main effect of load reached statistical significance [F(1, 39) = 16.17, p < 0.0005], with slower responses for high load.

Due to the presence of large standard error (SEM) in the psychotic symptoms group for high load, the coefficient of variation (standard deviation/mean) was calculated for reaction time data for both groups for both low and high load (see Figure 2). When tested with analysis of variance however, no significant difference was observed between the groups [F(1, 39) = 0.84, p = 0.37].

Figure 3
figure 3

Accuracy (mean and S.E.M.) and reaction times (mean in ms and S.E.M.) for the spatial working memory task.

Electrophysiological data

A between-groups effect was observed for mean amplitude at posterior electrode site Pz [F(1, 38) = 4.44, p = 0.04] for the timeframe 250 ms to 750 ms post-stimulus, with mean amplitude reduced in the psychotic symptoms group [Low Load: M = 4.15, SEM = 0.46; High Load: M = 3.57, SEM = 0.6] relative to the control group [Low Load: M = 4.94, SEM =0.37; High Load: M = 4.96, SEM =0.44]. A between-groups effect was also observed for mean amplitude at posterior electrode site POz [F(1, 38) = 7.53, p = 0.01], with mean amplitude reduced in the psychotic symptoms group [Low Load: M = 2.86, SEM = 0.55; High Load: M = 2.57, SEM = 0.68] group relative to the control group [Low Load: M = 4.42, SEM = 0.44; High Load: M = 4.61, SEM = 0.53]. For the same timeframe, no main or interaction effects of load or location were observed at electrode sites Pz or POz (see Figure 3).

Independent-samples t-tests revealed that when all correct responses were examined together, regardless of probe type, ERP amplitude at electrode site POz was reduced for both low load [t(40) = 2.21, p = 0.03] and high load [t(38) = 2.37, p = 0.02] for the timeframe 250 to 750 ms post-stimulus for the psychotic symptoms group compared to the control group. Significantly reduced mean amplitude was observed in the psychotic symptoms group relative to the control group at Low Load for Positive Probes [t(40) = 2.25, p = 0.03] and at High Load for Negative Probes [t(38) = 2.67, p = 0.01] at electrode site POz.

Due to the presence of two subcomponents of the P300, mean amplitude was assessed at Pz and POz for the timeframes 250 ms to 430 ms and 430 ms to 750 ms, respectively. A between-groups difference was observed at both electrodes [Pz: F(1, 38) = 4.11, p = 0.05; POz: F(1, 38) = 7.76, p = 0.01]. A Load * Time interaction [F(1, 38) = 7.18, p = 0.01] and a Load * Time * Group interaction were observed at POz [F(1, 38) = 5.68, p = 0.02]. Amplitude at POz was significantly reduced in the psychotic symptoms group at both early [250 ms to 430 ms; t(38) = 2.52, p = 0.02] and late time windows [430 ms to 750 ms; t(38) = 2.53, p = 0.02]. Further independent-samples t-tests revealed that mean amplitude for the psychotic symptoms group was reduced at low load for early [t(40) = 2.57, p = 0.01] and at high load for late [t(38) = 2.5, p = 0.02] time windows at POz. Mean amplitude was also reduced in the psychotic symptoms group at Pz for high load at the late timeframe [t(38) = 2.00, p = 0.05].

No main effect of load was observed for the latency of early P300 component at Pz [F(1, 38) = 0.01, p = 0.93] or POz[F(1, 38) = 0.61, p =0.44]. In addition, no between-group differences in the latency of the P300 component were observed at Pz [F(1, 38) = 0.1, p = 0.76] and POz [F(1, 38) = 0.97, p = 0.33] and no interaction effects were observed between load and group at either electrode site. In addition, mean reaction time was found to correlate negatively with mean amplitude at Pz at high load for the overall group [r = −0.4, p = 0.01; control: r = −0.326, p = 0.12; psychotic symptoms group: r = −0.48, p = 0.06]. This relationship was not observed at electrode site POz [overall group: r = 0.03, p = 0.86; control: r = 0.21, p = 0.32; psychotic symptoms group: r = −0.11, p = 0.69].

No between-groups effect on mean amplitude was observed at electrode site Fz for the timeframe 330 ms to 850 ms post-stimulus [F(1, 38) = 1.29, p = 0.26]. No main effects of load [F(1, 38) = 0.44, p = 0.51] or location [F(1, 38) = 1.35, p = 0.25] were observed, and no interaction effect [F(1, 38) = 2.58, p = 0.12] was observed at electrode site Fz for this timeframe.

