Skip to main content

Psychometric properties of an Arabic translation of the external and internal shame scale (EISS)

Abstract

Background

The concept of shame in Arab societies displays multiple differences when compared to Western societies in terms of nature, sources, types, and correlates. Surprisingly, we could not find any study investigating this increasingly important construct in Arab countries or the broad Arabic-speaking communities. This may likely be due to the lack of valid instruments assessing shame in the Arabic language. To address this major gap and contribute to the international literature, we sought to examine the psychometric properties of an Arabic translation of the External and Internal Shame Scale (EISS) among a community sample of Arabic-speaking adults from Lebanon.

Methods

An online survey was conducted among Lebanese adults between July and August 2022. A total of 570 Lebanese adults completed the EISS, as well as Depression Anxiety Stress Scales, Other as shamer scale, and the Standardized Stigmatization Questionnaire. Exploratory-to-confirmatory (EFA-CFA) factor analyses were conducted.

Results

Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses supported a unidimensional model of EISS scores, with all eight items retained. Scores achieved scalar invariance across gender, with no significant difference reported between females and males. EISS scores were found to have adequate composite reliability (McDonald’s ω = 0.88 for the total score); as well as adequate patterns of correlations with depression, anxiety and stress symptoms, as well as stigmatization scores. Finally, our analyses provide support to the concurrent validity of the Arabic version of the scale, by showing that the EISS total scores strongly correlated with the external shame measure “other as shamer”.

Conclusion

Although further validations are necessary before our findings could be generalized, we preliminarily suggest that this is a short, easy-to-use, self-report scale that enables a reliable and valid measure of the shame construct among Arabic-speaking people.

Peer Review reports

Background

Shame is a “self-conscious” feeling affecting a person’s sense of self and wellbeing [1]. It has been suggested that shame develops as a mechanism to control psychobiological reactions to social status. Historically, researchers referred to shame and guilt interchangeably. Shame and guilt share several common characteristics, including the fact that both are self-conscious emotions induced by self-evaluation and self-reflection [2], both help with self-regulation [3], both generate feelings of distress [4]. In addition, both constructs often coexist [5] and correlate with each other [6,7,8]. Despite the similarities, major differences between shame and guilt also exist. One difference lies to motivational consequences, with shame being driven by defensive interpersonal detachment and separation; while guilt involves positive preventive incentives [9, 10]. As opposed to guilt proneness, shame proneness is linked to a broad range of psychosomatic manifestations, including depression, suicidality, low self-esteem, eating problems, and posttraumatic stress disorders [11]. Both constructs differentially correlate with empathy; with guilt relating positively with this construct and shame showing reversed patterns of association [10]. The two constructs also play distinct roles in behavioral problems, with shame positively correlating with illegal behavior and guilt inversely correlating with risky and antisocial behavior [12]. Therefore, using measures that differentiate between these behavioral and emotional aspects of shame and guilt, or that focus solely on each of these constructs, are necessary [11].

The biopsychosocial and evolutionary model claimed that two distinct types of the shame experience exist: “internal shame” (IS) and “external shame” (ES) [13, 14]. IS is related to internal dynamics of the self and refers to the poor opinion of the self (such as being empty, undesirable, isolated, inferior, or inadequate) [14]; and how the self-judges and feels about itself [15]. In the IS, there is a negative self-assessment of one’s emotions, attributes, and behavior, by focusing on the self’s imperfections and flaws [16, 17]. On the other hand, ES focuses on the social environment and the perception of being judged, attacked or rejected by others, or that others have bad impressions of oneself, such as being inferior, defective, unattractive, undesirable, or worthless [3, 14]. The two concepts of IS and ES have been shown to be highly correlated and interdependent; with individuals who experience IS being highly likely to also feel ES [18]. Both ES and IS can lead to social exclusion and regarded as different dimensions of the identical emotional experience. Both components of shame are closely linked and encompass the same core domains of inferiority/inadequacy, exclusion, emptiness, and criticism [19]. Both aspects of shame are furthermore crucial for social functioning and regarded as a sign or warning of possible social harm [19]. In this regard, prior research demonstrated that shame positively correlates with a range of behavioral and psychopathological indicators, including depressive symptoms [20], social anxiety [21, 22], other anxiety disorders symptoms [23, 24], disordered eating [21, 25], substance use [26], and suicidal behavior [27]. It is of note, however, that despite presenting a negative valence, the sense of shame may be adaptive, enabling a certain compliance with social and moral norms, and facilitating social interactions [3, 28]. Given that shame is associated with psychological conditions, assessing shame and validating scales to measure the shame construct is extremely relevant [29].

