Skip to main content

Intrinsic and extrinsic goals as moderators of stress and depressive symptoms in Chinese undergraduate students: A multi-wave longitudinal study

Abstract

Background

Studies in western countries have examined the specific vulnerability hypothesis of Dykman’s theory of goal-orientation predispositions to depression through two-time point designs. The purpose of this prospective longitudinal study was to investigate the moderating effects of intrinsic and extrinsic goals on stress and depressive symptoms in Chinese undergraduate students.

Methods

A total of 462 undergraduate students [46 % female; mean age, 19.06 (range, 17–22) years] completed self-reported measures assessing intrinsic and extrinsic goals, depressive symptoms, and the occurrence of social and academic hassles. Every 3 months over the subsequent 12 months, the undergraduate students completed measures assessing depressive symptoms and the occurrence of daily hassles.

Results

Results of hierarchical linear modeling analyses indicated that undergraduate students with low levels of intrinsic goals reported greater depressive symptoms following the occurrence of social and academic hassles than did those with high levels of such goals. However, undergraduate students with high levels of extrinsic goals did not report greater depressive symptoms following the occurrence of social and academic hassles than did those possessing low levels.

Conclusions

These findings suggest that intrinsic goals can protect undergraduate students experiencing high levels of social and academic hassles from depressive symptoms. The study findings provide new insight into the course of depressive symptoms among undergraduate students, and offer psychologist and psychiatrists ways to protect individuals from depressive symptoms by building up intrinsic goals.

Background

Depression is the most serious mental health problem affecting undergraduate students, among whom rates of major depressive disorder range from 5.6 to 20 % [13]. Several recent studies have examined the prevalence of depression in university students worldwide [1, 4, 5]. Surveys conducted in the Chinese population have indicated that the prevalence of depressive symptoms among young Chinese undergraduate students ranges from 11.7 to 22.9 % [4, 6]. Depression in undergraduate students has been recognized as a major problem, as depressive symptoms are associated closely with suicidal behavior, substance abuse, college dropout, loss of academic productivity and antisocial behavior in this critical period of human development [7, 8]. Depression at college age also has a significant economic impact over the life course and serious consequences for later occupational trajectories [9].

Undergraduate students face a number of daily life stressors, and depressive symptoms have been shown to be closely related to various life events [10]. Problems that undergraduate students may face in this crucial stage, such as social competition, academic pressure, interpersonal may place them at high risk of developing negative effect (i.e., depressed mood) and, in serious cases, suicidal ideation and behavior [10, 11]. However, not all individuals who experience stressful life events develop depression, and a defense system can function to prevent this disease. The diathesis–stress framework posits that acute stressful life events and chronic stressful circumstances trigger an underlying predisposition, among the most promising explanations of how a depressive episode develops [12]. Such pre-existing vulnerability contributes to depressive symptoms and diagnoses only in the presence of life stress [13, 14]. However, the mechanism by which stress exerts this effect remains unclear.

Though the cognitive-diathesis-stress model is frequently used to explain people’s depressive symptoms, Dykman [15] listed three shortcomings of cognitive theories of depression and suggested these models lack “a vulnerability factor that is active or in motion prior to the occurrence of a negative event and that is capable of exerting a continuous influence on the depression-prone person’s functioning” (p.140). Shifting to the area of personal strivings and their relationships to motivated behavior, Dykman drew from the work of Dweck [16] and Eliott and Dweck [17], in which one’s implicit theories of learning and approach to a task determine subsequent engagement and achievement. He proposed a goal-orientation model of depression, positing that depression-prone and depression-resistant adults can be distinguished, at least in part, on the basis of whether they are primarily “validation seeking” (VS) or “growth seeking” (GS) in their personal strivings (or goal orientation). These scholars’ results supported the prediction that with increased stress, VS individuals would be more likely to experience depressive symptoms than GS individuals.