Discussion

The current paper reports reduced parietal positivity related to spatial working memory in a group of adolescents who reported psychotic symptoms compared to a control group using a computerised task based on the Sternberg working memory paradigm [38]. A sustained posterior positivity was observed in all conditions of the task with reduced P300 amplitude at posterior electrode sites in the psychotic symptoms group relative to the control group.

Although the groups were matched for accuracy and reaction times, subtle differences in the EEG data were observed which indicates that the PLEs group may have recruited difference brain regions or engaged in different cognitive strategies during the task despite performing at the same level as the control group, behaviourally. Mean reaction times were found to correlate negatively with mean amplitude at Pz at high load for the overall group, indicating those participants who took longer to respond at high load also had reduced amplitude at this posterior electrode site for the more difficult levels of the task. Although this correlation did not remain significant when each group was examined separately, the result for the overall group was mainly affected by a trend within the psychotic symptoms group (p = 0.06). When the P300 component observed at Pz and POz was divided and the early (250 ms to 430 ms) and late (430 ms to 750 ms) aspects of the component were examined separately reduced amplitude was observed in the psychotic symptoms group relative to the controls for both timeframes. Reduced amplitude was observed for low load at the early time window at POz and high load at the late time window at POz and also at Pz.

As anticipated, for the overall group, statistically significant decreases in accuracy scores and increases in mean reaction time were observed from low load to high load, i.e. as difficulty increased from Block 1 to Block 4 of the task. Accuracy and reaction time scores did not differ between the control and psychotic symptoms groups overall or at either low load or high load. Despite no statistically significant differences in the behavioural data, the pattern of responses indicates that at high load the psychotic symptoms group were slower to respond with greater deviation from the mean response time.

Reduced amplitude for the later part of the P300 component at high load indicates that, unlike the control group, the psychotic symptoms group did not show the same level of sustained parietal positivity to the probe at the more difficult levels of the task which other evidence supports [37]. The P300 ERP has been conceptualised as the physiological correlate of working memory updating of changes in the environment or as an index of allocation of attentional resources and memory search processes [35], [36]. Haenschel et al. [54] interpreted a decrease in the amplitude of the early P3b component during working memory retrieval in a group with early-onset schizophrenia to reflect a deficit in the evaluation of the probe stimulus against the stimulus representations held in memory. The reduced amplitude of early P300 observed in the present study may reflect disrupted neural processes underlying stimulus evaluation in the psychotic symptoms group which has not yet reached a level which would cause behavioural impairment. Reduced amplitude of the P300 component, particularly the late P300 at high load, could reflect an underlying reduced attention and memory retrieval capacity in the psychotic symptoms group. While impaired behavioural performance is commonly observed in patients with schizophrenia alongside abnormal neural responses to working memory tasks, we speculate that the disrupted neural responses observed in the PLEs group in the present study could reflect early changes in neuroanatomy which have not yet reached a level which would cause behavioural impairment on this task. Similarly, using a Go/No Go task with fMRI, Jacobson and colleagues [62] reported differences in patterns of brain activations in children reporting subclinical symptoms compared to a control group. Task related differences were noted in the absence of between-group differences in accuracy or reaction times in this study.

The current study adds to the previous literature by highlighting the P300 component and reductions in the amplitude of this component as a potential marker of psychotic symptoms in adolescence. Previous research by Murphy et al. [63] found reduced amplitude of the P300 component in a group of adolescents who reported psychotic symptoms compared to a control group on a receptive language task. Individuals with psychotic symptoms were characterised by reduced accuracy and smaller P300 amplitude on a computerised version of the British Picture Vocabulary Scale [64]. Similar to the present study, Murphy et al. [63] did not observe any between-group differences in the latency of the P300 component in their study. Previous research has suggested that increased latency of the P300 component in psychotic disorders is associated with increased age and illness duration [65].

A previous study by Jacobson et al. [62] found evidence of disrupted prefrontal-temporal connectivity in a group of adolescents reporting psychotic symptoms. Polich [40] hypothesised that the later P3b component may reflect attention and subsequent memory processes, and may originate from temporal-parietal activity associated with these processes. Evidence for parietal dysfunction at the earliest stages of psychosis has also been proposed by Whalley et al. [66]. Despite performing at a similar level to the control group behaviourally, the reduced P300 component may reflect a disrupted temporal-parietal network in the psychotic symptoms group.

