Skip to main content

Temperament and adolescent suicide attempts: a case-control study with multi-ethnic Asian adolescents

Abstract

Background

Suicide is the leading cause of death for adolescents in several parts of Asia, including Singapore. This study examines the relationship between temperament and youth suicide attempts in a sample of multi-ethnic Singaporean adolescents.

Methods

A case-control design compared 60 adolescents (Mage = 16.40, SDage = 2.00) with a recent suicide attempt (i.e., past 6 months) with 58 adolescents (Mage = 16.00, SDage = 1.68) without any history of suicide attempts. Presence of suicide attempts was established using the semi-structured interviewer-administered Columbia Suicide Severity Rating Scale. Participants also completed self-report measures on temperament traits, psychiatric diagnoses, stressful life events, and perceived parental rejection in an interview-based format.

Results

Psychiatric comorbidity, recent stressful life events, perceived parental rejection, and all five “difficult temperament” traits, were significantly overrepresented among adolescent cases relative to healthy controls. Adjusted logistic regression models revealed significant associations between suicide attempt, MDD comorbidity (OR: 10.7, 95% Cl: (2.24–51.39)), “negative mood” trait (OR: 1.12–1.18, 95% Cl: (1.00–1.27)), and the interaction term of “positive mood” and “high adaptability” traits (OR: 0.943 – 0.955, 95% Cl: (0.900 − 0.986)). Specifically, “positive mood” predicted lower likelihood of a suicide attempt when “adaptability” was high (OR: 0.335 – 0.342, 95% Cl: (0.186 − 0.500)) but not low (OR: 0.968 – 0.993, 95% Cl: (0.797 − 1.31)).

Conclusion

Temperament screening may be important to identify adolescents at higher or lower risk of suicide at an early stage. More longitudinal and neurobiological research converging on these temperament findings will be helpful in ascertaining temperament screening as an effective suicide prevention methodology for adolescents.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Adolescent suicide is universally recognized as a public health crisis. In Asia, suicide ranked the first leading cause of death for youths aged 10–19 among several countries (South Korea, [1]; China, [2]; Japan, [3]; Singapore, [4]). Though suicide attempts (SAs) are rare in childhood (i.e., ≤ 13 years old), they increased dramatically through adolescence [5, 6]. During this sensitive period of development, having a difficult temperament [7], a major depressive disorder (MDD) diagnosis [8] or comorbidity [9], experiencing stressful life events [10] or parental rejection [11] posed significant, independent risks to SA. Of which, temperament has garnered much attention in recent years for several reasons. First, a “difficult temperament” was found to elevate an adolescent’s susceptibility to these other SA-related risk factors. Traits remarked to be “difficult” in Western cultures, namely negative affectivity, low adaptability/inflexibility to environmental changes, high withdrawal from novelty, high activity levels, and reduced biological rhythmicity, either independently or interact to incline SA-related risks [12]. For example, the propensity to experience frequent, negative affect is central to the tripartite model of depression [13]. Combining this with an inflexible temperament, they increased the likelihood of alcohol and drug addictions through exacerbated emotional difficulties [14]. Biological irregularity and high activity levels in children can disrupt mutually supportive relationships with their parents, which later translates to difficulties in receiving parental acceptance during adolescence [15]. Highly withdrawn adolescents encountered more stressors in school and at home due to their deficits in social functioning [16]. Beyond the “difficult” profile as a predisposing risk factor, specific temperament traits additionally served as direct predictors or protective factors of adolescent SAs. Anxious, depressive, cyclothymic, and irritable styles of temperament, which were highly related to the temperament trait of “negative affectivity/mood” [17, 18], uniquely predicted SAs across all age groups [19]. The “negative mood” trait retained its predictive power even after controlling for psychiatric disorders (MDD, substance use), childhood sexual abuse, an inflexible temperament, and gender [20]. Currently, these robust findings were not observed among other “difficult” traits [20,21,22]. Conversely, a growing body of research found a hyperthymic temperament to be uniquely associated with reduced likelihood of SAs even after accounting for multiple protective factors [23,24,25,26,27]. Despite being a multifaceted temperament, researchers suggested that the sustained positive affectivity within this profile was largely responsible in providing effective defences against suicidal impulses [28].

As these findings were mainly derived from Western samples, researchers have challenged their generalizability to Asian populations [29]. Culturally and universally, we believe that three areas about temperament as an indirect and direct factor linking to SAs remained relatively understudied: (a) Between individualistic and collectivistic cultures, there were self-reported differences on what constitutes a “difficult” temperament. For instance, while “highly withdrawn” adolescents in Canada typically faced maternal and peer rejection, their counterparts in China experienced entirely opposite outcomes [30, 31]. Among Chinese adolescents, this trait further predicted other positive adjustments including teacher-assessed competence, leadership, academic achievements, self-efficacy, and lower feelings of loneliness and depression [32]. Possibly, this is because “shyness” was commonly perceived as an expression of social maturity and competence in interdependent societies [33]. In contrast, risks associated with the remaining “difficult” traits seemed to apply universally (see [34] for a review). A careful re-evaluation of Western definitions of a “difficult temperament” and its relation to Asian adolescent SAs may be helpful so as to not misinform early screening practices in Asia. (b) Despite existing evidence on the “negative mood” trait being a robust direct predictor, few studies have comprehensively assessed and controlled for other SA-related risk factors that were also influential during adolescent development (i.e., MDD morbidity and comorbidity, proximal stressful life events, perceived parental rejection, other difficult temperaments). (c) Recent studies investigating the protective role of hyperthymic temperament against adolescent SAs yielded mixed findings, either by failing to observe any significant associations [35] or even observing the inverse relationship [36, 37]. For example, Karam et al. (2015) discovered three risk facets underlying a hyperthymic temperament, including “liking to be the boss”, “getting into heated arguments”, and “the right and privilege to do as I please”. One suggestion to reconcile these findings may be to consider the degree of “adaptability” within this profile. Adolescents with low flexibility/adaptability temperaments were more likely to exhibit self-centeredness, higher impulsivity, and confrontational behaviours [38], resembling the three risk facets. Additionally, adolescents with temperaments of positive affectivity but low adaptability previously attempted suicide [39]. This finding challenged previous suggestions that sustained positive affectivity was the main protective factor underlying a hyperthymic temperament. High mood and adaptability were two traits previously associated with high self-confidence [40], which Karam et al. (2015) also found to be the only protective item against SA in hyperthymic female adults. Given these findings, the interplay between “positive affectivity” and “high flexibility/adaptability” may serve as one possible protective pathway against adolescent SAs.

The present study evaluated the risk and protective temperaments among Asian adolescents with previous suicide attempts, according to the following hypotheses: First, contemporaneously with other risk factors (i.e., MDD, other psychiatric disorders, recent stressful life events, parental rejection), a “difficult temperament” but without “high withdrawal” would be significantly overrepresented among Asian adolescent suicide attempters relative to non-attempters. Second, following earlier findings, we expected the “negative affectivity/mood” temperament trait to remain a robust predictor of SA even after controlling for other risk factors. Third, we hypothesized a significant interaction between “positive affectivity/mood” and “adaptability” traits, such that high levels of positive mood and adaptability would be significantly associated with a reduced likelihood of SAs suicide attempts.