People’s goals vary. Self-determination theorists have distinguished intrinsic and extrinsic personal goals. Intrinsic goals, such as satisfying relationships, personal growth, and community contribution, are defined as pursuits that are generally congruent with the psychological needs for relatedness and autonomy [1820]. Extrinsic goals, such as fame, attractiveness, and wealth, depend on the contingent reactions of others and are primarily concerned with obtaining some reward or social praise [20]. As a subtheory of self-determination theory, goal content theory distinguishes these two types of goal and examines their effects on behaviors and well-being [19, 21]. Extrinsic and intrinsic goals are thought to relate differently to basic need satisfaction and therefore to produce different psychological outcomes. Researchers have demonstrated that the prioritization of financial success over intrinsic goals (e.g., those related to community and family) is associated with decreased vitality, depression, and anxiety [22], whereas striving for intrinsic life goals is related to greater well-being [21, 23]. Relative intrinsic goal content has been found to positively predict physical self-worth, self-reported exercise behavior, psychological well-being, and psychological need satisfaction, and to negatively predict exercise anxiety [24].

There were two major limitations of previous research examining goal-orientation diathesis–stress models. First, the majority of previous research examining the specific vulnerability hypothesis of Dykman’s theory of goal-orientation predispositions to depression has relied on two-time point designs in which (a) goal orientation and depressive symptoms assessed initially and (b) depressive symptoms and negative events are assessed during follow up [15, 25]. Such a design necessitates the use of a nomothetic approach to the operationalization of high stress levels. Second, most previous prospective investigations of relationships between goal-orientation styles and depressive symptoms within a diathesis–stress framework have been conducted in Western cultural contexts. In a recent preliminary study conducted in Japan [25], researchers found that social learning goals reduced the effects of interpersonal stress, thereby protecting against depression. Social performance-avoidance goals, however, exacerbated the effects of interpersonal stress, hence facilitating thedevelopment of depression [25]. However, this study explored only social goal orientation, not general goal orientation.

The current study addresses these shortcomings by utilizing a multi-wave longitudinal design in which levels of stress and depressive symptoms were assessed at multiple time points (every 3 months) during a 12-month follow-up period and an idiographic approach to analysis. More specifically, we examined whether the slope of the relationship between negative events and depressive symptoms varied among undergraduate students as a function of levels of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Consistent with the goal-orientation model and goal contents theory, lower intrinsic goals and higher extrinsic goals always related to higher level of depression [22], we hypothesized that (1) lower levels of intrinsic goals and (2) higher levels of extrinsic goals would be associated with greater within-subject increases in depressive symptoms following within-subject increases in stress levels.

Method

Participants

Participants were drawn from a five-wave longitudinal study of late-adolescent students from a university in Guangzhou, Guangdong, mainland China. The university was in urban setting, and all participants were living on campus. Only individuals who participated in at least two of five measurement waves [n = 472, 46 % female; mean age = 19.06 years, standard deviation (SD) = .89 years] were included in analyses. The ethnic distribution of these individuals (93.4 % Han, 6.6 % ethnic minority) is consistent with that in mainland China.

Procedure

Prior to the initial assessment, consent forms that detailed the aims of the study were sent to potential participants. Undergraduate students were allowed to participate only if personal consent was received. A total of 553 students were asked to participate in the study, and 502 students returned their consent forms signed by themselves. Among the 502 students, only individuals who participated in at least two of five measurement waves were included in analyses. Finally, 472 students had effective data, and the effective response rate was 94 %.

The initial assessment was taken in September, 2013 and all of the assessments were taken during the free time after school. During the initial assessment, participants completed a demographic form and the following questionnaires: (1) the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) [26], (2) the revised Aspiration Index (AI-R) [21], and (3) the General, Academic, and Social Hassles Scale for Students (GASHSS; Blankstein K, Flett G.Development of the general hassles scale for students.1993. Unpublished) [27]. Follow-up assessments were performed every 3 months for 12 months, during which participants completed the CES-D and GASHSS. It would take participants about 20 min to finish the initial assessment and about 10 min to finish each the follow-up assessment. Adolescents had just started their first year in college at the time of the first measurement wave, and were in their second year (if they had progressed with studies at a normative pace) at the time of the final measurement wave. The ethics committee of the South China Normal University approved the study, and all participants provided written informed consent before the survey began.

Measurement

Depressive symptoms

The CES-D [26], a 20-item self-reported instrument, was used to assess students’ depressive symptoms. As an important screening tool for depressive symptoms, Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale(CES-D) was widely used for assessing depressive symptoms exist among the normal and clinic sample from different age, gender and nationality [2831]. Participants were asked to rate the occurrence of each symptom in the past week on a scale ranging from 1 (rarely) to 4 (most of the time). Total scores range from 20 to 80, with higher scores indicating higher levels of depressive symptoms. Scores of 36 or higher are considered indicative of depression [32]. The CES-D has been shown in numerous studies to have strong test-retest reliability and to be strongly correlated with other measures of depressive symptoms [33, 34]. The Chinese version of the CES-D has exhibited high degrees of reliability and validity [35]. In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha values for the CES-D ranged from 0.89 to 0.93 across administrations.