Limitations of the present study include the small sample size and the poor spatial resolution of ERPs. In addition, an extensive IQ test was not administered and as a result the groups cannot be compared on this variable. This study used electrophysiological measures to investigate the neurophysiological underpinnings of spatial working memory in an adolescent sample with self-reported psychotic symptoms. This study is to our knowledge the first study to test spatial working memory in this sample using ERP measures. The present study therefore adds to the current literature by revealing reduced P300 amplitude in the treatment-naive extended psychosis phenotype. Further research is needed in order to uncover the relationship between spatial working memory performance and risk for psychotic disorder. The results of this study suggest that the neural networks involved in spatial working memory may be disrupted early in adolescence prior to the onset of a psychotic disorder.

Conclusions

The present study expands existing findings of reduced P300 amplitude in adolescent onset schizophrenia [54] to the broader extended psychosis phenotype revealing reduced amplitude of the P300 ERP component in a community sample of adolescents who report psychotic symptoms during retrieval on a spatial working memory task. This reduction in P300 amplitude may reflect disrupted neural processes underlying stimulus evaluation and template matching during retrieval in the psychotic symptoms group. Reduced amplitude of the P300 component, particularly the late P300 at high load, observed in the present study could also reflect an underlying reduced attention and memory retrieval capacity in individuals with psychotic symptoms. These results identify neural correlates of neurocognitive dysfunction associated with population level psychotic symptoms and provide insights into ERP abnormalities associated with the extended psychosis phenotype. Follow-up studies may elucidate whether reduced P300 amplitude confers greater risk for developing a psychotic disorder in adulthood.

Abbreviations

μV:

Microvolts

ARMS:

At-risk mental state

C-GAS:

Children’s Global Assessment Scale

dlPFC:

Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

EEG:

Electroencephalography

ERP:

Event-related potential

FEP:

First-episode psychosis

K-SADS-PL:

Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia – Present and Lifetime version

PC:

Parietal cortex

PFC:

Prefrontal cortex

SEM:

Standard error

UHR:

Ultra high-risk

vlPFC:

Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex

WRAT:

Wide Range Achievement Test.

References

  1. Kelleher I, Keeley H, Corcoran P, Lynch F, Fitzpatrick C, Devlin N: Clinicopathological significance of psychotic experiences in non-psychotic young people: evidence from four population-based studies. Br J Psychiatry. 2012, 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.101543.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Kelleher I, Connor D, Clarke MC, Devlin N, Harley M, Cannon M: Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based studies. Psychol Med. 2012, 10.1017/S0033291711002960.

    Google Scholar 

  3. van Os J, Hanssen M, Bijl R, Ravelli A: Strauss (1969) revisited: a psychosis continuum in the general population?. Schizophr Res. 2000, 45: 11-20. 10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00224-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Poulton R, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Cannon M, Murray R, Harrington H: Children's self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: A 15-year longitudinal study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000, 57: 1053-1058. 10.1001/archpsyc.57.11.1053.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Welham J, Scott J, Williams G, Najman J, Bor W, O'Callaghan M: Emotional and behavioural antecedents of young adults who screen positive for non-affective psychosis: a 21-year birth cohort study. Psychol Med. 2009, 39: 625-634. 10.1017/S0033291708003760.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Kelleher I, Cannon M: Psychotic-like experiences in the general population: characterizing a high-risk group for psychosis. Psychol Med. 2011, 41: 1-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Kelleher I, Keeley H, Corcoran P, Lynch F, Fitzpatrick C, Devlin N: Clinicopathological significance of psychotic experiences in non-psychotic young people: evidence from four population-based studies. Br J Psychiatry. 2012, 201: 26-32. 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.101543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Addington J, Cornblatt BA, Cadenhead KS, Cannon TD, McGlashan TH, Perkins DO: At clinical high risk for psychosis: outcome for nonconverters. Am J Psychiatry. 2011, 168: 800-5. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10081191.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Lencz T, Smith CW, Auther A, Correll CU, Cornblatt B: Nonspecific and attenuated negative symptoms in patients at clinical high-risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004, 68: 37-48. 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00214-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Svirskis T, Korkeila J, Heinimaa M, Huttunen J, Ilonen T, Ristkari T: Axis-I disorders and vulnerability to psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2005, 75: 439-46. 10.1016/j.schres.2004.11.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Rosen JL, Miller TJ, D'Andrea JT, McGlashan TH, Woods SW: Comorbid diagnoses in patients meeting criteria for the schizophrenia prodrome. Schizophr Res. 2006, 85: 124-31. 10.1016/j.schres.2006.03.034.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Yung AR, McGorry PD, McFarlane CA, Jackson HJ, Patton GC, Rakkar A: Monitoring and care of young people at incipient risk of psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 1996, 22: 283-303. 10.1093/schbul/22.2.283.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Wigman JT, van Nierop M, Vollebergh WA, Lieb R, Beesdo-Baum K, Wittchen HU: Evidence that psychotic symptoms are prevalent in disorders of anxiety and depression, impacting on illness onset, risk, and severity–implications for diagnosis and ultra-high risk research. Schizophr Bull. 2012, 38: 247-57. 10.1093/schbul/sbr196.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Kelleher I, Lynch F, Harley M, Molloy C, Roddy S, Fitzpatrick C: Psychotic symptoms in adolescence index risk for suicidal behavior: findings from two population-based case–control clinical interview studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012, 69: 1277-83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kelleher I, Corcoran P, Keeley H, Wigman JTW, Devlin N, Ramsay H: Psychotic symptoms and population risk for suicide attempt: a prospective cohort study. JAMA Psychiatry.