Methods

Participants and procedures

Ethics approval was obtained from the Domains-Specific Review Board (DSRB) at a large teaching hospital (i.e., National University Hospital) in Singapore. Recruitment procedures for this study lasted a total of 20 months before the COVID-19 pandemic. The inclusion criteria for cases were adolescents aged 13–19 who were admitted to the National University Hospital’s Emergency Department for a recent SA (i.e., over the last 6 months). The inclusion criteria for controls were same-age adolescents who were admitted to the National University Hospital either (i) for an elective surgery or (ii) for having acute medical conditions without severe morbidity which may not require an elective surgery. Controls who have (i) attempted suicide or self-harm at least once in their lifetime, and/or (ii) suffered from significant physical or clinical morbidity were excluded. This exclusion criteria only applies to control subjects as we intended to approximate them to healthy youths within the community. Control subjects were matched as closely as possible to cases by demographic variables (i.e., age, sex, ethnicity). All adolescents who met the above eligibility criteria as a case or control, along with their accompanying parent(s)/guardian(s), were briefed by their attending doctors on the study’s objectives on the day they were admitted to the ward. These attending doctors then sought the permission of both the adolescent and the parent(s)/guardian(s) to be referred to the study team if the adolescent showed interest to participate in the study. If the parent(s)/guardian(s) of the interested adolescent was not present during the day of admission, the doctors would contact the parent/guardian separately over a phone call to describe the study and seek his/her permission to be referred to the study team. When permission was granted by both the adolescent and the parent(s)/guardian(s), the trained research assistants contacted both parties over phone to schedule an in-person interview at the convenience of all personnels. Over these phone calls, the research assistants also explained that the interview with the adolescent may last for two hours, should informed consent be obtained from both the parent/guardian and the adolescent before the interview commences. All interviews were scheduled within the duration of each adolescent’s ward stay and conducted in their wards. These interviews were conducted over a few days to a week after the initial recruitment. Before the start of the interview, the research assistants refreshed the study’s main purposes and procedures for the adolescent and the parent(s)/guardian(s). After obtaining written informed consent from the parent(s)/guardian(s) and the adolescent himself/herself, the interview proceeds between two trained research assistants and the adolescent for the next two hours during which the adolescent completed an interviewer-administered questionnaire (i.e., Columbia Suicide Severity Scale/C-SSRS; see Sect. 2.2.2) and several self-reports. On average, administration of the C-SSRS took 35 min while completion of the remaining self-reports took 85 min. Fifteen-dollar supermarket vouchers were reimbursed for complete responses. For each adolescent who did not complete the full set of questionnaires due to other reasons, s/he will be reimbursed with a five-dollar supermarket voucher for partial response.

Figure 1 illustrates the CONSORT diagram for this case-control study. Over the 20-month recruitment window, a total of 198 eligible adolescents were admitted to the National University Hospital’s Emergency Department and referred by the attending doctors to the study team. Of these 198 adolescents, 59 of them (30 cases, 29 controls) further declined to schedule an in-person interview with the research assistants. Among the remaining 139 adolescents, permission could not be sought from their parent(s)/guardian(s) for 17 of them (9 cases, 8 controls) as they were uncontactable. Informed consent was obtained from the remaining 122 adolescents (61 cases, 61 controls) and his/her parent(s) before the commencement of their interviews. Partial responses were obtained from 1 case subject and 3 control subjects. These 3 control subjects disclosed previous SAs or self-harm during the administration of the C-SSRS, rendering them unfit as a healthy control for this study. The only case subject described his/her SA as knocking an arm against the wall repetitively, which did not fit into the C-SSRS’s recommended description of a typical SA [41]. As the C-SSRS was the first item to be administered in the interview, these adolescents did not continue with the interview. Subsequently, these responses were excluded from the analyses. Together, the response rate achieved was 65.6% for cases and 60.6% for controls respectively. Our final sample comprises 118 adolescents with complete responses (60 cases, 58 controls, M = 16.20, SD = 1.852), with the majority being Chinese (51%) and Females (72%).

Fig. 1
figure 1

CONSORT Flowchart

All of the research assistants were trained in consent taking procedures and administration of the semi-structured interview and self-reports. Before recruitment commences, the research assistants underwent one full day of training and supervision conducted by author John C.M. Wong who is a certified child and adolescent senior consultant psychiatrist with more than 20 years of practice. The first half of the training session extensively introduced the background and scoring instructions of all the self-reports to be administered to the adolescents. Each research assistant was tasked to familiarise with all items, content, and scoring instructions present within each self-report. This segment adequately prepares the research assistants for potential queries posted by the adolescent respondents when they attempt the self-reports. The second half of the session comprises training the administration of the semi-structured C-SSRS, including planning the safety management protocols if adolescents were to indicate high suicidal risk [42]. During this segment, research assistants took turns to roleplay as the adolescent or interviewer and each roleplay was assessed by author John C.M. Wong. The research assistant imitating a case subject may spontaneously indicate high suicidal risk, which tests the risk management skills of the other research assistant roleplaying as the interviewer. In such an event where high suicidal risk was detected during the interview, the research assistants would gently ask these adolescents if they would be comfortable receiving any formal support from healthcare professionals (i.e., hospital psychiatrists). Independent of their responses, the case would be highlighted immediately to the study team for the authors to inform the parent(s) about his/her suicidal risk. All participants would have consented to these procedures before commencing with the interview.

Measures

Demographic variables

Self-reported information on demographic characteristics included age, sex, ethnicity, religious affiliation, level of education and housing information.

Suicide attempts

The semistructured interviewer-administered Columbia Suicide Severity Rating Scale (C-SSRS; [41]) consists of two subscales, each quantifying the severity of suicidal ideation and the intensity of suicidal behaviours respectively. Items from the “suicidal ideation” subscale were used to evaluate the severity and intensity of suicidal ideation. Similarly, items from the “suicidal behaviour” subscale were used to assess the presence of self-harm and lifetime/previous SAs. The single item on “actual attempts” obtained from the “suicidal behaviour” subscale was used as a screening tool to assess the presence of a recent SA (i.e. in the past 6 months, yes/no). Since this was the only item relevant in answering the study’s hypothesis, it was retained for further statistical analyses. The C-SSRS was widely known as the “gold standard” for suicidal risk assessment [43].

Temperament

Adolescents’ temperaments were assessed using the Revised Dimensions Of Temperament survey (DOTS-R; [44]), which is a 54-item self-report questionnaire that measures nine temperament dimensions as described in Table 1:

Table 1 Nine Dimensions of Temperament measured by DOTS-R

Each of the nine dimensions uses a four-point likert scale ranging from 1 (usually false) to 4 (usually true). Higher scores reflected lower levels of activity, approach, adaptability, persistence, positive mood, and more regular eating and sleeping habits. The DOTS-R was known for its desirable psychometric properties, showing high convergent validity with other temperament measures [45], concurrent validity with personality traits [46], and excellent longitudinal stability across childhood and adolescence [47]. In our sample, all the nine subscales of the DOTS-R showed acceptable internal consistency. (α = 0.62 – 0.89).