Intrinsic and extrinsic goals

Intrinsic and extrinsic goals were measured using the 35-item self-reported AI-R [21, 36]. Responses are structured by a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very), with higher scores reflecting greater orientation toward intrinsic and extrinsic goals. The intrinsic goal score is the sum of scores for the self-acceptance, affiliation, community feeling, and physical fitness subscales, and the extrinsic goal score is the sum of scores for the financial success, social recognition, and appealing appearance subscales. The AI-R has been shown to be reliable and to possess strong internal consistency [21, 37]. The Chinese version of the AI-R has also exhibited high degrees of reliability and validity [38]. In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha value for the AI-R was 0.92.

Stress

Stress were evaluated using the abbreviated 30-item version of the GASHSS [27]. This instrument assesses general, academic, and social hassles. For each item, participants are asked to rate the frequency and duration of the hassle in the last 7 days (0 = no or not at all persistent hassle, 6 = extremely persistent or high-frequency and/or long duration hassle). Hassle scores range from 0 to 180, with higher scores reflecting a greater frequency of hassles. The Chinese version of the GASHSS has demonstrated strong internal consistency [39]. In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha values for the GASHSS ranged from 0.93 to 0.96 across administrations.

Statistical analyses

Analyses were carried out using the SAS (version 9.0; SAS Institute, Cary, NC) MIXED procedure and maximum likelihood estimation. The dependent variable was within-subject fluctuations in depressive symptoms during the follow-up period, as predicted by goal-orientation style (intrinsic vs. extrinsic) and fluctuations in hassles. As goal-orientation style scores were between-subject predictors, they were standardized prior to analyses. As hassle was a within-subject predictor, measures were centered at each participant’s mean prior to analyses. Separate analyses were conducted for intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Preliminary analyses indicated that no reported association was moderated by age or gender; analyses are thus presented for the sample as a whole.

When fitting hierarchical linear models, appropriate mean and covariance structures must be specified. Covariance structures are commonly used in studies in which multiple responses are obtained from the same individual over time and include compound symmetry, first-order auto-regressive, heterogeneous autoregressive, and banded Toeplitz structures. To select a covariance structure for our analyses, we fitted the models utilizing each structure and chose the best fit based on standard and corrected Akaike information criteria and the Schwarz Bayesian information criterion. In all cases, the best fit was an autoregressive heterogeneous (ARH) structure.

After choosing the appropriate covariance structure, we next examined the random-effects component of our model. According to Diggle, Heagerty, Liang, and Zeger’s (2002) recommendation to use a “saturated” model for mean structure while searching for an appropriate covariance structure, we chose a mean structure that included intrinsic goals, hassles, and intrinsic goals × hassles interaction. Four additional effects were also included in this initial mean structure. First, as levels of depressive symptom likely differ among participants experiencing average individual levels of hassles, a random intercept was included in the model. Second, given that depressive symptoms are a within-subject predictor whose effect is expected to vary among participants, a random effect for slope was included in the model. Third, to control for individual differences in baseline levels of depressive symptoms, participants’ initial depressive symptoms were included in the model. Finally, given that hassle is a within-subject predictor, a random slope for hassles was also inserted into the model.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Means and standard deviations of depressive symptom and hassle measures obtained at T1–T5 are presented in Table 1. Although CES-D scores were higher at the T3 than at other time points, assessment time point had no significant main effect on CES-D scores [F(4,2146) = .61, p = .656]. Assessment time point had a significant main effect on GASHSS scores [F(4,2141) = 30.13, p < .001], which were significantly higher at the initial assessment [Student-Newman-Keuls (SNK) tests, p < .001)], and significantly lower at the follow-up 4 assessment (SNK test, p < .05) than at other time points.

Table 1 Observed means and standard deviations of depressive symptoms and hassles

Pearson correlations between all initial measures are presented in Table 2. Higher levels of extrinsic goals were associated with higher levels of depressive symptoms and more frequent occurrence of hassles, whereas no significant correlation was observed between these factors and intrinsic goals.

