Skip to main content

Detecting subtle signs of depression with automated speech analysis in a non-clinical sample

Abstract

Background

Automated speech analysis has gained increasing attention to help diagnosing depression. Most previous studies, however, focused on comparing speech in patients with major depressive disorder to that in healthy volunteers. An alternative may be to associate speech with depressive symptoms in a non-clinical sample as this may help to find early and sensitive markers in those at risk of depression.

Methods

We included n = 118 healthy young adults (mean age: 23.5 ± 3.7 years; 77% women) and asked them to talk about a positive and a negative event in their life. Then, we assessed the level of depressive symptoms with a self-report questionnaire, with scores ranging from 0–60. We transcribed speech data and extracted acoustic as well as linguistic features. Then, we tested whether individuals below or above the cut-off of clinically relevant depressive symptoms differed in speech features. Next, we predicted whether someone would be below or above that cut-off as well as the individual scores on the depression questionnaire. Since depression is associated with cognitive slowing or attentional deficits, we finally correlated depression scores with performance in the Trail Making Test.

Results

In our sample, n = 93 individuals scored below and n = 25 scored above cut-off for clinically relevant depressive symptoms. Most speech features did not differ significantly between both groups, but individuals above cut-off spoke more than those below that cut-off in the positive and the negative story. In addition, higher depression scores in that group were associated with slower completion time of the Trail Making Test. We were able to predict with 93% accuracy who would be below or above cut-off. In addition, we were able to predict the individual depression scores with low mean absolute error (3.90), with best performance achieved by a support vector machine.

Conclusions

Our results indicate that even in a sample without a clinical diagnosis of depression, changes in speech relate to higher depression scores. This should be investigated in more detail in the future. In a longitudinal study, it may be tested whether speech features found in our study represent early and sensitive markers for subsequent depression in individuals at risk.

Peer Review reports

Background

Major depression is a rising mental health concern, affecting more than 264 million people worldwide [1]. Detecting depressive symptoms as early as possible becomes increasingly important in younger adults as mental health concerns are rising in this population, particularly since the COVID-19 pandemic [2]. The analysis of speech offers a promising avenue to objectively identify signs of depression as the cognitive and behavioural changes associated with depression influence the production as well as the quality of speech [3, 4]. Patients with depression typically present with decreased or less fluent speech, with diminished prosody, or with monotonous speech [3, 5]. It is, therefore, not surprising that clinicians either consciously or unconsciously watch for signs of changed speech during the diagnostic process of this disorder. The subjective evaluation of a change in speech, however, is open to bias and requires a large degree of clinical training to produce reliable results. In addition, subtle changes may remain undetected. Thus, clinicians would greatly benefit from objective measures to identify impairment in speech control or the acoustic quality of the speech produced. During recent years, automated analysis of speech has gained increasing attention and paralinguistic features have been investigated in particular [6, 7]. These features convey or modify meaning beyond the words and grammar used and include pitch, prosody, volume, or intonation (among others) and can be considered a key behavioural marker of depression [3]. Early studies investigating paralinguistic features found that patients with depression consistently demonstrated prosodic speech abnormalities such as reduced pitch, reduced pitch range, slower speaking rate and articulation errors [8,9,10]. Others identified decreased intonation, slow articulation and vocal monotony in patients with depression [3]. A recent review on studies using automated speech analysis added that changes in fundamental frequency, jitter, and shimmer have been repeatedly reported to be associated with depression [7]. Automated speech analysis, therefore, has made it possible to study and compare speech features on even finer scales. Most of the studies so far, however, focused on discovering speech features associated with depressive symptoms by comparing healthy volunteers to patients with major depression. In order to find early and sensitive markers for possible preventive strategies, however, it may be useful to also consider a ‘healthier’ population. In addition, a timely detection of psychological distress is particularly crucial in younger adults since a depressive episode will follow if no support is provided [11]. The objective of this study was, therefore, to test whether there would be an association between subtle signs of depression and speech features in a non-clinical population (i.e., in healthy young adults). Since the major cognitive concerns in depression are deficits in controlling attention due to rumination, cognitive slowing, and problems with executive control [12, 13], we also tested whether subtle signs of depression would relate to attentional deficits, cognitive slowing, or executive function deficits.