The measures that have been designed and largely used to assess shame were either targeting ES (e.g., the Other as Shamer Scale [18]), or IS (e.g., the Internalized shame scale [30]). It is only recently that a self-report measure assessing ES and IS simultaneously, and yielding a global score of sense of shame, has been developed by Ferreira et al. (i.e., the External and Internal Shame Scale (EISS) [19]). The EISS consists of 8 items assessing both ES and IS as well as shame as a general concept [19]. The original validation of the EISS in Portuguese among community adults showed good concurrent validity, internal consistency, and positive correlations with depressive symptoms [19]. Since then, the EISS has been translated in different cultures and languages other than English (e.g., Japanese [31], Portuguese and French [32]), it has also been adapted and validated for various populations (e.g., adolescents [1]). More recently, Matos et al. [32] provided evidence of the validity of the EISS in a cross-national study including community samples of adults from five countries (Portugal, France, Australia, Singapore and Japan). However, no Arabic version exists yet, to the best of our knowledge.

Although the shame construct represents a universal and basic human emotion [33,34,35], it has also been shown to vary in meaning and positioning across people and cultures [36]. Some societies are much more shaming than others (e.g., Japanese [37]). Shame would be more prominent in collectivistic cultures [38]. Regarding Arab societies in particular, the concept of shame displays multiple differences when compared to Western societies, including possible sources (e.g., meeting strangers), types (e.g., praised or respect-related shame, such as Arab children who are taught to be shy and respectful to others), and correlates (e.g., age- and sex-graded shame, such as speaking loudly in public which is shaming for Arab women but not for men) [39]. To date, there is still a very limited body of research investigating shame in Arab countries or the broad Arabic-speaking communities. This may likely be due to the lack of valid instruments assessing this concept in the Arabic language. For instance, a cross-cultural study by Grey et al. [40] revealed that Arab Emirati students displayed significantly greater tendency to evaluate the self negatively following an experience of shame than their Irish counterparts; and that, in contrast, Irish students reported higher levels of shame withdrawal following a transgression. In light of their results, authors suggested that shame would help Emirati individuals abide by the norms of the Muslim culture, and thus appears to play a regulatory function [41]; whereas shame seems to be related to a more maladaptive behavioral tendency (i.e., avoidance) in Irish individuals [40]. These differences in features of shame across the two countries have been explained by cultural variations in shame’s responses that extend beyond the dichotomous categorization of individualistic versus collectivistic cultures [40]. However, the scarcity of research in this area hampers our understanding of the mechanisms underlying the cultural diversity regarding the shame construct. Arab countries are becoming increasingly open to Westernization and globalization, and most of the Arab societies are experiencing continuous changes, making unclear whether concepts such shame are also changing. This highlight the urgent need for future research on this topic in Arab countries. To address this major gap, and contribute to the international literature in this increasingly important area, we sought to examine the psychometric properties of an Arabic translation of the EISS among a community sample of Arabic-speaking adults from Lebanon. We hypothesized that analyses will confirm the factor structure found in the original validation. We also expect that the Arabic EISS will show adequate reliability and validity.

Methods

Participants

A total of 570 Lebanese adults enrolled in this study, with a mean age of 24.59 years (SD = 6.75) and 68.6% women. Other sample characteristics are displayed in Table 1.

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of the participants

Measures

The External and Internal Shame Scale (EISS). This scale consists of eight items, generated to measure the four central domains of general feelings of shame, and present in both ES and IS: inferiority/inadequacy, sense of exclusion, uselessness/emptiness and criticism/judgment [19]. Each of the dimensions is composed of four items: external (e.g., “I feel that others see me as uninteresting”) and internal dimensions (e.g., “I feel that I am different and inferior to the others”), to which the participants must answer using a 5-point scale (0 = “Never” to 4 = “Always”). Scores vary between 0 and 32 points, with higher values indicating higher global sense of shame.

Translation procedure

A common procedure of back-translation was followed in the present study for all non-validated scales, in which a text is translated from a source into a target language, and then independently back-translated into the source language by a second interpreter. Therefore, the English version of the EISS was translated to Arabic by a Lebanese translator who was completely unrelated to the study. Afterwards, a Lebanese psychologist with a full working proficiency in English, translated the Arabic version back to English. To evaluate the accuracy of the translation, the initial and back-translated English versions were compared [42, 43]; and any inconsistencies were detected and eliminated by a committee composed of the research team and the two translators. A pilot study was done on 20 participants to make sure that the questions are well understood; no changes were done afterwards.