Table 2 Intercorrelations amongst initial CES-D, GSAHS and AI

Intrinsic goals predicting depressive symptoms

When the random-effects component of the “saturated” model was examined, non-significant random effects were removed from the model prior to examination of the fixed-effects component. The ARH parameter (r = 2.46, p < .05), random intercept (r = 8.80, p < .001), and random slope (r = 9.81, p < .001) were significant and thus retained in the model. Table 3 shows covariance parameter estimates for the final model. The fixed-effects component of the model showed no significant main effect of intrinsic goals [b = –.048, standard error (SE) = .246, F(1,458) = –.20, p = .845] and significant two-way, cross-level interaction between intrinsic goals and stress [b = –.022, SE = .010, F(1,1175) = –2.30, p < .05].

Table 3 Estimates of the fixed effects component of the simultaneous model: hassles* intrinsic goals → depressive symptoms

The fixed effects summarized in Table 3 were used to calculate predicted depressive symptoms for participants with high and low levels of intrinsic goals (±1.5 × mean between-subject standard deviation). Participants with low levels of intrinsic goals reported higher levels of depressive symptoms when experiencing high levels of hassles than when experiencing low levels of hassles [b = .076, SE = .018, t(1175) = 4.17, p < .001; Fig. 1]. However, levels of depressive symptoms did not vary as a function of high levels of intrinsic goals [b = .010, SE = .017, t(1175) = .60, p = .546). The slope of the relationship between hassles and depressive symptoms was significantly greater in participants with low than in those with high levels of intrinsic goals [b = –.066, SE = .029, t(1175) = –2.30, p < .05].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Intrinsic goals and stress as predictors of depressive symptoms

Extrinsic goals predicting depressive symptoms

Similar analyses were conducted to examine whether students with extrinsic goals exhibited greater elevations in depressive symptoms following increases in hassles. The ARH parameter(r = 2.31, p < .05), random intercept (r = 8.78, p < .001), and random slope (r = 9.69, p < .001) were significant and thus retained in the model. In the first model, we estimated the effects of extrinsic goals on the relationship between hassles and depressive symptoms. Extrinsic goals exhibited no significant main effect on depressive symptoms [b = .231, SE = .247, F(1,460) = .93, p = .351] and no significant cross-level interaction between extrinsic goals and hassles was observed [b = –.006, SE = .010, F(1,1179) = –.57, p = .571].

Discussion

In this study, we focused on the moderating roles that intrinsic and extrinsic goals can have on hassles and depressive symptom development among undergraduate students using a five-wave longitudinal design with 3-monthly assessments. First, in line with a previous study [40] in which stress was found to be highest at the initial assessment, our findings indicate that the level of hassles was highest at the time of the first assessment and declined gradually over time. These results likely reflect practice effects due to repeated administration of measures. Additionally, it likely reflects the major transition of beginning college. Stress and depression levels may decrease as students acclimate to college life.

Second, consistent with our hypotheses, lower levels of intrinsic goals were associated with greater increases in depressive symptoms following the occurrence of hassles. The finding that intrinsic goals might protect undergraduate students from depressive symptoms in the presence of hassles is consistent with Dykman’s conceptualization of the association between intrinsic goals and depressive symptoms within a diathesis–stress framework [15]. Kuroda and Sakurai obtained similar results indicating that social learning goals reduced the effects of interpersonal stress, thereby protecting against depression [25]. Sideridis argued that mastery-oriented individuals’ (those with intrinsic goals) engagement in a task arises from inherent needs for growth, learning, and improvement and is a consequence of secure attachment [41]. Undergraduate students pursuing these goals are thus more likely to show adaptive cognitive, affective, and behavioral patterns in the face of all kinds of hassle. Even in the case of failure, intrinsic goal–oriented undergraduate students appraise a given situation as an opportunity to learn and grow from their mistakes. In other words, they are motivated to interpret negative events as opportunities for self-growth. Such functioning buffers against the adverse effects of hassles, thereby aiding in the protection against the onsets of depressive symptoms. Fostering the development of intrinsic goals in prevention and intervention programs may, ultimately, help to prevent the onset and maintenance of depressive disorders.