Materials and methods

Participants

We initially recruited n = 164 healthy young University students (20–30 years of age, 79% women) from the University of Saarbrücken or the University of Koblenz-Landau (both in Germany). The study procedures were similar at both universities. All participants gave written informed consent prior to study participation. All experiments were done in accordance with the ethical standards of the Universities of Koblenz-Landau and Saarbrücken.

Study procedure

The participants completed the Trail Making test (see below) on a tablet in a lab environment at the University of Saarbrücken or the University of Koblenz-Landau. Afterwards, the participants were asked to talk about a negative and a positive event in their life. We standardized the recording procedure by using an iOS-based iPad Pro A1584, with a 12.9’ display and a resolution of 2732 × 2048 pixels. The procedure always took place in the same room with identical settings: Noise-cancelled surrounding, standard-lightings, iPad lying on the table (in near visual acuity distance; i.e., 35–40 cm) and was operated by the (diagonally opposite sitting) examiner. Finally, we examined symptoms of depression with a self-report questionnaire (see below). All participants received course credit for their participation.

Trail making test (A and B)

With the Trail Making Test [14] A and B, we assessed speed of information processing, focused or divided attention, and switching abilities. For the Trail Making Test A, the participants were asked to connect randomly positioned numbered circles (1–25) in ascending order as quickly as possible. For the Trail Making Test B the participants needed to connect circles that included numbers (1–13) or letters (A-L) in numeric and alphabetic order as quickly as possible, alternating between numbers and letters. We used time to completion for statistical analysis.

Depression scale

For the assessment of depressive symptoms, we used a reliable and valid questionnaire (‘Allgemeine Depressionsskala’), which assesses disturbances caused by depressive symptoms during the last week [15]. For the total score (range between 0–60), the frequency of motivational, emotional, somatic, or cognitive symptoms (20 items in total) had to be rated on a scale ranging from 0 (never or seldom) to 4 (mostly or all the time). Based on the total score, participants were classified as either above (score ≥ 22) or below (score < 22) the cut-off for clinically relevant depressive symptoms [15].

Speech task (positive and negative story)

Free and natural speech tasks are capable of eliciting emotional reactions (or a lack thereof) by asking to describe events that triggered recent affective arousal. To this end, and based on our previous research [16, 17], the participants were asked to talk about a positive and a negative event in their life. Instructions for the speech tasks (‘Can you tell me in one minute about a positive/negative event in your life?’) were pre-recorded by psychologists and played to the participants from a tablet ensuring standardized instructions. The answers were recorded with the tablet’s microphone.