Psychiatric disorders

The Youth Inventory-4 (YI-4 ; [48]) is a self-report rating scale that assesses symptoms of DSM-4 emotional and behavioural disorders in youths between 12 and 18 years old. The YI-4 contains 120 items that assess symptoms of 18 disorders, with each item rated on a four-point likert scale (0 = never, 3 = very often). The scale yields Symptom Count scores that are summed to derive criteria for diagnosis. The subscales for 8 diagnoses (yes/no), were used in the present study. MDD Comorbidity (yes/no) was determined when the adolescent met the symptom count criteria for another psychiatric disorder (i.e., Substance Use, Bulimia Nervosa, Generalised Anxiety Disorder, Schizophrenia, Dysthymia, Anorexia Nervosa) on top of MDD. The YI-4 previously demonstrated high reliability, showing consistency in its subscales with those of other diagnostic tools for youths (e.g., Youth Self-Report, [49, 50]). The internal consistency for all 8 symptom count scales in this study were excellent (α = 0.90 ~ 0.97).

Stressful life events

The Adolescent Stress Questionnaire (ASQ; [51]) consists of 58 items reflecting 10 common stress dimensions over the last 12 months. These are the stresses of (a) home life, (b) school performance, (c) school attendance, (d) romantic relationships, (e) peer pressure, (f) teacher interaction, (g) future uncertainty, (h) school/leisure conflict, (i) financial pressure, and k) emerging adult responsibility. Each participant rated the level of stress experienced on a 5-point likert scale (1 = not at all stressful to 5 = very stressful). The ASQ is widely regarded as a reliable and valid instrument in evaluating adolescent stressors from various sources [52,53,54,55]. In this study, the 10 subscales of ASQ showed excellent psychometric properties (α = 0.90–96).

Parental rejection

The Child Parental Acceptance-Rejection Questionnaire (PARQ; [56]) is a 60-item self-report questionnaire reflecting 4 subscales: warmth and affection (reverse scored), aggression and hostility, indifference and neglect, and undifferentiated rejection. Using a 4-point Likert scale, adolescents rated items from 1 = almost never true to 4 = almost always true, with higher scores indicating that s/he perceived the parent to be cold, aggressive, and/or neglecting. The PARQ has demonstrated high reliability across adolescent samples from 8 different countries, along with high discriminant validity of scores [57]. Coefficient alpha for both father and mother forms ranged from 0.86 to 0.92 for all subscales.

Data analysis

All statistics were calculated using IBM SPSS v29.0 [58]. Student’s t-tests and chi-squared analyses were conducted to examine possible group differences in demographic variables, risk factors, and temperament traits. Binary logistic-regression models (i.e. Yes/No for Suicide Attempts) were used to determine the risk and protective temperament traits after adjustment of every risk factor (i.e., MDD/MDD comorbidity, stressful life events, parental rejection). To address the third hypothesis, we included the interaction term (i.e., Flexibility/Rigidity x Mood) in each of these models. To minimise multicollinearity, all variables were centred respectively onto their means and entered into the model simultaneously.

Independent variables for the logistic regression equation of the full model would consist of all demographic variables (age, sex, religion, education level, housing type), temperament traits (Activity Level General, Activity Level Sleep, Approach/Withdrawal, Flexibility/Rigidity, Mood, Rhythmicity (Sleep), Rhythmicity (Eating), Rhythmicity (Daily Habits), Task Orientation), the interaction term between “Flexibility/Rigidity” and “Mood” (Flexibility/Rigidity x Mood), perceived maternal and parental rejection (PARQ (Mother), PARQ (Father)), recent stressful life events during adolescence (ASQ), and MDD/MDD comorbidity.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Among the 60 cases, 43 adolescents (71.7%) indicated presence of suicidal ideation with 41 of them (68.3%) having suicidal thoughts for at least once a week. 23 case adolescents (38.3%) reported themselves as active suicidal ideators with a specific plan. 41 adolescents (68.3%) also previously attempted self-harm. 29 (48.3%) of the case adolescents were first-time attempters while the remaining 31 (51.7%) were multiple attempters. None of the 58 control adolescents showed any presence of suicidal ideation or any attempts of self-harm and suicide. In addition, there were no differences in age (t(187) = 0.925, p = 0.356) and sex (X2(1, N = 189) = 1.58, p = 0.210) between the 71 adolescents who did not participate and the 118 adolescents who remained in the study. However, both groups significantly differed on ethnicities (X2(3, N = 189) = 12.7, p = 0.005) where the 71 adolescents who did not participate were more likely to be Chinese as compared to other ethnicities.

Risk factors of adolescent SAs

As detailed in Table 2, the average adolescent suicide attempter was significantly more likely to be psychiatrically diagnosed (t = -6.62, p < 0.001), with MDD alone (t = -6.64, p < 0.001) or comorbid with MDD (t = -7.03, p < 0.001), experience more stressful life events (t = -5.69, p < 0.001), more paternal (t = -2.79, p < 0.01) and maternal rejection (t = -4.06, p < 0.001). Compared to non-attempters, adolescent suicide attempters experienced significantly higher rates in nearly all disorders and recent stressors (all p < 0.05), except for bipolar disorder (t = -1.23, p = 0.001). Cases and Controls were not significantly different in any of the sociodemographic characteristics (all p > 0.05). In terms of temperament (Table 3), these cases scored significantly lower than controls on four “difficult” traits (Flexibility/Rigidity: t = 3.85, p < 0.001; Rhythmicity in Sleep: t = 2.98, p < 0.01, Rhythmicity in Eating: t = 1.92, p < 0.05; Mood: t = 4.88, p < 0.001; Approach/Withdrawal: t = 2.67, p < 0.001) and higher in Activity Level (General) (t = -1.63, p = 0.050).

Table 2 Differences in Clinical and Demographic Characteristics between adolescent suicide attempters and non-attempters
Table 3 Differences in Temperament Trait scores between adolescent suicide attempters and non-attempters

Odds ratios for adolescent suicide attempts

Table 4 displays the results of four binary logistic regression models with the “difficult” traits and the interaction term “Flexibility x Mood” as predictors of suicide attempts. The first model adjusted for demographic characteristics and the remaining traits, with the second, third, and fourth model additionally controlling for paternal and maternal rejection, stressful life events, and MDD comorbidity respectively. Since all but one adolescent in the control group met criteria for another psychiatric disorder (i.e., all except bipolar disorder) on top of MDD (Table 2), there was no point in controlling for MDD morbidity as a covariate. Therefore, the fourth model adjusted for MDD comorbidity together with the aforementioned factors. The fourth model, which controls all covariates, explained 59.1% (Nagelkerke R2 = 0.591) of the variance in SA and accurately classified 81.8% of the cases. Sensitivity and specificity of this model was 77.6% and 82.1% respectively. Across all four models, a unit decrease in “Mood” was significantly associated with a 12 − 18% (OR: 1.12–1.18, p = < 0.05) increase in the likelihood of suicide attempts. Even after controlling for all covariates, “Negative Mood” (OR: 1.18, p = < 0.05), MDD comorbidity (OR: 10.7, p = < 0.01), and stressful life events (OR: 1.02, p = < 0.05), remained as statistically significant predictors. Likewise, a unit increase in the interaction term “Flexibility x Mood” was significantly associated with a 5 − 6% decrease in the likelihood of suicide attempts (OR: 0.943 – 0.955, p = < 0.05) across all four models. This significant interaction was later probed at -1SD and + 1SD of the Flexibility/Rigidity (Adaptability) scores. Figure 2 visualised the gradual changes in the simple slopes after controlling for each additional risk factor. After adjustment of all temperament traits, demographic and risk factors, simple slope analyses revealed that “Positive Mood” significantly predicted lower probability of suicide attempts when “Adaptability” was high (b = − 0.288 – − 0.293, p < 0.05) but not low (b = − 0.003 – − 0.012, p > 0.87) (Model 4, Fig. 2).