Finally, contrary to our hypothesis, undergraduate students with higher levels of extrinsic goals did not report greater increases in depressive symptoms following the occurrence of hassles relative to those with low extrinsic goals. Researchers found that social performance-avoidance goals, a specific form of extrinsic goal, exacerbated the effects of interpersonal stress, thereby leading to the development of depression; however, they also found that social performance-approach goals (another form of extrinsic goal) did not have the same effect [25]. For undergraduate students pursuing extrinsic goals such as fame, attractiveness, and wealth, the occurrence of hassles implies that they have failed to attain their goals. They are thus threatened by such hassles. However, extrinsic goals may have different meanings for Chinese and Western undergraduate students. Previous researchers examining the life goals of undergraduate students in 15 countries found that intrinsic and extrinsic life goals opposed each other, but that extrinsic goals were grouped more with safety goals in poorer compared with wealthier countries, indicating that they have different meanings for individuals in these countries [42]. This finding has been confirmed by other researchers, who have found no clear relationship between wealth and happiness once basic human needs have been met [43]. Thus, because Chinese undergraduate students pursuing extrinsic goals focus on potential positive social outcomes (i.e., positive evaluations from others; valence hypothesis), they are more likely to show adaptive cognitive, affective, and behavioral patterns after the occurrence of hassles. For example, they are motivated not to focus on past negative evaluations, but to anticipate positive evaluations from others. Furthermore, they are more likely to utilize active and relatively adaptive coping strategies to deal with hassles in order to attain positive social outcomes in the future. Overall, extrinsic goals play both negative and positive roles in the face of hassles, which might explain why depressive symptoms among adolescents cannot be predicted on the basis of interactions between extrinsic goals and hassles.

Collectively, these findings advance our knowledge on the roles of intrinsic and extrinsic goals in the development of depressive symptoms following the occurrence of hassles. More specifically, the occurrence of negative events was associated with increases in depressive symptoms, but only among those with low levels of intrinsic goals. This result suggests that high levels of intrinsic goals might buffer against the deleterious effect of hassles on depression. Such findings highlight the importance of intrinsic goal orientation for the prevention and treatment of depression [44]. Intrinsic goal–oriented approaches to intervention focus on the cultivation of intrinsic goals such as self-acceptance, affiliation, community feeling, and physical fitness. Such interventions are likely to increase the quality and degree of goal orientation, thereby providing individuals with better resources to cope with the stressors they face and consequently avoid depression.

The present study implemented several empirical and methodological improvements over previous studies of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. First, it was the first study to examine the roles of intrinsic and extrinsic aspirations among Chinese undergraduate students using a diathesis–stress framework. Second, the study utilized a multi-wave design with repeated measures of stressors and psychological symptoms. In contrast to many cross-sectional studies of goal orientation, the use of a longitudinal design enabled the examination of temporal change in symptoms of depression and idiographic analysis of the diathesis–stress hypotheses. Third, this study extends knowledge on diathesis–stress models of depression in Chinese undergraduate students.

Several limitations of the study should be noted to provide direction for future research. First, the study utilized self-reported measures, which are prone to response biases and have diagnostic limitations. Future research should continue to explore how aspirations shape subsequent psychopathology by utilizing more sophisticated assessment techniques, such as peer and parent ratings, semi-structured diagnostic interviews, and direct behavioral observation, to examine these constructs. Second, the homogeneity of the sample of undergraduate students may limit the generalizability of our results to community or clinical populations. Additional research is warranted to determine whether the present results can be replicated in such populations. Third, as stress is one of the most robust predictors of psychopathology [45], other types of stressors are also likely to shape depressive symptoms. Further, the current study did not identify specific negative life events that may have exhibited particularly robust associations with depressive symptoms. Thus, future research is warranted to examine whether different types of stressors differentially predict psychopathology and whether specific life events may be differentially predictive of depression.

Conclusions

The results of the current study provide support for the hypothesis that intrinsic goals serve as a protective factor in the development of depressive symptoms in a sample of Chinese undergraduate students. The study also provides further support for the applicability of the diathesis–stress model of depression to Chinese undergraduate students, suggesting that diathesis–stress processes predicting depressive symptoms may be universal, rather than culturally specific. In summary, intrinsic goals may protect Chinese individuals against the development of depression; thus, fostering the development of intrinsic goals in prevention and intervention programs may, ultimately, help to prevent the onset and maintenance of depressive disorders.

Ethics

This study has been approved by the ethics committee of South China Normal University.

Consent to participate

Each subject has signed the written informed consent before participation.

Consent to publish

All subjects provided written consent for the publications of anonymized individual data.