Processing of speech data

Recordings of the positive and negative story were transcribed by two Psychology students (M. Sc.) according to the CHAT protocol [18]. Audio features were automatically extracted from the audio signal. Textual features were automatically extracted from the manual transcripts. We used the proprietary speech processing engine SIGMA [19] to extract textual and audio features. Features were extracted separately for positive and negative stories. These include acoustic features with segmental features (e.g., the number or length of words or pauses or the number or length of speaking segments), supra-segmental or prosodic features based on the processing of the frequency spectrum (e.g., pitch, intensity), and textual features. Acoustic features were calculated only for the parts when the participants told the positive/negative story. Feature extraction was similar as in our previous studies [16, 17]. Different software packages were utilized to calculate the features. The computation of fundamental frequency related features relied on the parselmouth package [20] (which is a python wrapper for praat; [21]). The pitches were computed with the standard value (i.e., zero) for time step, 50 Hz for pitch floor, and 250 Hz for pitch ceiling. The WebRTC Voice Activity Detector (py-webrtcvad; https://github.com/wiseman/py-webrtcvad) package was used to estimate pause and speech ratio. A frame size of 10 ms was used to partition the audio data before marking them as pause or speech. A threshold of 1 s was used to determine whether consecutive frames form meaningful speech. Most of the linguistic features relied on the stanza package [22] with the default tokenization. The computation of the rates of parts of speech (POS) is straightforward with stanza's provided POS-tagging. Graph related features were calculated with the NetworkX package [23]. Semantic clusters were computed using transcript's word embedding (provided by the flair package [24]) by applying sklearn's affinity propagation. More precisely, word vectors of the tokens in the transcripts (without any processing like stemming) were computed using word embedding provided by the flair package. Then affinity propagation clustering on word vectors was computed with default Euclidean affinity and default damping of 0.5. The coherence metric was estimated following the work by Iter and colleagues [25]: the cosine similarity between consecutive sentences was computed and then the mean of the similarity value was taken. Sentiment related features were computed with the sentiment intensity analyser of the nltk package [26]. A list of all extracted features with an explanation of their meaning can be found in supplementary Tables S1 and S2. Since speech features vary naturally between men and women, we normalized features by gender (i.e., they were scaled by their minimum and maximum absolute value, by gender).

Statistical analysis

Our first aim was to test whether speech features differ between individuals below and above the cut-off of clinically relevant depressive symptoms. Since previous studies found that patients with depression used more words than healthy volunteers in written self-reports [27], we first compared the number of words in the positive and the negative story between groups, using multivariate analysis of variance with group as between-subject factor and the number of words as dependent variable. Next, we tested whether there were any differences in speech features between individuals below and above the cut-off using multivariate analysis of variance, with group (below or above cut-off) as between-subject factor and speech features as dependent variables, when controlling for the number of words in each story. Next, we performed logistic regression to test whether speech features can predict whether or not a person would score below or above the cut-off. We used binary logistic regression with backward selection using Wald statistics (probability of F for entry: p ≤ 0.05, probability of F for removal: p ≥ 0.10). Then, we predicted the individual depression scores using speech features and machine learning based regression models (i.e., support vector machine, extra trees, or random forest). These were independently trained on 322 extracted features (i.e., 161 features for each story, positive and negative). We used leave-one-out cross validation and grid search for hyper parameter tuning. We trained each of the three models (support vector machine, extra tree, random forest) 118 times and selected the best model based on the lowest Mean Absolute Error. In addition, we tested the significance of each model by comparing its’ performance to an extra trees model trained while permuting the true depression scores 1000 times. Next, we tested whether we would find significant differences in Trail Making Test performance between individuals below and above cut-off using multivariate ANOVA, with group as between-subject factor and performance as dependent variable. Finally, we correlated, in each group (i.e., below or above cut-off), the scores achieved in the depression score with time to complete the Trail Making Test (A and B), using Pearson correlation or Spearman correlation (to account for potential outliers).

We used SPSS (version 26.0; IBM Inc.; USA) and R (version 1.4.1106) for statistical analyses and GraphPad Prism (version 9.0.0; USA) for visualization of the results. Statistical significance levels were set to p < 0.05 (two-tailed). We adjusted for multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni-Holm method.

Results

We had to exclude 44 participants due to missing data (data was missing at random). We additionally excluded two participants whose answers on the depression questionnaire indicated a response bias. One participant was classified as an age outlier (> 3 SD above mean) and was therefore also excluded. Thus, a total of n = 118 was included in all statistical analyses, of whom n = 93 were below the cut-off of relevant depressive symptoms and n = 25 were above that cut-off. We did not observe significant differences regarding age, sex-ratio, or education between these two groups (Table 1). In neither of the two groups, we found a significant difference between males and females in the depression scores.