Table 4 A logistic regression estimation results of adolescent’s temperaments with attempted suicide as a dependent variable, after controlling for individual differences
Fig. 2
figure 2

Probability of adolescent suicide attempts as a function of mood and flexibility/rigidity

Functions are graphed for two levels of flexibility/rigidity: 1 standard deviation above the mean and 1 standard deviation below the mean. F/R = Flexibility/Rigidity

Model 1 adjusted for all temperament traits and demographic variables

Model 2 adjusted for all temperament traits, demographic variables, PARQ (Mother) and PARQ (Father)

Model 3 adjusted for all temperament traits, demographic variables, PARQ (Mother) and PARQ (Father), and ASQ (Total)

Model 4 adjusted for all temperament traits, demographic variables, PARQ (Mother) and PARQ (Father), ASQ (Total), and MDD Comorbidity

Discussion

This study sets out to investigate three main aims. Besides greater perceived parental rejection, higher frequencies of stressful life events, psychiatric disorders, and MDD comorbidity, we expected Asian adolescent suicide attempters to fare significantly worse than non-attempters on four “difficult temperament” traits (i.e., except Approach/Withdrawal). Our first hypothesis was only partially supported, such that suicide attempters were also significantly less approachable than non-suicide attempters. This finding contradicts previous observations that a high withdrawal temperament was protective against suicidality-related stressors among Asian adolescents. There are two possible reasons. With globalisation, education systems, school cultures, and parenting styles in traditionally collectivistic societies have shifted towards emphasising individualism [59]. The most recent studies of urban adolescents in China suggested robust negative relations between “withdrawal” and maladjustment [60,61,62], comparable to the findings of Western individualistic samples [63]. Due to the likeness between urban China and contemporary Singapore culture [64], similar inferences may apply to these equally independent Singaporean adolescents. Researchers are encouraged to replicate this observation with adolescent samples in other conservative parts of Asia (e.g., Korea). Otherwise, it is plausible that all five “difficult temperament” traits can be overrepresented even among Asian adolescent suicide attempters. While highly withdrawn Asian adolescents were generally better adjusted in school and at home, they were nonetheless disadvantaged in formal situations whereby they hesitate to participate in organised learning opportunities, teenage romance, and resolving serious peer-to-peer conflicts [65]. Such negative experiences could precipitate a cascade of self-esteem or self-competence issues [66]. Competitiveness in the contemporary Asian societies may then aggravate the effects of these internalising problems [67], strengthening the predictability of SAs. Together, this study found that adolescent suicide attempters significantly differed from non-attempters in scores favouring all five “difficult temperament” traits.

Our second hypothesis that the “Low/Negative Mood” trait remained a robust, significant predictor of adolescent SA was supported. Our study extended earlier findings by adjusting for additional risk factors that are important during this vulnerable stage of development (i.e., perceived parental rejection, proximal stressful life events, MDD Comorbidity). Adolescents who experienced more frequent, negative emotions were also inherently less tolerable of them, putting them at risk of using non-suicidal self-injury or SA as a maladaptive emotion regulation strategy [68]. This increased risk was also independent of their psychiatric disorders or stressful life events [20]. Our final hypothesis that the interaction between “Mood” and “Flexibility (Adaptability)” may be a possible pathway of buffering against SAs was also supported. This finding mirrors previous studies which demonstrated that a “hyperthymic” temperament uniquely reduced the likelihood of SAs even after controlling for sociodemographic, internalising and externalising disorders. While we may infer that the interplay between “Positive Mood” and “High Adaptability” underlies the protective role of a hyperthymic temperament, future researchers may further validate this suggestion using confirmatory factor analyses. Neuroimaging studies have somewhat supported this finding by revealing that “Positive Mood” and “Adaptability” temperaments share biological correlates with the personality trait resilience developed at late adolescence or young adulthood [69]. Irrespective of flexibility levels, adolescents low in mood were significantly more likely to attempt suicide (Fig. 2), further justifying our earlier results on “Negative Mood” as a direct risk factor. Notably, recent stressful life events (OR: 1.02) or MDD comorbidity (OR: 10.2) also retained their significance in predicting adolescent SAs.

Implications

This study highlights the importance of early identification of temperament traits among Asian youths. Not only were the “difficult temperament” traits significantly overrepresented among suicide attempters in Asia, but also the temperament trait of “Negative Mood” should warrant greater attention as a robust predictor of adolescent SAs independent of other established high-risk factors. In a similar vein, our results on the interaction between “Positive Mood” and “High Flexibility” traits being a protective mechanism notably challenged previous speculations that a positive outlook alone was sufficient to reduce adolescent SAs. This finding is timely given that recent research was also identifying protective traits within the multifaceted hyperthymic temperament [36]. Yet, it may not be wise to proceed immediately with mandatory early screening of these “difficult” or “protective” traits, given that predictions of SAs have been dominated with false-positive and false-negative findings [70,71,72]. Practitioners are calling for a more measured and strategic approach to adolescent suicide prevention in order to minimise false-positive findings and maximise precision [73]. To do this, we suggest three possible directions that future researchers can undertake. One, future researchers should aim to converge on these findings even with employing different study designs, samples, and measurement types. For instance, using a 10-year prospective longitudinal study design with the administration of annual self-reports on Mexican-origin adolescents, Lawson et al. (2022) established consistent associations between the “Negative Emotionality” trait and active suicidal ideation and behaviours across their development from 13 to 22 years old [74]. Despite having administered a different temperament scale (i.e., Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire-Revised/EATQ-R; [75]), the “Negative Emotionality” subscale has shown good convergent validity with the “Negative Mood” trait assessed by the DOTS-R [76]. The confluence of both findings onto this “Negative Mood” trait gives confidence of its robustness in predicting suicidality-related behaviours among these adolescents regardless of culture. However, our last finding that the synergistic effects of high adaptability and positive mood temperaments were protective against adolescent SAs were not explicitly tested elsewhere. Without the support of other research methodologies, such as factor analyses to validate if these temperament traits truly underlie the protective “hyperthymic” profile, it may be ill-advised to proceed with screening for adolescents with high adaptability and emotionality and consider them to be “not at-risk” as this may result in false-negatives. Two, we suggest that collection of more neurobiological evidence (i.e., biological markers and correlates) linking adolescent temperaments and suicidal behaviours may be very helpful in objectively validating these connections. For example, few studies have already established strong relationships between different facets of a depressive temperament (e.g., fearfulness, pessimism; [77]), an impulsive temperament [78], and lower serotonergic activities, in which the latter was known to substantially increase the presence and lethality of SAs [79]. In a similar vein, we encourage biological researchers to test for common linkages between the neurobiology underlying a “hyperthymic” (or “High Flexibility” and “Positive Mood”) temperament and the protective neurobiological correlates of adolescent suicidal behaviours. Three, and in particular, adolescent suicide research still suffers from a serious lack of longitudinal data [74, 80]. This may be important considering that some developmental models of temperament and psychopathology (e.g., scar model; [81, 82]) postulated that experiencing suicidal ideation can lead to further changes in adolescent temperament. Longitudinal work that examines temperament development from an early age and all across adolescence may derive more insights on causality, pertaining to (i) whether temperament traits can lead to the onset of suicidal risk and if so, (ii) what are these traits and (iii) what were the contexts? We suggest that when there is sufficient research in these three directions, meaning that the developmental associations between these temperament traits and adolescent suicidal behaviours were consistent from a biopsychosocial perspective, there is greater conviction in temperament screening as an effective methodology in improving suicide prevention efforts (i.e., identifying true-positives while minimising false-positives). In this scenario, we may be more prepared to follow previous suggestions of implementing a collaborative network of school counsellors, paediatricians, and mental health care providers to partake in early screening of these “at-risk” temperaments [83]. Early identification of these traits could help to inform carefully tailored early interventions and prevent future SAs. For instance, to reduce direct influence of the “Negative Mood” trait on the likelihood of adolescent SAs, researchers and practitioners may consider the possibility of early CBT-skills training (e.g., cognitive restructuring) delivered in the form of youth-friendly psychoeducation for older children or early adolescents. This can help address cognitive vulnerabilities (i.e., hopelessness) that previously mediated the relationship between this temperament trait and adolescent SAs [84]. Such intervention has also worked well for “difficult” adolescents in coping with eating disorders [85]. In short, more research on intervention targets that mediate the effects of temperaments on SA may be important in tailoring effective early interventions [86]. Early detection of “at-risk” temperaments may also help in directing substantial support towards their parents as they raise these children/adolescents by means of increasing awareness of suicidal ideation and knowledge to mitigate suicidal risk before they intensify. The capacity to implement safety management procedures within the family can also greatly reduce the likelihood of a SA [87]. .