Other as shamer (OAS-2) [44], a short version of the OAS [18], includes 8 items intended to measure external shame (global judgements of how people think others view them). Respondents are asked to rate on a 5-point scale (0–4) the frequency of their feelings and experiences in items such as “People distance themselves from me when I make mistakes”. Higher scores reveal high external shame.

The Standardized Stigmatization Questionnaire (SSQ) [45]: This is a 13-item, 4-point Likert measure assessing the perception of social stigmatization and the predisposition to enact stigmatization through three dimensions: Social self-interest, Evolutionary self-interest, and Psychological self-interest. More elevated scores refer to greater stigmatization.

Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-8), validated in Arabic [46], comprises eight items, in three subscales: depression (three items e.g., felt down hearted and blue), anxiety (three items e.g., felt scared without reason), and stress (two items e.g., was using a lot of my mental energy) [46]. The total scores of the DASS-8 and its subscales range between 0 and 24, 0 to 9, 0 to 9, and 0 to 6, respectively.

Demographics. Participants were asked to provide their demographic details consisting of age, gender, highest educational attainment, region of living, marital status. The number of persons and rooms in the house were used to compute the household crowding index (person/room); the higher the number, the lower the socioeconomic status [47].

Procedures

All data were collected via a Google Form link, between July and August 2022. The project was advertised on social media and needed between 10 and 15 min to be completed. The link was shared among participants and sent to all districts/governorates of Lebanon (Beirut, Mount Lebanon, North Lebanon, South Lebanon, and Bekaa) through social networks, using the snowball technique. Inclusion criteria for participation included being of a resident and citizen of Lebanon of adult age. Internet protocol (IP) addresses were examined to ensure that no participant took the survey more than once. After providing digital informed consent, participants were asked to complete the instruments described above, which were presented in a pre-randomized order to control for order effects. The survey was anonymous and participants completed the survey voluntarily and without remuneration.

Analytic strategy

Data treatment.

There were no missing responses in the dataset. To achieve the goal of this study, we first sought to identify the appropriate factor structure of scores on the Arabic EISS. Thus, we followed best-practice recommendations in adopting an EFA-to-CFA strategy [48, 49], which allows to examine the most suitable model of EISS scores in our sample without modelling limitations (i.e., through EFA) and to cross-validate the EFA-derived model, as well as the original unidimensional model (if discrepant), in a separate subsample (i.e., using CFA). To ensure adequate sample sizes for both EFA and CFA, we split the main sample using an SPSS computer-generated random technique; sample characteristics of the two split-halves are reported in Table 1. There were no significant differences between the two subsamples in terms of mean age, t(568) = 0.984, p = .326, household crowding index, t(568) = 0.072, p = .170, and the distribution of women and men, χ2(1) = 0.296, p = .587, single and married, χ2(1) = 0.002, p = .965, education level χ2(1) = 2.22, p = .136 and region of living χ2(1) = 0.683, p = .409.

Exploratory factor analysis.

To explore the factor structure of the EISS scale, we computed a principal-axis EFA with the first split-half subsample using the FACTOR software [50, 51]. We verified all requirements related to item-communality [52], average item correlations, and item-total correlations [53]. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy (which should ideally be ≥ 0.80) and Bartlett’s test of sphericity (which should be significant) ensured the adequacy of our sample [54]. The procedure for determining the number of factors to extract was parallel analysis (PA; [55] using the Pearson correlation matrix. Weighted Root Mean Square Residual (WRMR) was also calculated to assess the model fit (values < 1 have been recommended to represent good fit) [56].

Item retention was based on the recommendation that items with “fair” loadings (i.e., ≥ 0.40), communality (i.e., ≥ 0.30) and above (i.e., ≥ 0.33) and with low inter-item correlations (suggestive of low item redundancy) as indicated by the anti-image correlation matrix should be retained [57].

Confirmatory factor analysis.

We used data from the second split-half to conduct a CFA using the SPSS AMOS v.29 software. A previous study suggested that the minimum sample size to conduct a confirmatory factor analysis ranges from 3 to 20 times the number of the scale’s variables [58]. Therefore, we assumed a minimum sample of 250 participants needed to have enough statistical power based on a ratio of 15 participants per one item of the scale, which was exceeded in this subsample. Parameter estimates were obtained using the robust maximum likelihood method and fit indices. Values ≤ 5 for χ²/df, and ≤ 0.08 for RMSEA, and 0.90 for CFI and TLI indicate good fit of the model to the data [59]. However, these cut-off values should not be interpreted rigidly (Heene, Hilbert, Draxler, Ziegler, & Bühner, 2011; Perry, Nicholls, Clough, & Crust, 2015); values between 0.08 and 0.10 for RMSEA can indicate acceptable but mediocre fit to the data [60, 61].