Table 1 Demographics of the sample when divided into participants above cut-off for relevant depressive symptoms and below that cut-off. We used Χ2 (sex) or ANOVA (all other variables) to test for significant differences between both groups

When comparing the number of words in the positive or negative story between both groups, we found that individuals in the above cut-off group spoke more than individuals below cut-off (F(2, 115) = 3.71, p = 0.028) in both the positive (F(1, 116) = 6.94, p = 0.01) and the negative story (F(1, 116) = 5.83, p = 0.02) (Fig. 1). When comparing other speech features between groups, we did not find any significant difference for features obtained from the positive story F(1, 115) = 1.13, p = 0.65) or the negative story F(1, 115) = 57.29, p = 0.11), when controlling for the number of words in each story. When predicting whether a person would be below or above the cut-off of clinically relevant depressive symptoms using logistic regression (backward method), we found that the logistic regression model was statistically significant χ2 (18) = 25.26, p < 0.0001. The model explained 78.4% of the variance (Nagelkerke R2) and correctly classified 93.2% of cases (Table 2).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Number of words in a positive (blue) or negative (red) story in participants that were either below or above the cut-off of clinically relevant depressive symptoms

Table 2 Classification table for the results of the binary logistic regression model. (i.e., prediction whether a person would be below or above cut-off for clinically meaningful depressive symptoms)

The variables significantly contributing to the prediction model are presented in Table 3. When predicting scores in the depression questionnaire, best performance was achieved with a support vector machine (mean absolute error = 3.90) which was significantly better than the prediction of the random models (mean absolute error = 4.43, standard deviation = 0.17; p < 0.05).

Table 3 Variables that significantly contributed to explained variance in the binary logistic regression model (i.e., prediction whether a person would be below or above cut-off for clinically relevant depressive symptoms)

Finally, we tested whether there were any significant differences in Trail Making Test performance and whether performance correlated significantly with scores of depressive symptoms in each group. We found no significant difference between groups for Trail Making Test performance (F(2, 115) = 0.78, p = 0.46). However, in the above cut-off group, those with higher depression scores were slower in performing the Trail Making Test A, as indicated by a significant positive correlation between performance and the depression score (r(25) = 0.52, p = 0.008; Fig. 2). We neither found a significant correlation in the group below cut-off (r(93) = -0.02, p = 0.87; Fig. 2) nor with performance in the Trail Making Test B (above cut-off: r(25) = 0.28, p = 0.18; below cut-off: r(93) = 0.18, p = 0.07). The results were similar when using non-parametric correlations (i.e., Spearman rank correlations).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Correlation between the time it took to perform the Trail Making Test (part A) and scores in a depressive symptoms questionnaire in participants above or below cut-off of clinically relevant depressive symptoms

Discussion

In this study, young University students talked about a positive and a negative event in their life and we tested whether speech features in either story would be associated with the amount of depressive symptoms. We first classified participants as either above or below cut-off of clinically relevant depressive symptoms and tested whether individuals above or below cut-off would differ in the number of words used in either story. This was the case as participants above cut-off spoke significantly more than participants below cut-off in both the positive and the negative story (Fig. 1). This is contrary to previous findings in patients with manifest depression, which tended to speak less than healthy volunteers [5]. Our sample, however, was not considered pathological (i.e., clinically depressed) and thus, clinical signs of depression may have been too subtle to show speech patterns typically associated with manifest depression. This is supported by the mean score in the above cut-off group which was 26.5 (± 4.7). Since the cut-off for meaningful depressive symptoms is 22, the participants in the above cut-off group seem only mildly affected. An alternative explanation may be that along the dimensionality of depressive symptoms (i.e., from subtle signs to manifest depression), the number of words used to describe or tell something may change from very much to very little. This is supported by studies that included patients with mild symptoms of depression, who also used more words than healthy volunteers in written self-reports or in social media posts [27, 28]. It could be that the increase in the number of words in our participants, as well as in patients with mild depression, indicate rumination or ‘mind-wandering’, reflected by an increase in verbalized thought production [29]. Rumination is a predisposing factor contributing to an increased risk for developing major depression [30]. Therefore, it could be that our participants above the cut-off are at risk for major depression in the future. Interestingly, participants above cut-off appeared to produce more words in the negative story than in the positive story (Fig. 1, non-significant finding), which is in line with previous studies in patients with manifest depression [31]. Our findings therefore indicate that even in a non-clinical sample, those with symptoms of depression produce more words when a negative cue is given. Another possible explanation may be that the increase in word count indicates loosening of associations. By increasing the number of words, the individuals may compensate for distraction, cognitive slowing or even for hesitancy.