Limitations and future directions

Several limitations of the current study should be acknowledged. First, our findings must be interpreted with caution as adolescents’ self-reports of their temperament, psychiatric symptoms, recent stressful life events, and perceived parental rejection may have been subjected to different kinds of biases. For example, adolescents may have been influenced by negative state emotions induced by their present psychiatric conditions or unstable cognitions associated with their admission in an emergency setting. Such negative mood or cognition may bias the responses of these adolescents in unpredictable ways [88]. Adolescents who meet the symptom criteria for specific disorders such as substance use or schizophrenia may also be suffering from memory or cognitive deficits which can further hamper the accuracy of self-reports [89, 90]. Social desirability biases could have also led to the under-reporting of mental disorders or symptoms by the adolescents. Future studies are encouraged to either minimise the use of self-reports or administer some of these scales (i.e., DOTS-R, PARQ) together with the parent/guardian and align their reports with the adolescent’s in order to reduce response biases by these affected adolescents. To minimise mood-dependent recall biases, we also encourage researchers to carry out the measures outside of the emergency setting. To our knowledge, the most robust developmental relations between temperament and suicidal behaviours were derived using prospective longitudinal designs, where researchers diligently monitored semi-annual/annual self-reports or interviews of temperament, suicidal behaviours, and demographical variables starting from early adolescence to young adulthood (e.g., [74]). Such prospective longitudinal designs with multiple waves of data collected over the period of adolescence may be optimal in depicting the development of suicidal risk in relation to temperament traits across time and should be considered for future studies. Second, self-reported temperaments could have been the reflections of recent states rather than traits. Nevertheless, temperament traits as measured by the DOTS-R have previously demonstrated excellent stability from early childhood to late adolescence in longitudinal studies [91], while trained research assistants who were present to clarify any queries from the respondent(s) may favour the assessment of traits more than states [92]. Third, there exists structural limitations within the self-reports used to measure adolescents’ temperaments (i.e., DOTS-R) and behavioural disorders in youths (YI-4R). Though the nine dimensions of temperament assessed by the DOTS-R were extensive [93], however, previous factor analytic research found that they were not entirely independent of each other. Rothbart and Mauro (1990) observed that the definition of “Positive/Negative Mood” overlapped with the definition of “Approach/Withdrawal” [94]. This may limit the implications that can be drawn from our findings. For instance, we cannot be certain if the predictive strength of the “Negative Mood” trait was due to some elements of “Withdrawal” correlated with negative emotionality. Yet, for clarity, the DOTS-R was carefully selected for this study due to two features that may be lesser known for other temperament measures [95]. One, the DOTS-R was tested to be highly invariant across cultures, demonstrating the universality of these nine dimensions of temperament. These nine dimensions showed structural consistency across adolescents of different cultures and ethnic groups [96, 97]. Recently, this instrument demonstrated high internal and external validity among 775 Chinese adolescents and was suggested to be optimal for assessing Asian adolescents’ temperament profiles [98]. Two, the DOTS-R demonstrated long-term predictive associations with some internalising and externalising disorders (i.e., psychiatric and substance use disorders; [12, 99]. Therefore, some researchers considered this scale to be one of the very few highly validated instruments for at-risk adolescents. These researchers suggested that the DOTS-R may help to detect developmental deviance at an early age as its broad dimensions of temperament sufficiently describe emotional or behavioural patterns that can lead to clinically relevant symptoms [100]. Nonetheless, to obtain less ambiguous conclusions [101], future studies may utilise other temperament assessments like the EATQ-R [75] which empirically categorises temperament into three distinctive superordinate factors — positive emotionality, negative emotionality, and self-regulation [102]. Next, the YI-4 was preferred to other reliable diagnostic tools such as the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID) due to primary concerns about the availability of the research assistants (interviewers) and the parent(s)/guardian(s) of the subjects. For the interviewers, administration of the YI-4 does not require any formal training except to instruct the adolescents to self-report their symptoms. Parental reports are not needed, while adolescents’ self-reported symptom counts were reviewed by a senior psychiatrist to give the diagnoses [48]. These procedures were helpful for our research assistants who were already assisting in various projects at that time, and may have prevented further attrition from the sample if the parent(s)/guardian(s) were also required to participate in the interviews. Consequently, we acknowledge that the YI-4 self-reports may have been influenced by recall biases or mood-dependent response biases associated with the adolescents’ psychiatric conditions or their unstable cognitions in an emergency setting [88]. These biases may be reduced using structured diagnostic interviews such as MINI-KID, which aligns the responses of both adolescent and parent/guardian typically present during a MINI-KID interview [103].Furthermore, these responses are separately evaluated by an interviewer who has received formal training on basic knowledge of all psychiatric disorders and independent rating skills [103, 104]. For these benefits, future researchers are encouraged to adopt these alternative measures to derive more objective findings. Fourth, independent of the psychiatric conditions already assessed using the YI-4, neurodevelopmental disorders such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) which were heavily linked to suicidal behaviours were not evaluated and controlled for as covariates in the statistical models. This may have affected the validity of our findings and gives another reason why future researchers should consider structured clinical assessments such as the MINI-KID which comprehensively assesses all psychiatric and developmental disorders for children and adolescents. Fifth, our small sample size also did not allow for any meaningful comparisons between case adolescents with different history of suicidal behaviours (e.g., mild vs. active suicidal ideator, multiple attempters vs. single attempters) unlike other recent studies [105]. Future studies may wish to recruit larger samples and compare the differential associations between temperament and adolescent SA across suicide attempters with different backgrounds. Sixth, as the study team was mainly interested in exploring systematic differences between adolescent suicide attempters and healthy youths, there was no matching between cases and controls on the severity of medical conditions. It is possible that some of these cases suffered from more serious medical conditions than controls. Future researchers may match cases and controls based on similar medical conditions or control for these conditions as covariates to allow a cleaner comparison between both groups. Seventh, this study was conducted before the pandemic. Though temperament traits were broadly considered to be highly stable across time and that COVID-19 infections did not biologically affect early temperament development [106], this may nonetheless limit the applicability of our findings. For instance, stress and life disruptions associated with maternal COVID-19 infections may still result in maternal-rated changes in infant temperament at 6 months [107]. This may subsequently affect parental attitudes or upbringing towards these infants, which would then affect their development. Future researchers can attempt to replicate our findings during pandemic or even post-pandemic times. Eighth, there existed some difficulties in recruitment as all 59 adolescents who rejected the in-person interviews with the research assistants despite showing interest initially did not state any reasons for doing so. In view of the fact that suicidal behaviours are still heavily stigmatized, future studies may consider recruiting more mental health professionals to proactively assist with recruitment procedures. Ninth, demographics are limited to Singaporean youths, primarily at late adolescence, which limits the generalizability of our findings across all adolescents and those beyond Asia. Possibly, our individualistic, Singaporean sample may also not be representative of highly collectivistic Asian youths. Future studies may consider re-evaluating the relationship between temperaments and SAs with cross-cultural Asian youths. Lastly, the cross-sectional nature meant that we cannot conclude causality between the two. To validate these findings, researchers are highly encouraged to consider examining longitudinal associations between temperament and suicidal behaviours throughout adolescence (e.g., [74]).