Gender invariance.

To examine gender invariance of the EISS, we conducted multi-group CFA [62] using the second split-half subsample. Measurement invariance was assessed at the configural, metric, and scalar levels [63]. Configural invariance implies that the latent scales’ variable(s) and the pattern of loadings of the latent variable(s) on indicators are similar across gender (i.e., the unconstrained latent model should fit the data well in both groups). Metric invariance implies that the magnitude of the loadings is similar across gender; this is tested by comparing two nested models consisting of a baseline model and an invariance model. Lastly, scalar invariance implies that both the item loadings and item intercepts are similar across gender and is examined using the same nested-model comparison strategy as with metric invariance [62]. Following previous recommendations [62, 64], we accepted ΔCFI ≤ 0.010 and ΔRMSEA ≤ 0.015 or ΔSRMR ≤ 0.010 (0.030 for factorial invariance) as evidence of invariance. We aimed to test for gender differences on latent EISS scores using an independent-samples t-test only if scalar or partial scalar invariance were established [65].

Further analyses.

Composite reliability in both subsamples was assessed using McDonald’s (1970) ω and its associated 95% CI, with values greater than 0.70 reflecting adequate composite reliability [66]. McDonald’s ω was selected as a measure of composite reliability because of known problems with the use of Cronbach’s α (e.g., [67]. To assess convergent and criterion-related validity, we examined bivariate correlations between the EISS scores and the additional measures included in the survey (DASS-8 and Others as shame). All scores had a normal distribution, as identified by skewness and kurtosis values varying between − 1 and + 1 [68]; therefore, Pearson correlation test was used to correlate two continuous variables, whereas the Student t test was used for the comparison of two means. Based on [69], values ≤ 0.10 were considered weak, ~ 0.30 were considered moderate, and ~ 0.50 were considered strong correlations.

Results

Exploratory factor analysis of the EISS

Factor analysis on sample 1 (EISS- 8 items).

The Bartlett’s test of sphericity, χ2(28) = 1190.2, p < .001, and KMO (0.892) indicated that the EISS items had adequate common variance for factor analysis. The results of the EFA revealed one factor, which explained 57.85% of the common variance. The WRMR value was also adequate (= 0.116; 95% CI 0.089-0.134), indicating good fit of the model. McDonald’s ω was adequate in the total subsample (ω = 0.89).

Factor structure congruence and composite reliability (EISS- 8 items).

The factor loadings reported in Table 2. McDonald’s ω was adequate in women (ω = 0.88), men (ω = 0.89), and the total subsample (ω = 0.89).

Confirmatory factor analysis of the EISS scale on sample 2

CFA indicated that fit indices of the 1-factor model of EISS (8 items) were acceptable: χ2/df = 66.71/18 = 3.70, RMSEA = 0.096 (90% CI 0.072, 0.122), SRMR = 0.037, CFI = 0.958, TLI = 0.935. When adding a correlation between items 2–3 and 4–7, the fit indices improved as follows: χ2/df = 66.71/18 = 3.71, RMSEA = 0.096 (90% CI 0.072, 0.122), SRMR = 0.036, CFI = 0.958, TLI = 0.935. The standardized estimates of factor loadings were all adequate (see Table 1).

Composite reliability.

Composite reliability of scores was adequate in women (ω = 0.88), men (ω = 0.90), and the total sample (ω = 0.88).

Table 2 Items of the EISS in English and Factor Loadings Derived from the Exploratory Factor Analyses (EFA) in the First Split-Half Subsample, and Standardised Estimates of Factor Loadings from the Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) in the Second Split-Half Subsample

Gender invariance of the EISS 8 items scale

As reported in Table 3, all indices suggested that configural, metric, and scalar invariance was supported across gender. No significant difference was found between women (M = 7.67, SD = 5.76) and men (M = 8.92, SD = 6.54) in the second subsample, t(291) = 1.638, p = .102, d = 0.202.

Table 3 Measurement Invariance of the EISS 8 items Across Gender in the Second Split-Half Subsample

Convergent and Criterion-Related Validity

To assess the validity of the EISS scores, we examined bivariate correlations with all other measures included in the present study using the total sample. Higher EISS scores correlated significantly and positively with higher stigmatization dimensions (social self-interest, evolutionary self-interest, psychological self-interest), stress, depression, anxiety and other as shamer scores (Table 4).