We did not find any other significant differences in speech features between both groups, which may be due to the rather artificial nature of the task not allowing for spontaneous speech to be affected. However, other speech features were relevant for improving prediction accuracy. We were able to predict whether a person would be below or above cut-off with very high accuracy (i.e., 93%). The features that significantly contributed to explained variance in the binary logistic regression model were previously found to be associated with depressive symptoms (e.g., temporal (speech ratio), spectral (MFCC), and prosodic (F0) features; see Table 3) [5, 32,33,34]. Our results therefore support existing findings [35] and indicate that even in a non-clinical sample, similar speech patterns may be important predictors of depressive symptoms. We similarly achieved good performance (i.e., low mean error) when predicting the actual score in the depression questionnaire. We used random forest regression, which has been used previously to predict depression scores based on speech patterns [36, 37]. These findings may contribute to an early identification of people at risk of developing depression and may allow timely preventive measures. This would, however, require a longitudinal study design in a future study.

Finally, we tested whether we would find significant differences in the Trail Making Test between the above cut-off and the below cut-off group. In addition, we correlated depression scores with the time it took to complete the Trail Making Test. We did not find significant differences between both groups regarding performance but there was a strong positive correlation between time to complete part A of the test and depression scores in the above cut-off group (Fig. 2). Here, individuals with higher depression scores were slower in completing the test. This indicates that those with higher depression scores had deficits in controlling attention or, at least, a reduction in information processing speed. Interestingly, we did not find a correlation between depression scores and time to complete the Trail Making Test B, a measure for attention and executive functions. In manifest depression, both attentional deficits and problems with executive functions are common [38,39,40]. Comparable to a change in the number of words along the dimensionality of depressive symptoms, also cognitive deficits seem to change along this dimensionality, with attentional deficits appearing very early and problems with executive functions emerging only with manifest depression. This may indicate that attentional deficits are among the first to be associated with symptoms of depression and once depression is clinically manifest, executive functions deficits follow.

Interestingly, in our non-clinical sample, attentional deficits or slower perceptual processing were related to the amount of depressive symptoms, while others reported that attentional deficits in patients with manifest depression were related to illness duration [38]. This may suggest that, once depression becomes manifest, attentional deficits are more related to the accumulating burden of illness than to the severity of the current episode. This, again, points to a qualitative change between depressive symptoms and manifest depression, reflected by diverging speech patterns as well as cognitive deficits.

Our study may have several limitations. First, the speech recordings were relatively short and may have been not long enough for an identification of salient speech features associated with signs of depression. Future studies may consider longer natural speech tasks, particularly in non-clinical samples to test whether the patterns we identify in this study can be replicated. Second, our study sample consisted only of University students which are not representative for the general population. Thus, our results may have been different if we had included participants with different educational backgrounds. Third, we did not do a clinical interview and therefore, we cannot rule out a clinical diagnosis of depression in any of our participants. However, the mean score in those above cut-off was only slightly higher than the cut-off of 22. Therefore, it seems very unlikely that any of our participants suffered from a major depressive episode.