Conclusion

Overall, this study examined difficult temperament traits, especially “Negative Mood” trait, to be indirect and direct risk factors respectively among Asian adolescent suicide attempters. This study also examined the interactive relationship between “Positive Mood” and “Adaptability” temperament traits as a possible protective pathway against adolescent SAs. Our results advocate for early temperament screening as an important means to adolescent suicide prevention. Though, more longitudinal and neurobiological research converging on these temperament findings may be necessary before ascertaining temperament screening as an effective methodology to improve suicide prevention efforts for adolescents.

Data availability

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author(s) on reasonable requests.

References

  1. Kwak CW, Ickovics JR. Adolescent suicide in South Korea: risk factors and proposed multi-dimensional solution. Asian J Psychiatry. 2019;43:150–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Liu XC, Chen H, Liu ZZ, Wang JY, Jia CX. Prevalence of suicidal behaviour and associated factors in a large sample of chinese adolescents. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2019;28(3):280–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Tanji F, Tomata Y, Zhang S, Otsuka T, Tsuji I. Psychological distress and completed suicide in Japan: a comparison of the impact of moderate and severe psychological distress. Prev Med. 2018;116:99–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Suicide Facts and Figures | Samaritans of Singapore (SOS). [Internet]. [cited 2022 Oct 8]. Available from: https://www.sos.org.sg/learn-about-suicide/quick-facts.

  5. Nock MK, Borges G, Bromet EJ, Cha CB, Kessler RC, Lee S. Suicide and suicidal behavior. Epidemiol Rev. 2008;30(1):133–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Nock MK, Green JG, Hwang I, McLaughlin KA, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, et al. Prevalence, correlates, and treatment of lifetime suicidal behavior among adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication adolescent supplement. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(3):300–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Newman DL, Silva PA. Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders: longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53(11):1033–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Nanayakkara S, Misch D, Chang L, Henry D. Depression and exposure to suicide predict suicide attempt. Depress Anxiety. 2013;30(10):991–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Bronisch T, Wittchen HU. Suicidal ideation and suicide attempts: comorbidity with depression, anxiety disorders, and substance abuse disorder. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;244(2):93–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Liu RT, Miller I. Life events and suicidal ideation and behavior: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2014;34(3):181–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Fotti SA, Katz LY, Afifi TO, Cox BJ. The associations between peer and parental relationships and suicidal behaviours in early adolescents. Can J Psychiatry. 2006;51(11):698–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Tubman JG, Windle M. Continuity of difficult temperament in adolescence: relations with depression, life events, family support, and substance use across a one-year period. J Youth Adolesc. 1995;24(2):133–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Clark LA, Watson D. Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100(3):316.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Wu TCH, Meehan AJ, Rijlaarsdam J, Maughan B, Fearon P, Barker ED. Developmental pathways from toddler difficult temperament to child generalized psychopathology and adult functioning. J Affect Disord. 2022;301:14–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Cameron JR. Parental treatment, children’s temperament, and the risk of childhood behavioral problems: 1. Relationships between parental characteristics and changes in children’s temperament over time. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1977;47(4):568.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Ladd GW, Kochenderfer-Ladd B, Eggum ND, Kochel KP, McConnell EM. Characterizing and comparing the friendships of anxious-solitary and unsociable preadolescents. Child Dev. 2011;82(5):1434–53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Chess S, Thomas A, Hassibi M. Depression in childhood and adolescence: a prospective study of six cases. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1983;171(7):411–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Akiskal HS, Akiskal KK, Haykal RF, Manning JS, Connor PD. TEMPS-A: progress towards validation of a self-rated clinical version of the temperament evaluation of the Memphis, Pisa, Paris, and San Diego Autoquestionnaire. J Affect Disord. 2005;85(1–2):3–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Vazquez GH, Gonda X, Lolich M, Tondo L, Baldessarini RJ. Suicidal risk and affective temperaments, evaluated with the TEMPS-A scale: a systematic review. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2018;26(1):8–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Yen S, Shea MT, Sanislow CA, Skodol AE, Grilo CM, Edelen MO, et al. Personality traits as prospective predictors of suicide attempts. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2009;120(3):222–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Brezo J, Paris J, Turecki G. Personality traits as correlates of suicidal ideation, suicide attempts, and suicide completions: a systematic review. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2006;113(3):180–206.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Rumble ME, Dickson D, McCall WV, Krystal AD, Case D, Rosenquist PB, et al. The relationship of person-specific eveningness chronotype, greater seasonality, and less rhythmicity to suicidal behavior: a literature review. J Affect Disord. 2018;227:721–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Pompili M, Iliceto P, Innamorati M, Rihmer Z, Lester D, Akiskal HS, et al. Suicide risk and personality traits in physically and/or sexually abused acute psychiatric inpatients: a preliminary study. Psychol Rep. 2009;105(2):554–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Pompili M, Innamorati M, Lester D, Akiskal HS, Rihmer Z, Casale AD, et al. Substance abuse, temperament and suicide risk: evidence from a case-control study. J Addict Dis. 2009;28(1):13–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Pompili M, Innamorati M, Rihmer Z, Gonda X, Serafini G, Akiskal H, et al. Cyclothymic–depressive–anxious temperament pattern is related to suicide risk in 346 patients with major mood disorders. J Affect Disord. 2012;136(3):405–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Pompili M, Rihmer Z, Akiskal H, Amore M, Gonda X, Innamorati M, et al. Temperaments mediate suicide risk and psychopathology among patients with bipolar disorders. Compr Psychiatry. 2012;53(3):280–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Pompili M, Innamorati M, Milelli M, Battuello M, Erbuto D, Lester D, et al. Temperaments in completed suicides: are they different from those in suicide attempters and controls? Compr Psychiatry. 2016;65:98–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Janiri D, De Rossi P, Kotzalidis GD, Girardi P, Koukopoulos AE, Reginaldi D, et al. Psychopathological characteristics and adverse childhood events are differentially associated with suicidal ideation and suicidal acts in mood disorders. Eur Psychiatry. 2018;53:31–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Choo C, Diederich J, Song I, Ho R. Cluster analysis reveals risk factors for repeated suicide attempts in a multi-ethnic asian population. Asian J Psychiatry. 2014;8:38–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Chen X, Hastings PD, Rubin KH, Chen H, Cen G, Stewart SL. Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in chinese and canadian toddlers: a cross-cultural study. Dev Psychol. 1998;34(4):677.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Chen X, Rubin K, Liu M, Chen H, Wang L, Li D, et al. Compliance in chinese and canadian toddlers: a cross-cultural study. Int J Behav Dev. 2003;27(5):428–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Chen X, Rubin KH, Li B, shu, Li D. Adolescent outcomes of social functioning in chinese children. Int J Behav Dev. 1999;23(1):199–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Keller H, Yovsi R, Borke J, Kärtner J, Jensen H, Papaligoura Z. Developmental consequences of early parenting experiences: self-recognition and self-regulation in three cultural communities. Child Dev. 2004;75(6):1745–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Chen X, Yang F, Fu R, Zentner M, Shiner RL. Culture and temperament. Handb Temperam. 2012;462–78.