Table 4 Correlations of the EISS scores with the other measures in the second split-half subsample

Discussion

To date, there is only one measure that assesses both ES and IS as conceptualized by the evolutionary biopsychosocial model, the EISS. We aimed through the present study to validate this instrument in Arabic, in order to enable its use for clinical and research purposes among the wide Arabic-speaking populations globally. Our results support the validity of the EISS, as well as invariance across gender and composite reliability (McDonald’s ω = 0.88 for the total score). We suggest, accordingly, that the Arabic EISS is psychometrically sound and recommend its use for measuring shame in different Arab settings and contexts.

In terms of the factorial validity of the Arabic EISS, our analyses failed to support the two-factor structure originally supported by Ferreira et al. [19]. The eight items retained in the final model were based on a unidimensional factor structure, enabling the simultaneous assessment of ES and IS through a global score rather than two separate dimensions (ES and IS) as suggested by the developers of the scale. Indeed, fit of the unidimensional model of EISS scores in the present study was adequate when tested using both EFA and CFA. These results are broadly in line with the Japanese validation study in which EFA revealed that, unlike the originally proposed factor structure, all the eight items retained loaded into three factors [31]. This might be explained by cultural considerations. The EISS has been developed in a Western cultural background. As previously mentioned, the concept of shame as experienced by Arab people has its peculiarities and characteristics that differentiate it from that perceived by people from the Western, individualistic and developed countries. One important difference is that shame in the Arab culture is closely related to the fact that one imagines or knows that other people are watching them or thinking about them [39]. In other words, feeling IS would rather be intimately related to ES, and cannot exist without others’ perceptions about the self in Arab cultures. This could explain the unidimensionality of the shame concept in our sample. However, in light of these inconsistencies regarding the factor structure of the EISS, additional research still needs to be done to confirm the plausibility of the Arabic EISS structure in other Arab cultures, as well as in non-clinical samples. Our results also indicated that the single-factor structure of EISS scores was identical across women and men (in our EFA) and achieved full invariance (in our CFA). Previous studies have showed that variances of EISS scores exist across gender where females generally report higher scores both in ES and IS [19, 28, 70]. Similar patterns of comparisons of shame across gender has also been reported in the original validation [19], and in the adolescent version of the scale [1].

Our results showed that higher EISS scores were correlated with greater symptoms of depression, anxiety and stress. These findings are consistent with previous literature where the crucial role of shame in a range of psychological problems, including as depression, anxiety and stress, has been largely highlighted [29, 31, 71,72,73]. Overall, these results are consistent with the original validation by Ferreira et al. [19], and with those in adolescent samples [1]. Shame plays a critical role in the vulnerability and maintenance of mental health problems and psychological disorders, mainly depression [19]. Our findings also revealed that shame is associated with the three stigmatization dimensions (evolutionary, social, and psychological self-interest), which is consistent with previous literature data widely acknowledging a positive association between stigma and shame [74]. Stigma is found to result in numerous negative self-conscious emotions, including shame [74]. Finally, our analyses provide support to the concurrent validity of the Arabic version of the scale, by showing that the EISS total scores strongly correlated with the ES measure “other as shamer”. This demonstrates that the Arabic EISS is a valid scale to assess the shame construct.

Limitations

A number of limitations of the present study could be improved in future studies. First, given the method of recruitment, which was performed online, and mostly attracted educated and female participants, it is unlikely that our sample is representative of the wider Lebanese population. Consequently, the gender invariance results should be interpreted with caution because of the numbers inequality between males and females. The response rate could not be determined. A further limitation of the present work was that we did not assess the relationship between shame and other relevant indices, such as other self-conscious emotions (e.g., guilt and humiliation) or psychological adjustment (e.g., self-reassurance). In addition, other psychometric characteristics, such as temporal stability and validity across Arab countries, still need to be examined to confirm the robustness of the Arabic EISS. Finally, psychometric properties of the Arabic version of the EISS have been examined in one Arab country, Lebanon; which may limit the generalizability of our findings to Arabic-speaking populations from other countries and cultural backgrounds. We highlight, however, that the scale was translated to literary Arabic and not Lebanese dialect; which guarantees its readability across all Arab countries. Nevertheless, we recognize that future linguistic invariance studies still need to be performed to further support the psychometric properties of the Arabic EISS. before generalizing the EISS to other Arab countries.