Availability of data and materials

In accordance with subjects’ informed consent, data and materials supporting our findings in the manuscript will not be shared.

Abbreviations

AI:

Aspiration Index

AI-R:

the revised Aspiration Index

ARH:

autoregressive heterogeneous

CES-D:

Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale

GASHSS:

the General, Academic, and Social Hassles Scale for Students

GS:

growth seeking

SD:

standard deviation

SE:

standard error

SNK:

Student-Newman-Keuls

VS:

validation seeking

References

  1. Othieno CJ, Okoth RO, Peltzer K, Pengpid S, Malla LO. Depression among university students in Kenya: Prevalence and sociodemographic correlates. J Affect Disord. 2014;165:120–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Vázquez FL, Blanco V. Prevalence of DSM-IV major depression among Spanish university students. J Am Coll Heal. 2008;57(2):165–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Tomoda A, Mori K, Kimura M, Takahashi T, Kitamura T. One‐year prevalence and incidence of depression among first‐year university students in Japan: A preliminary study. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2000;54(5):583–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Xu Y, Chi X, Chen S, Qi J, Zhang P, Yang Y. Prevalence and correlates of depression among college nursing students in China. Nurse Educ Today. 2014;34(6):e7–e12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Richards D, Salamanca SA. Point-prevalence of depression and associated risk factors. J Psychol. 2014;148(3):305–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Chen L, Wang L, Qiu XH, Yang XX, Qiao ZX, Yang YJ, et al. Depression among Chinese university students: prevalence and socio-demographic correlates. PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e58379.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Lamis DA, Saito M, Osman A, Klibert J, Malone PS, Langhinrichsen-Rohling J. Hopelessness and suicide proneness in US and Japanese college students depressive symptoms as a potential mediator. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2014;45(5):805–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Adams TB, Wharton CM, Quilter L, Hirsch T. The association between mental health and acute infectious illness among a national sample of 18-to 24-year-old college students. J Am Coll Heal. 2008;56(6):657–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Eisenberg D, Golberstein E, Hunt JB. Mental health and academic success in college. BE J Econ Anal Policy. 2009;9(1).

  10. Blanco V, Rohde P, Vázquez FL, Otero P. Predictors of prevention failure in college students participating in Two indicated depression prevention programs. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2014;11(4):3803–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Liu RT, Miller I. Life events and suicidal ideation and behavior: A systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2014;34(3):181–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Hankin BL, Abela JR. Development of psychopathology: A vulnerability-stress perspective. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2005.