  35. Rihmer A, Rozsa S, Rihmer Z, Gonda X, Akiskal KK, Akiskal HS. Affective temperaments, as measured by TEMPS-A, among nonviolent suicide attempters. J Affect Disord. 2009;116(1–2):18–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Karam EG, Itani L, Fayyad J, Hantouche E, Karam A, Mneimneh Z, et al. Temperament and suicide: a national study. J Affect Disord. 2015;184:123–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Baldessarini RJ, Vázquez GH, Tondo L. Affective temperaments and suicidal ideation and behavior in mood and anxiety disorder patients. J Affect Disord. 2016;198:78–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Joyce DK. Temperament-based learning styles of children with Conduct Disorder and Oppositional defiant disorder. University of Florida; 2000.

  39. Beach VL, Gissandaner TD, Schmidt AT. The UPPS model of impulsivity and suicide: a systematic literature review. Arch Suicide Res. 2022;26(3):985–1006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Sorte JG. The relationship of child temperament and maternal behavior to the child’s self-esteem. 1990.

  41. Posner K, Brown GK, Stanley B, Brent DA, Yershova KV, Oquendo MA, et al. The Columbia–Suicide severity rating scale: initial validity and internal consistency findings from three multisite studies with adolescents and adults. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(12):1266–77.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Stanley B, Brown GK. Safety planning intervention: a brief intervention to mitigate suicide risk. Cogn Behav Pract. 2012;19(2):256–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Madan A, Frueh BC, Allen JG, Ellis TE, Rufino KA, Oldham JM, et al. Psychometric reevaluation of the Columbia-Suicide severity rating scale: findings from a prospective, inpatient cohort of severely mentally ill adults. J Clin Psychiatry. 2016;77(7):16125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Windle M, Lerner RM. Reassessing the dimensions of temperamental individuality across the life span: the revised dimensions of temperament survey (DOTS-R). J Adolesc Res. 1986;1(2):213–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Goldsmith HH, Rieser-Danner LA, Briggs S. Evaluating convergent and discriminant validity of temperament questionnaires for preschoolers, toddlers, and infants. Dev Psychol. 1991;27(4):566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Windle M. Temperament and personality: an exploratory interinventory study of the DOTS-R, EASI-II and EPI. J Pers Assess. 1989;53(3):487–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Luby JL, Steiner H. Concordance of parent-child temperament ratings in a clinical sample of adolescent girls. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 1993;23:297–305.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Gadow KD, Sprafkin J, Carlson GA, Schneider J, Nolan EE, Mattison RE, et al. A DSM-IV–referenced, adolescent self-report rating scale. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(6):671–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Ivanova MY, Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA, Dumenci L, Almqvist F, Bilenberg N, et al. The generalizability of the Youth Self-Report syndrome structure in 23 societies. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2007;75(5):729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Ang X. Positive psychology intervention for girls with conduct problems: a single-case time-series design. The University of Alabama; 2016.

  51. Byrne DG, Davenport SC, Mazanov J. Profiles of adolescent stress: the development of the adolescent stress questionnaire (ASQ). J Adolesc. 2007;30(3):393–416.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. De Vriendt T, Clays E, Moreno LA, Bergman P, Vicente-Rodriguez G, Nagy E, et al. Reliability and validity of the adolescent stress questionnaire in a sample of european adolescents-the HELENA study. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Darviri C, Legaki PE, Chatzioannidou P, Gnardellis C, Kraniotou C, Tigani X, et al. Adolescent stress questionnaire: reliability and validity of the greek version and its description in a sample of high school (lyceum) students. J Adolesc. 2014;37(8):1373–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. McKay MT, Percy A, Byrne DG. Support for the multidimensional adolescent stress questionnaire in a sample of adolescents in the United Kingdom. Stress Health. 2016;32(1):12–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Ye T, Cui N, Yang W, Liu J. Evaluation of the factor structure of the adolescent stress questionnaire in chinese adolescents. Psychol Rep. 2019;122(6):2366–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Rohner RP, Khaleque A. Parental acceptance-rejection questionnaire (PARQ): test manual. Handb Study Parent Accept Rejection. 2005;4:43–106.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Rohner RP, Lansford JE. Deep structure of the human affectional system: introduction to interpersonal acceptance–rejection theory. J Fam Theory Rev. 2017;9(4):426–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Field A. Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. sage; 2013.

  59. Chen X, Cen G, Li D, He Y. Social functioning and adjustment in chinese children: the imprint of historical time. Child Dev. 2005;76(1):182–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Coplan RJ, Liu J, Cao J, Chen X, Li D. Shyness and school adjustment in chinese children: the roles of teachers and peers. Sch Psychol Q. 2017;32(1):131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Zhang L, Eggum-Wilkens ND. Unsociability in chinese adolescents: cross-informant agreement and relations with social and school adjustment. Soc Dev. 2018;27(3):555–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. An D, Eggum-Wilkens ND. Do cultural orientations moderate the relation between chinese adolescents’ shyness and depressive symptoms? It depends on their academic achievement. Soc Dev. 2019;28(4):908–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Liu J, Chen X, Coplan RJ, Ding X, Zarbatany L, Ellis W. Shyness and unsociability and their relations with adjustment in chinese and canadian children. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2015;46(3):371–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Wang TY, Liu H. An emerging asian model of governance and transnational knowledge transfer: an introduction. An emerging asian model of governance and transnational knowledge transfer. Routledge; 2020. 1–15.