Clinical implications

As for clinical implications, our findings point to the clinical relevance of the shame construct in Arab individuals. Indeed, the association of shame with high levels of psychological distress and stigma supports the previous assumptions that shame could be an indicator of a maladaptive response styles in Arab cultural backgrounds [40]. As such, professionals working in Arab settings ought to acknowledge the role of shame in mental health. Our findings shed light on the scarcity of research in this area in Arab contexts; which is partly due to a lack of universal, standardized and valid instruments to assess this construct. We therefore call for further studies to investigate the cultural peculiarities of shame and how it interferes with health indicators in Arabic-speaking communities. We believe that making the Arabic EISS available will hopefully encourage fruitful research output on shame from diverse disciplinary backgrounds, and have significant implications for improving the quality of cross-cultural studies in this area. Additionally, by ascertaining the consistency of measurement quality across gender groups, we suggest that the Arabic EISS can be used for gender comparisons in future research among Arabic-speaking populations.

Conclusion

The present results support the reliability and validity of the one-factor, 7-item Arabic EISS. Although further validations are necessary before our findings could be generalized, we preliminarily suggest that this is a short, easy-to-use, self-report scale that enables a reliable and valid measure of the shame construct among Arabic-speaking people. The availability of the Arabic EISS will hopefully benefit researchers and clinicians who work in Arab settings, and allow for future cross-national comparisons of shame while including Arab cultures.

Data Availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are not publicly available due the restrictions by the ethics committee. The dataset supporting the conclusions is available upon request to the corresponding author S.H.

References

  1. Cunha M, Silva P, Ferreira C, Galhardo A. Measuring Shame in Adolescents: Validation Studies of the External and Internal Shame Scale in a Community Sample. Child & Youth Care Forum: 2021:Springer; 2021:pp. 971–989.

  2. Tangney JPE, Tracy JL. Self-conscious emotions. 2012.

  3. Tangney JP, Dearing RL. Shame and guilt:Guilford press; 2003.

  4. Tangney JP, Stuewig J, Mashek DJ. Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:345–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Eisenberg N. Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Ann Rev Psychol. 2000;51(1):665–97.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Ferguson TJ, Crowley SL. Measure for measure: a multitrait-multimethod analysis of guilt and shame. J Pers Assess. 1997;69(2):425–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Harder DW. Shame and guilt assessment, and relationships of shame-and guilt-proneness to psychopathology. 1995.

  8. Cohen TR, Wolf ST, Panter AT, Insko CA. Introducing the GASP scale: a new measure of guilt and shame proneness. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2011;100(5):947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Elison J, Lennon R, Pulos S. Investigating the compass of shame: the development of the compass of shame scale. Social Behav Personality: Int J. 2006;34(3):221–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Tangney JP. Conceptual and methodological issues in the assessment of shame and guilt. Behav Res Ther. 1996;34(9):741–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Makogon IK, Enikolopov SN. Problems with the assessment of shame and guilt. Psychology in Russia State of the Art 2013, 6(4).

  12. Guimón J, Las Hayas C, Guillén V, Boyra A, González-Pinto A. Shame, sensitivity to punishment and psychiatric disorders. Eur J psychiatry. 2007;21(2):124–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Gilbert P, Irons C. Shame, self-criticism, and self-compassion in adolescence. Adolesc emotional Dev emergence depressive disorders. 2009;1:195–214.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Gilbert P. Evolution, social roles, and the differences in shame and guilt. Social Research: An International Quarterly. 2003;70(4):1205–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Sedighimornani N. Shame and its features: understanding of shame. European Journal of Social Sciences Studies 2018.

  16. Gilbert P, Miles J. Body shame: Conceptualisation, research, and treatment. Psychology Press; 2002.

  17. Michael L, Lewis M, Lisa FB. Self-Conscious Emotions: Embarrassment, Pride, Shame, and Guilt. 2000.

  18. Goss K, Gilbert P, Allan S. An exploration of shame measures—I: the other as Shamer scale. Pers Indiv Differ. 1994;17(5):713–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Ferreira C, Moura-Ramos M, Matos M, Galhardo A. A new measure to assess external and internal shame: Development, factor structure and psychometric properties of the external and internal shame scale. Current Psychology 2020:1–10.