  13. Yi J, Zhu X, Auerbach RP, Gardiner CK, Cai L, Wang Y, et al. Insecure attachment as a predictor of depressive and anxious symptomology. Depress Anxiety. 2012;29(9):789–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Abela JR, Aydin C, Auerbach RP. Operationalizing the “vulnerability” and “stress” components of the hopelessness theory of depression: A multi-wave longitudinal study. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44(11):1565–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Dykman BM. Integrating cognitive and motivational factors in depression: initial tests of a goal-orientation approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74(1):139.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Dweck CS. Motivational processes affecting learning. Am Psychol. 1986;41(10):1040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Elliott ES, Dweck CS. Goals: an approach to motivation and achievement. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(1):5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Deci EL, Ryan RM. The“ what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol Inq. 2000;11(4):227–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Kasser T. Sketches for a self-determination theory of values. Handbook of self-determination research. 2002. p. 123–40.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am Psychol. 2000;55(1):68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Kasser T, Ryan RM. Further examining the American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 1996;22(3):280–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Carver CS, Baird E. The American dream revisited: Is it what you want or why you want it that matters? Psychol Sci. 1998;9(4):289–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Lekes N, Gingras I, Philippe FL, Koestner R, Fang J. Parental autonomy-support, intrinsic life goals, and well-being among adolescents in China and North America. J Youth Adolesc. 2010;39(8):858–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Sebire SJ, Standage M, Vansteenkiste M. Examining intrinsic versus extrinsic exercise goals: Cognitive, affective, and behavioral outcomes. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2009;31(2):189–210.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Kuroda Y, Sakurai S. Social goal orientations, interpersonal stress, and depressive symptoms among early adolescents in Japan: a test of the diathesis-stress model using the trichotomous framework of social goal orientations. The journal of early adolescence. 2011;31(2):300–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1(3):385–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Dunkley DM, Blankstein KR, Halsall J, Williams M, Winkworth G. The relation between perfectionism and distress: Hassles, coping, and perceived social support as mediators and moderators. J Counsel Psycho. 2000;47(4):437–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Essau CA, Olaya B, Pasha G, Gilvarry C, Bray D. Depressive symptoms among children and adolescents in Iran: A confirmatory factor analytic study of the Centre for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale for Children. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2013;44(1):123–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Zhang J, Sun W, Kong Y, Wang C. Reliability and validity of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale in 2 special adult samples from rural China. Compr Psychiatry. 2012;53(8):1243–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Zhang B, Fokkema M, Cuijpers P, Li J, Smits N, Beekman A. Measurement invariance of the center for epidemiological studies depression scale (CES-D) among chinese and dutch elderly. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2011;11(1):74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Dozeman E, van Schaik DJ, van Marwijk HW, Stek ML, van der Horst HE, Beekman AT. The center for epidemiological studies depression scale (CES‐D) is an adequate screening instrument for depressive and anxiety disorders in a very old population living in residential homes. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;26(3):239–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Weissman MM, Sholomskas D, Pottenger M, Prusoff BA, Locke BZ. Assessing depressive symptoms in five psychiatric populations: a validation study. Am J Epidemiol. 1977;106(3):203–14.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Morin A, Moullec G, Maïano C, Layet L, Just J-L, Ninot G. Psychometric properties of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) in French clinical and nonclinical adults. Rev Epidemiol Sante Publique. 2011;59(5):327–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Choi SW, Schalet B, Cook KF, Cella D. Establishing a common metric for depressive symptoms: Linking the BDI-II, CES-D, and PHQ-9 to PROMIS depression. 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Yang H-J, Soong W-T, Kuo P-H, Chang H-L, Chen WJ. Using the CES-D in a two-phase survey for depressive disorders among nonreferred adolescents in Taipei: a stratum-specific likelihood ratio analysis. J Affect Disord. 2004;82(3):419–30.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Niemiec CP, Ryan RM, Deci EL, Williams GC. Aspiring to physical health: The role of aspirations for physical health in facilitating long-term tobacco abstinence. Patient Educ Couns. 2009;74(2):250–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Kasser T, Ryan RM. Be careful what you wish for: Optimal functioning and the relative attainment of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Auerbach RP, Webb CA, Schreck M, McWhinnie CM, Ho M-HR, Zhu X, et al. Examining the pathway through which intrinsic and extrinsic aspirations generate stress and subsequent depressive symptoms. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2011;30(8):856–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Guo R, Xiao J, Yang X, Zhu X, Yao S. Chinese version of the General Social and Academic Hassles Scale: reliability and validity. Chin J Health Psychol. 2009;7:876–9.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Yang J, Yao S, Zhu X, Zhang C, Ling Y, Abela JR, et al. The impact of stress on depressive symptoms is moderated by social support in Chinese adolescents with subthreshold depression: A multi-wave longitudinal study. J Affect Disord. 2010;127(1):113–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Sideridis GD. Goal orientation, academic achievement, and depression: Evidence in favor of a revised goal theory framework. J Educ Psychol. 2005;97(3):366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Grouzet FM, Kasser T, Ahuvia A, Dols JMF, Kim Y, Lau S, et al. The structure of goal contents across 15 cultures. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;89(5):800.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Diener E, Biswas-Diener R. Will money increase subjective well-being? Soc Indic Res. 2002;57(2):119–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Young JF, Mufson L. Interpersonal psychotherapy for treatment and prevention of adolescent depression. Handbook of depression in children and adolescents. 2008. p. 288–306.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Grant KE, Compas BE, Thurm AE, McMahon SD, Gipson PY. Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Measurement issues and prospective effects. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2004;33(2):412–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Dr. Jinyao Yi for her great help in study design and analysis of data.

Funding

This study was funded by the National Natural Science Foundations of China (No. 81371487).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mingtian Zhong.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing of interests.

Authors’ contributions

YL and MZ were involved in the design of the study. YH, YW, WC and QZ were responsible for data collection and analyses. YL wrote the manuscript and MZ revised the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ling, Y., He, Y., Wei, Y. et al. Intrinsic and extrinsic goals as moderators of stress and depressive symptoms in Chinese undergraduate students: A multi-wave longitudinal study. BMC Psychiatry 16, 138 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-016-0842-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-016-0842-5

Keywords