  16. Goldman-Rakic PS: Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Journal Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosc. 1994, 6: 348-357.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Wood SJ, Pantelis C, Proffitt T, Phillips LJ, Stuart GW, Buchanan JA: Spatial working memory ability is a marker of risk-for-psychosis. Psychol Med. 2003, 33: 1239-1247. 10.1017/S0033291703008067.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Aleman A, Hijman R, de Haan EHF, Kahn R: Memory impairment in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 1999, 156: 1358-1366.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Kim J, Glahn DC, Nuechterlein KH, Cannon TD: Maintenance and manipulation of information in schizophrenia: further evidence for impairment in the central executive component of working memory. Schizophr Res. 2004, 68: 173-187. 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00150-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Gooding DC, Tallent KA: Nonverbal working memory deficits in schizophrenia patients: Evidence of a supramodal executive processing deficit. Schizophr Res. 2004, 68: 189-201. 10.1016/j.schres.2003.07.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Lee J, Park S: Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Journal of Abnorm. Psychol. 2005, 114: 599-611.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Glahn DC, Therman S, Manninen M, Huttunen MO, Kaprio J, Lonnqvist J: Spatial working memory as an endophenotype for schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2003, 53: 624-626. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01641-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Brahmbhatt SB, Haut K, Csernansky JG, Barch DM: Neural correlates of verbal and nonverbal working memory deficits in individuals with schizophrenia and their high-risk siblings. Schizophr Res. 2006, 87: 191-204. 10.1016/j.schres.2006.05.019.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Horan WP, Braff DL, Nuechterlein KH, Sugar CA, Cadenhead KS, Calkins ME: Verbal working memory impairments in individuals with schizophrenia and their first-degree relatives: findings from the consortium on the genetics of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2008, 103: 218-228. 10.1016/j.schres.2008.02.014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Bachman P, Kim J, Yee CM, Therman S, Manninen M, Lönnqvist J: Efficiency of working memory encoding in twins discordant for schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res.: Neuroimaging. 2009, 174: 97-104. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.04.010.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Smith CW, Park S, Cornblatt B: Spatial working memory deficits in adolescents at clinical high risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2006, 81: 211-215. 10.1016/j.schres.2005.09.019.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Pflueger MO, Gschwandtner U, Stieglitz RD, Riecher-Rossler A: Neuropsychological deficits in individuals with an at risk mental state for psychosis - Working memory as a potential trait marker. Schizophr Res. 2007, 97: 14-24. 10.1016/j.schres.2007.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Frommann I, Pukrop R, Brinkmeyer J, Bechdolf A, Ruhrmann S, Berning J: Neuropsychological profiles in different at-risk states of psychosis: executive control impairment in the early--and additional memory dysfunction in the late--prodromal state. Schizophr Bull. 2011, 37: 861-873. 10.1093/schbul/sbp155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Laurens K, Cullen A, West S, Morris R, Murray R, Hodgins S: Working memory dysfunction in children presenting psychotic-like experiences and other developmental antecedents of schizophrenia [abstract]. Schizophr Res. 2008, 102 (1–3, Supplement 2): 154-154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Vance A, Hall N, Bellgrove MA, Casey M, Karsz F, Maruff P: Visuospatial working memory deficits in adolescent onset schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2006, 87: 223-227. 10.1016/j.schres.2006.04.025.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Vance A, Hall N, Casey M, Karsz F, Bellgrove MA: Visuospatial memory deficits in adolescent onset schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2007, 93: 345-349. 10.1016/j.schres.2007.02.020.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Kelleher I, Clarke MC, Rawdon C, Murphy J, Cannon M: Neurocognition in the extended psychosis phenotype: performance of a community sample of adolescents with psychotic symptoms on the MATRICS neurocognitive battery. Schizophr Bull. 2012, http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2012/08/30/schbul.sbs086.long,