  20. Andrews B, Qian M, Valentine JD. Predicting depressive symptoms with a new measure of shame: the experience of shame scale. Br J Clin Psychol. 2002;41(1):29–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Levinson CA, Byrne M, Rodebaugh TL. Shame and guilt as shared vulnerability factors: shame, but not guilt, prospectively predicts both social anxiety and bulimic symptoms. Eat Behav. 2016;22:188–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Arditte KA, Morabito DM, Shaw AM, Timpano KR. Interpersonal risk for suicide in social anxiety: the roles of shame and depression. Psychiatry Res. 2016;239:139–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Muris P, Meesters C, Bouwman L, Notermans S. Relations among behavioral inhibition, shame-and guilt-proneness, and anxiety disorders symptoms in non-clinical children. Child Psychiatry & Human Development. 2015;46(2):209–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Shahar B, Bar-Kalifa E, Hen-Weissberg A. Shame during social interactions predicts subsequent generalized anxiety symptoms: a daily-diary study. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2015;34(10):827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Duarte C, Pinto-Gouveia J, Rodrigues T. Being bullied and feeling ashamed: implications for eating psychopathology and depression in adolescent girls. J Adolesc. 2015;44:259–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Luoma JB, Chwyl C, Kaplan J. Substance use and shame: a systematic and meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2019;70:1–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Cameron AY, Shea MT, Randall AB. Acute shame predicts urges for suicide but not for substance use in a veteran population. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior. 2020;50(1):292–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Muris P, Meesters C, van Asseldonk M. Shame on me! Self-conscious emotions and big five personality traits and their relations to anxiety disorders symptoms in young, non-clinical adolescents. Child Psychiatry & Human Development. 2018;49(2):268–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Kim S, Thibodeau R, Jorgensen RS. Shame, guilt, and depressive symptoms: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(1):68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Cook D. Internalized shame scale: technical manual. Multi-Health Systems. Inc, North Tonawanda, NY 1994.

  31. Hiramatsu Y, Asano K, Kotera Y, Endo A, Shimizu E, Matos M. Development of the external and internal shame scale: japanese version. BMC Res Notes. 2021;14(1):1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Matos M, Galhardo A, Moura-Ramos M, Steindl SR, Bortolon C, Hiramatsu Y, Baumann T, Yiu RXQ, Ferreira C. Measuring shame across five countries: dimensionality and measurement invariance of the external and internal shame scale. Current Psychology 2021:1–10.

  33. Sznycer D, Xygalatas D, Agey E, Alami S, An X-F, Ananyeva KI, Atkinson QD, Broitman BR, Conte TJ, Flores C. Cross-cultural invariances in the architecture of shame. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2018, 115(39):9702–9707.

  34. Sznycer D, Takemura K, Delton AW, Sato K, Robertson T, Cosmides L, Tooby J. Cross-cultural differences and similarities in proneness to shame: an adaptationist and ecological approach. Evolutionary Psychol. 2012;10(2):147470491201000213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Tracy JL, Matsumoto D. The spontaneous expression of pride and shame: Evidence for biologically innate nonverbal displays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2008, 105(33):11655–11660.

  36. Fujikawa H, Eto M. The Complex Concept of shame: a product of history and culture. Acad Med. 2022;97(8):1095.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Takada A. Socialization practices regarding shame in japanese caregiver–child interactions. Front Psychol. 2019;10:1545.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Fessler D. Shame in two cultures: implications for evolutionary approaches. J Cognition Cult. 2004;4(2):207–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Al Jallad N. The concept of” shame” in Arabic: bilingual dictionaries and the challenge of defining culture-based emotions. Lang design: J theoretical experimental linguistics. 2010;12:0031–57.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Grey I, Daly R, Thomas J, Marassas W. The relationship between shame and guilt: cultural comparisons between Ireland and the United Arab Emirates. Mental Health Religion & Culture. 2018;21(3):221–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Bedford O, Hwang KK. Guilt and shame in chinese culture: a cross-cultural framework from the perspective of morality and identity. J Theory Social Behav. 2003;33(2):127–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Hambleton RK. The next generation of the ITC Test Translation and Adaptation Guidelines. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2001;17(3):164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Van de Vijver F, Tanzer NK. Bias and equivalence in cross-cultural assessment: an overview. Eur Rev Appl Psychol. 2004;54(2):119–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Matos M, Pinto-Gouveia J, Gilbert P, Duarte C, Figueiredo C. The other as Shamer Scale–2: development and validation of a short version of a measure of external shame. Pers Indiv Differ. 2015;74:6–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Haghighat R. The development of an instrument to measure stigmatization: factor analysis and origin of stigmatization. Eur J psychiatry. 2005;19(3):144–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Ali AM, Hori H, Kim Y, Kunugi H. The Depression anxiety stress scale 8-items expresses robust psychometric properties as an ideal shorter version of the Depression anxiety stress scale 21 among healthy respondents from three continents. Front Psychol. 2022;13:799769.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Melki I, Beydoun H, Khogali M, Tamim H, Yunis K. Household crowding index: a correlate of socioeconomic status and inter-pregnancy spacing in an urban setting. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004;58(6):476–80.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Swami V, Barron D. Translation and validation of body image instruments: Challenges, good practice guidelines, and reporting recommendations for test adaptation. Body image. 2019;31:204–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Swami V, Todd J, Barron D. Translation and validation of body image instruments: an addendum to Swami and Barron (2019) in the form of frequently asked questions. Body image. 2021;37:214–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Lorenzo-Seva U, Ten Berge J. Tucker’s congruence coefficient as a meaningful index of factor similarity. Methodology: Eur J Res Methods Behav Social Sci. 2006;2(2):57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Lorenzo-Seva U, Ferrando P. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2006;38(1):88–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Worthington RL, Whittaker TA. Scale development research: a content analysis and recommendations for best practices. Couns Psychol. 2006;34(6):806–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Clark L, Watson D. Construct validity: basic issues in objective scale development. Psychol Meas. 1995;28:61–75.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Hair JF. Multivariate data analysis. 2009.