Conclusions

Taken together, our study adds to the current literature that speech features are sensitive for the detection of depressive symptoms even in a non-clinical sample. In a future longitudinal study, it may be tested whether these are early and sensitive features in individuals at risk of developing depression.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. World Health Organisation. Depression and Other Common Mental Disorders. 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Son C, Hegde S, Smith A, Wang X, Sasangohar F. Effects of COVID-19 on College Students’ Mental Health in the United States: Interview Survey Study. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22:e21279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Cummins N, Scherer S, Krajewski J, Schnieder S, Epps J, Quatieri TF. A review of depression and suicide risk assessment using speech analysis. Speech Commun. 2015;71:10–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Koops S, Brederoo SG, de Boer JN, Nadema FG, Voppel AE, Sommer IE. Speech as a Biomarker for Depression. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets. 2021. https://doi.org/10.2174/1871527320666211213125847

  5. Mundt JC, Vogel AP, Feltner DE, Lenderking WR. Vocal Acoustic Biomarkers of Depression Severity and Treatment Response. Biol Psychiat. 2012;72:580–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. He L, Cao C. Automated depression analysis using convolutional neural networks from speech. J Biomed Inform. 2018;83:103–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Low DM, Bentley KH, Ghosh SS. Automated assessment of psychiatric disorders using speech: A systematic review. Laryngoscope Investig Otolaryngol. 2020;5:96–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Darby JK, Hollien H. Vocal and speech patterns of depressive patients. Folia Phoniatr (Basel). 1977;29:279–91.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Hardy P, Jouvent R, Widlöcher D. Speech pause time and the retardation rating scale for depression (ERD). J Affect Disord. 1984;6:123–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Nilsonne Å, Sundberg J, Ternström S, Askenfelt A. Measuring the rate of change of voice fundamental frequency in fluent speech during mental depression. J Acoust Soc Am. 1988;83:716–28.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Huckvale K, Venkatesh S, Christensen H. Toward clinical digital phenotyping: a timely opportunity to consider purpose, quality, and safety. npj Digit Med. 2019;2:88.

  12. Gotlib IH, Joormann J. Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:285–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Rock PL, Roiser JP, Riedel WJ, Blackwell AD. Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2014;44:2029–40.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. REITAN RM. The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. J Consult Psychol. 1955;19:393–4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Hautzinger M, Bailer M, Hofmeister D, Keller F. Allgemeine Depressionsskala (ADS). Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  16. König A, Linz N, Zeghari R, Klinge X, Tröger J, Alexandersson J, et al. Detecting Apathy in Older Adults with Cognitive Disorders Using Automatic Speech Analysis. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;69:1183–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. König A, Mallick E, Tröger J, Linz N, Zeghari R, Manera V, et al. Measuring neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with early cognitive decline using speech analysis. Eur Psychiatry. 2021;64:e64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. MacWhinney B. The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk. Child Language Teaching and Therapy. 1992;8:217–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. ki elements. SIGMA. 2021.

  20. Jadoul Y, Thompson B, de Boer B. Introducing Parselmouth: A Python interface to Praat. J Phon. 2018;71:1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Boersma P, Weenink D. PRAAT, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International. 2001;5:341–5.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Qi P, Zhang Y, Zhang Y, Bolton J, Manning CD. Stanza: A Python Natural Language Processing Toolkit for Many Human Languages. 2020. https://doi.org/10.48550/ARXIV.2003.07082.

  23. Hagberg AA, Schult DA, Swart P. Exploring Network Structure, Dynamics, and Function using NetworkX. 2008.

  24. Akbik A, Blythe D, Vollgraf R. Contextual String Embeddings for Sequence Labeling. Proceedings of the 27th International Conference on Computational Linguistics. 2018;1638–49. https://aclanthology.org/C18-1139.

  25. Iter D, Yoon JH, Jurafsky D. Automatic Detection of Incoherent Speech for Diagnosing Schizophrenia. In: CLPsych@NAACL-HTL. 2018.