    Google Scholar 

  33. Kelleher I, Murtagh A, Clarke MC, Murphy J, Rawdon C, Cannon M: Neurocognitive performance of a community-based sample of young people at putative ultra high risk for psychosis: Support for the processing speed hypothesis. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2012, 10.1080/13546805.2012.682363.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Bramon E, Rabe-Hesketh S, Sham P, Murray RM, Frangou S: Meta-analysis of the P300 and P50 waveforms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004, 70: 315-329. 10.1016/j.schres.2004.01.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Donchin E, Coles M: Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?. Behav Brain Sci. 1988, 11: 406-417.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Posner M: Psychobiology of attention. Handbook of Psychobiology. Edited by: Gazzaniga M, Blakemore C. 1975, New York: Academic

    Google Scholar 

  37. Marchand Y, Lefebvre CD, Connolly JF: Correlating digit span performance and event-related potentials to assess working memory. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006, 62: 280-289. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.05.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Sternberg S: High-speed scanning in human memory. Science. 1966, 153: 652-654. 10.1126/science.153.3736.652.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Bledowski C, Kadosh KC, Wibral M, Rahm B, Bittner RA, Hoechstetter K: Mental chronometry of working memory retrieval: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and event-related potentials approach. J Neurosci. 2006, 26: 821-829. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3542-05.2006.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Polich J: Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007, 118: 2128-2148. 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Postle BR, Stern CE, Rosen BR, Corkin S: An fMRI investigation of cortical contributions to spatial and nonspatial visual working memory. Neuroimage. 2000, 11: 409-423. 10.1006/nimg.2000.0570.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Meda SA, Bhattarai M, Morris NA, Astur RS, Calhoun VD, Mathalon DH: An fMRI study of working memory in first-degree unaffected relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res. 2008, 104: 85-95. 10.1016/j.schres.2008.06.013.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Grimault S, Robitaille N, Grova C, Lina J-M, Dubarry A-S, Jolicœur P: Oscillatory activity in parietal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during retention in visual short-term memory: additive effects of spatial attention and memory load. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009, 30: 3378-3392. 10.1002/hbm.20759.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Curtis CE, D'Esposito M: Persistent activity on the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003, 7: 415-423. 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00197-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Cannon TD, Glahn DC, Kim J, Van Erp TGM, Karlsgodt K, Cohen MS: Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity during maintenance and manipulation of information in working memory in patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005, 62: 1071-1080. 10.1001/archpsyc.62.10.1071.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Schlösser RGM, Koch K, Wagner G, Nenadic I, Roebel M, Schachtzabel C: Inefficient executive cognitive control in schizophrenia is preceded by altered functional activation during information encoding: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2008, 46: 336-347. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Manoach DS, Press DZ, Thangaraj V, Searl MM, Goff DC, Halpern E: Schizophrenic subjects activate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during a working memory task, as measured by fMRI. Biol. Psychiat. 1999, 45: 1128-1137. 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00318-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Manoach DS, Gollub RL, Benson ES, Searl MM, Goff DC, Halpern E: Schizophrenic subjects show aberrant fMRI activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia during working memory performance. Biol. Psychiat. 2000, 48: 99-109. 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00227-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Perlstein W, Carter CS, Noll DC, Cohen JD: Relation of prefrontal cortex dysfunction to working memory and symptoms in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. 2001, 158: 1105-1113. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.7.1105.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Callicott JH, Egan MF, Mattay VS, Bertolino A, Bone AD, Verchinksi B: Abnormal fMRI response of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in cognitively intact siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. 2003, 160: 709-719. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.709.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Keshavan MS, Diwadkar VA, Spencer SM, Harenski KA, Luna B, Sweeney JA: A preliminary functional magnetic resonance imaging study in offspring of schizophrenic parents. Prog Neuro-Psychoph. 2002, 26: 1143-1149. 10.1016/S0278-5846(02)00249-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Broome MR, Fusar-Poli P, Matthiasson P, Woolley JB, Valmaggia L, Johns LC: Neural correlates of visuospatial working memory in the 'at-risk mental state'. Psychol Med. 2010, 40: 1987-1999. 10.1017/S0033291710000280.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Galletly CA, MacFarlane AC, Clark CR: Impaired updating of working memory in schizophrenia. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007, 63: 265-274. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.11.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Haenschel C, Bittner RA, Haertling F, Rotarska-Jagiela A, Maurer K, Singer W: Contribution of impaired early-stage visual processing to working memory dysfunction in adolescents with schizophrenia: a study with event-related potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007, 64: 1229-1240. 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Kok A: On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology. 2001, 38: 557-577. 10.1017/S0048577201990559.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Kelleher I, Murtagh A, Molloy C, Roddy S, Clarke MC, Harley M: Identification and characterization of prodromal risk syndromes in young adolescents in the community: a population-based clinical interview study. Schizophr Bull. 2012, 38: 239-246. 10.1093/schbul/sbr164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Ryan N: The schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school aged children: present and lifetime version. 1996, Pittsburgh, P.A.: University of Pittsburgh, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic

    Google Scholar 

  58. Shaffer D, Gould MS, Brasic J, Ambrosini P, Bird H, Aluwahlia S: A Children ’s Global Assessment Scale (CGAS). Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983, 40: 1228-1231. 10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790100074010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Wilkinson G, Robertson G: Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT-4). 2005, Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc, 4

    Google Scholar 

  60. Berg P, Scherg M: Dipole models of eye movements and blinks. Electroen Clin Neuro. 1991, 79: 36-44. 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90154-V.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  61. Ille N, Berg P, Scherg M: Artifact correction of the ongoing EEG using spatial filters based on artifact and brain signal topographies. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2002, 19: 113-124. 10.1097/00004691-200203000-00002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Jacobson S, Kelleher I, Harley M, Murtagh A, Clarke M, Blanchard M: Structural and functional brain correlates of subclinical psychotic symptoms in 11–13 year old schoolchildren. Neuroimage. 2010, 49: 1875-1885. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Murphy J, Blanchard MM, Rawdon C, Kavanagh F, Kelleher I, Clarke MC: Language processing abnormalities in adolescents with psychotic-like experiences: An event related potential study. Schizophr Res. 2012, 137: 91-96. 10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Dunn L, Whetton C, Burley J: British Picture Vocabulary Scale (BPVS-II) (2rd ed.). 1997, London: nferNelson

    Google Scholar 

  65. Frangou S, Sharma T, Alarcon G, Sigmudsson T, Takei N, Binnie C: The maudsley family study, II: endogenous event-related potentials in familial schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1997, 23: 45-53. 10.1016/S0920-9964(96)00089-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Whalley HC, Simonotto E, Marshall I, Owens DGC, Goddard NH, Johnstone EC: Functional disconnectivity in subjects at high genetic risk of schizophrenia. Brain. 2005, 128: 2097-2108. 10.1093/brain/awh556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

The research leading to these results was supported by an Essel Foundation NARSAD Independent Investigator Award. Caroline Rawdon is supported by an Irish Research Council Postgraduate Scholarship. Jennifer Murphy was supported by Health Research Award (HRA_PHS_2010/4). Ian Kelleher was supported by an Interdisciplinary Capacity Enhancement Award from the Health Research Board (Ireland) (ICE/2012/11). Fergal Kavanagh was supported by an Undergraduate Summer Studentship from the Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland. Mary Cannon was supported by a Health Research Board (Ireland) Clinician Scientist Award (CSA/2004/1). We wish to thank NUI Maynooth and the Clinical Research Centre, Beaumont Hospital for the use of their facilities. We acknowledge the technical assistance provided by Derek Walsh (NUI Maynooth) during the data acquisition.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Caroline Rawdon.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

CR, MC, RR, MCC and MB designed the study. CR, JM, MB, MC and IK recruited the participant sample. CR set up and recorded the EEG, analysed the data and wrote the first and final drafts of the manuscript. JM, MB and FK helped carry out EEG set up and recording. IK conducted clinical interviews with the participants and their parents. RR aided the writing process and the data analysis. MCC and MC also helped to write and structure the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Authors’ original file for figure 3

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rawdon, C., Murphy, J., Blanchard, M.M. et al. Reduced P300 amplitude during retrieval on a spatial working memory task in a community sample of adolescents who report psychotic symptoms. BMC Psychiatry 13, 125 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-125

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-125

Keywords