  55. Timmerman ME, Lorenzo-Seva U. Dimensionality assessment of ordered polytomous items with parallel analysis. Psychol Methods. 2011;16(2):209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Yu C, Muthen B. Evaluation of model fit indices for latent variable models with categorical and continuous outcomes. In: Paper presented at the annual conference of the American Educational Research Association, April 4, 2002, New Orleans: 2002; 2002.

  57. Tie B, Chen G, He J. Validation of the inflexible eating questionnaire in a large sample of chinese adolescents: psychometric properties and gender-related differential item functioning. Eat Weight Disorders-Studies Anorexia Bulimia Obes. 2022;27(3):1029–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Mundfrom DJ, Shaw DG, Ke TL. Minimum sample size recommendations for conducting factor analyses. Int J Test. 2005;5(2):159–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Hu Lt, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct equation modeling: multidisciplinary J. 1999;6(1):1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Hooper D, Coughlan J, Mullen MR. Structural equation modelling: guidelines for determining model fit. Electron J Bus Res methods. 2008;6(1):pp53–60.

    Google Scholar 

  61. MacCallum RC, Browne MW, Sugawara HM. Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychol Methods. 1996;1(2):130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct equation modeling: multidisciplinary J. 2007;14(3):464–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Vadenberg R, Lance C. A review and synthesis of the measurement in variance literature: suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organ Res Methods. 2000;3:4–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct Equ Model. 2002;9(2):233–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Swami V, Todd J, Azzi V, Malaeb D, El Dine AS, Obeid S, Hallit S. Psychometric properties of an arabic translation of the functionality appreciation scale (FAS) in lebanese adults. Body Image. 2022;42:361–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Dunn TJ, Baguley T, Brunsden V. From alpha to omega: a practical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency estimation. Br J Psychol. 2014;105(3):399–412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. McNeish D. Thanks coefficient alpha, we’ll take it from here. Psychol Methods. 2018;23(3):412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Hair JF Jr, Sarstedt M, Ringle CM, Gudergan SP. Advanced issues in partial least squares structural equation modeling. saGe publications; 2017.

  69. Cohen J. Quantitative methods in psychology: a power primer. In: psychological bulletin: 1992. Citeseer; 1992.

  70. Else-Quest NM, Higgins A, Allison C, Morton LC. Gender differences in self-conscious emotional experience: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2012;138(5):947.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Pinto-Gouveia J, Matos M. Can shame memories become a key to identity? The centrality of shame memories predicts psychopathology. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2011;25(2):281–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Cunha M, Matos M, Faria D, Zagalo S. Shame memories and psychopathology in adolescence: the mediator effect of shame. Int J Psychol Psychol Therapy. 2012;12(2):203–18.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Paulus DJ, Vanwoerden S, Norton PJ, Sharp C. Emotion dysregulation, psychological inflexibility, and shame as explanatory factors between neuroticism and depression. J Affect Disord. 2016;190:376–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Dolezal L. Shame anxiety, stigma and clinical encounters. J Eval Clin Pract. 2022;28(5):854–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank all participants.

Funding

None.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SH designed the study; MD collected the data, FFR and DM drafted the manuscript; SH carried out the analysis and interpreted the results; RH and SO reviewed the paper for intellectual content; all authors reviewed the final manuscript and gave their consent.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Sahar Obeid or Souheil Hallit.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Ethics and Research Committee at the School of Pharmacy of the Lebanese International University approved this study protocol (2022RC-048-LIUSOP). A written informed consent was considered obtained from each participant when submitting the online form. All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors have nothing to disclose.

Additional information

Feten Fekih-Romdhane and Diana Malaeb are first coauthors.

Sahar Obeid and Souheil Hallit are last coauthors.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fekih-Romdhane, F., Malaeb, D., Dabbous, M. et al. Psychometric properties of an Arabic translation of the external and internal shame scale (EISS). BMC Psychiatry 23, 242 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-04729-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-04729-5

Keywords