  26. Xue N. Steven Bird, Evan Klein and Edward Loper. Natural Language Processing with Python. O’Reilly Media, Inc.2009. ISBN: 978-0-596-51649-9. Nat Lang Eng. 2011;17:419–24

  27. Smirnova D, Cumming P, Sloeva E, Kuvshinova N, Romanov D, Nosachev G. Language Patterns Discriminate Mild Depression From Normal Sadness and Euthymic State. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Coppersmith G, Ngo K, Leary R, Wood A. Exploratory Analysis of Social Media Prior to a Suicide Attempt. In: Proceedings of the Third Workshop on Computational Lingusitics and Clinical Psychology. San Diego, CA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics; 2016. p. 106–17.

  29. Chaieb L, Hoppe C, Fell J. Mind wandering and depression: A status report. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2022;133:104505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Kertz SJ, Petersen DR, Stevens KT. Cognitive and attentional vulnerability to depression in youth: A review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2019;71:63–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Alison J, Burgess C. Effects of chronic non-clinical depression on the use of positive and negative words in language contexts. Brain Cogn. 2003;53:125–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Alghowinem SM, Gedeon T, Goecke R, Cohn J, Parker G. Interpretation of Depression Detection Models via Feature Selection Methods. IEEE Trans Affective Comput. 2020;:1–1.

  33. Horwitz R, Quatieri TF, Helfer BS, Yu B, Williamson JR, Mundt J. On the relative importance of vocal source, system, and prosody in human depression. In: 2013 IEEE International Conference on Body Sensor Networks. Cambridge, MA, USA: IEEE; 2013. p. 1–6.

  34. Yamamoto M, Takamiya A, Sawada K, Yoshimura M, Kitazawa M, Liang K, et al. Using speech recognition technology to investigate the association between timing-related speech features and depression severity. PLoS ONE. 2020;15:e0238726.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Cummins N, Epps J, Breakspear M, Goecke R. An investigation of depressed speech detection: features and normalization. 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Sun B, Zhang Y, He J, Yu L, Xu Q, Li D, et al. A Random Forest Regression Method With Selected-Text Feature For Depression Assessment. In: Proceedings of the 7th Annual Workshop on Audio/Visual Emotion Challenge. Mountain View California USA: ACM; 2017. p. 61–8.

  37. Tasnim M, Stroulia E. Detecting Depression from Voice. In: Meurs M-J, Rudzicz F, editors. Advances in Artificial Intelligence. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2019. p. 472–8.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  38. Nilsson J, Thomas AJ, Stevens LH, McAllister-Williams RH, Ferrier IN, Gallagher P. The interrelationship between attentional and executive deficits in major depressive disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2016;134:73–82.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Snyder HR. Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: a meta-analysis and review. Psychol Bull. 2013;139:81–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Tran T, Hagen AEF, Hollenstein T, Bowie CR. Physical- and Cognitive-Effort-Based Decision-Making in Depression: Relationships to Symptoms and Functioning. Clinical Psychological Science. 2021;9:53–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Anja Blaser and Lea Pfister for the transcription of the speech recordings. We would also like to thank all participants.

Funding

This study did not receive any funding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Author contributions included conception and study design (AK, JP, JK, CK, NL, JT), data acquisition (JT, NL, CW), statistical analysis (MM, EM, JT, NL), interpretation of results (all authors), drafting the manuscript or revising it critically for important intellectual content (all authors), and approval of the final version to be published and agreement to be accountable for the integrity and accuracy of all aspects of the work (all authors).

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jessica Peter.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All participants gave written informed consent prior to study participation. According to the standards of the Ethics Committees of the Universities of Koblenz-Landau and Saarbrücken, no formal ethical approval was required for this study, since it did not include patients, the participants were not expected to take risks, there was no fMRI, magnetic stimulation, or a pharmacological intervention. This is also in accordance with the standards of the German Research Council (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft; see here: https://www.dfg.de/en/research_funding/faq/faq_humanities_social_science/index.html).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:

Table S1. Overview of extracted speech features. Table S2. Overview of extracted transcript features.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

König, A., Tröger, J., Mallick, E. et al. Detecting subtle signs of depression with automated speech analysis in a non-clinical sample. BMC Psychiatry 22, 830 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-04475-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-04475-0

Keywords