  65. An D, Eggum-Wilkens ND. Situation-specific shyness among chinese adolescents: measurement and associations with adjustment. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2021;74:101278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Schmidt LA, Robinson TN Jr. Low self-esteem in differentiating fearful and self-conscious forms of shyness. Psychol Rep. 1992;70(1):255–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Wee SX, Cheng CY, Choi H, Goh C. Toxic effect of fear of losing out on self-esteem: a moderated mediation model of conformity and need for cognitive closure in Singapore. Asian J Soc Psychol. 2022;25(4):773–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Nicolai KA, Wielgus MD, Mezulis A. Identifying risk for self-harm: rumination and negative affectivity in the prospective prediction of nonsuicidal self-injury. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2016;46(2):223–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Rutten BP, Hammels C, Geschwind N, Menne-Lothmann C, Pishva E, Schruers K, et al. Resilience in mental health: linking psychological and neurobiological perspectives. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2013;128(1):3–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Conrad R, Walz F, Geiser F, Imbierowicz K, Liedtke R, Wegener I. Temperament and character personality profile in relation to suicidal ideation and suicide attempts in major depressed patients. Psychiatry Res. 2009;170(2–3):212–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Ekinci O, Albayrak Y, Ekinci AE. Temperament and character in euthymic major depressive disorder patients: the effect of previous suicide attempts and psychotic mood episodes. Psychiatry Investig. 2012;9(2):119–26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Petek Erić A, Erić I, Ćurković M, Dodig-Ćurković K, Kralik K, Kovač V, et al. The temperament and character traits in patients with major depressive disorder and bipolar affective disorder with and without suicide attempt. Psychiatr Danub. 2017;29(2):171–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Pena JB, Caine ED. Screening as an approach for adolescent suicide prevention. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2006;36(6):614–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Lawson KM, Kellerman JK, Kleiman EM, Bleidorn W, Hopwood CJ, Robins RW. The role of temperament in the onset of suicidal ideation and behaviors across adolescence: findings from a 10-year longitudinal study of mexican-origin youth. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2022;122(1):171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Ellis LK, Rothbart MK. Revision of the early adolescent temperament questionnaire. Poster presented at the 2001 biennial meeting of the society for research in child development, Minneapolis, Minnesota. Citeseer; 2001.

  76. Capaldi DM, Rothbart MK. Development and validation of an early adolescent temperament measure. J Early Adolesc. 1992;12(2):153–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Van Heeringen C, Audenaert K, Van Laere K, Dumont F, Slegers G, Mertens J, et al. Prefrontal 5-HT2a receptor binding index, hopelessness and personality characteristics in attempted suicide. J Affect Disord. 2003;74(2):149–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Nielsen DA, Goldman D, Virkkunen M, Tokola R, Rawlings R, Linnoila M. Suicidality and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration associated with a tryptophan hydroxylase polymorphism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51(1):34–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. van Heeringen K, Mann JJ. The neurobiology of suicide. Lancet Psychiatry. 2014;1(1):63–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Franklin JC, Ribeiro JD, Fox KR, Bentley KH, Kleiman EM, Huang X, et al. Risk factors for suicidal thoughts and behaviors: a meta-analysis of 50 years of research. Psychol Bull. 2017;143(2):187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Tackett JL. Evaluating models of the personality–psychopathology relationship in children and adolescents. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26(5):584–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Durbin CE, Hicks BM. Personality and psychopathology: a stagnant field in need of development. Eur J Personal. 2014;28(4):362–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Frick PJ. Integrating research on temperament and childhood psychopathology: its pitfalls and promise. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2004;33(1):2–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Rosellini AJ, Bagge CL. Temperament, hopelessness, and attempted suicide: direct and indirect effects. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2014;44(4):353–61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Allen KL, Mountford VA, Elwyn R, Flynn M, Fursland A, Obeid N, et al. A framework for conceptualising early intervention for eating disorders. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2023;31(2):320–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Toh SH, Kanter JW, Keenan ME, Berlin KS. Testing Functional Analytic Psychotherapy’s mediational model of change in social connectedness for people with fear of intimacy. J Context Behav Sci. 2022;24:18–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Chen YY, Chien-Chang Wu K, Yousuf S, Yip PS. Suicide in Asia: opportunities and challenges. Epidemiol Rev. 2012;34(1):129–44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Hanita M. Self-report measures of patient utility: should we trust them? J Clin Epidemiol. 2000;53(5):469–76.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Bruffaerts R, Sabbe M, Demyttenaere K. Predicting community tenure in patients with recurrent utilization of a psychiatric emergency service. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2005;27(4):269–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Chiu CY, Liu CC, Hwang TJ, Hwu HG, Hua MS. Metamemory in patients with schizophrenia measured by the feeling of knowing. Psychiatry Res. 2015;230(2):511–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Windle M, Windle RC. Adolescent temperament and lifetime psychiatric and substance abuse disorders assessed in young adulthood. Personal Individ Differ. 2006;41(1):15–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Karam EG, Mneimneh ZN, Salamoun MM, Akiskal HS, Akiskal KK. Suitability of the TEMPS-A for population-based studies: ease of administration and stability of affective temperaments in its lebanese version. J Affect Disord. 2007;98(1–2):45–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Thomas A, Chess S. Temperament and development. Brunner/Mazel; 1977.

  94. Rothbart MK, Mauro JA. Questionnaire approaches to the study of infant temperament. Individ Differ Infancy Reliab Stab Predict. 1990;411–29.

  95. Kiuru N, Hirvonen R, Ahonen T. Assessing temperament among finnish early adolescents and their parents: psychometric properties of the short forms of the temperament questionnaires. Merrill-Palmer Q. 2019;65(3):294–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Windle M, Iwawaki S, Lerner RM. Cross-cultural comparability of temperament among japanese and american preschool children. Int J Psychol. 1988;23(1–6):547–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Cloninger CR. Temperament and personality. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1994;4(2):266–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Chen C, Zhang LF. Temperament, personality and achievement goals among chinese adolescent students. Educ Psychol. 2011;31(3):339–59.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  99. Shaw RJ, Steiner H. Temperament in juvenile eating disorders. Psychosomatics. 1997;38(2):126–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Díaz-Caneja CM, Morón-Nozaleda MG, Vicente-Moreno RP, Rodríguez-Toscano E, Pina-Camacho L, de la Serna E, et al. Temperament in child and adolescent offspring of patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2018;27:1459–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Latham MD, Dudgeon P, Yap MB, Simmons JG, Byrne ML, Schwartz OS, et al. Factor structure of the early adolescent temperament questionnaire–revised. Assessment. 2020;27(7):1547–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Rothbart MK. Temperament in childhood: a framework. Temperam Child. 1989;5973.

  103. Sheehan DV, Sheehan KH, Shytle RD, Janavs J, Bannon Y, Rogers JE, et al. Reliability and validity of the mini international neuropsychiatric interview for children and adolescents (MINI-KID). J Clin Psychiatry. 2010;71(3):17393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Högberg C, Billstedt E, Björck C, Björck PO, Ehlers S, Gustle LH, et al. Diagnostic validity of the MINI-KID disorder classifications in specialized child and adolescent psychiatric outpatient clinics in Sweden. BMC Psychiatry. 2019;19(1):1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Choo CC, Harris KM, Chew PK, Ho RC. Clinical assessment of suicide risk and suicide attempters’ self-reported suicide intent: a cross sectional study. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(7):e0217613.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  106. Fiske A, Scerif G, Holmboe K. Maternal depressive symptoms and early childhood temperament before and during the COVID-19 pandemic in the United Kingdom. Infant Child Dev. 2022;31(6):e2354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  107. Bianco C, Sania A, Kyle MH, Beebe B, Barbosa J, Bence M, et al. Pandemic beyond the virus: maternal COVID-related postnatal stress is associated with infant temperament. Pediatr Res. 2023;93(1):253–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge Professor Tan Chay Hoon (NUH, Pharmacology Department), Ms Pooi Khay Chong (Research Coordinator) and Dr Adrian Loh (Recruitment, Data collection, IMH) for their contributions in the duration of the study project. In addition, the authors would like to thank Dr. Shen Liang (NUS, Medicine Biostatistics Unit) and Ms Yap Qai Ven (NUS, Medicine Biostatistics Unit) for consulting on the interpretation of the results.

Finally, the authors would like to express their fullest gratitude to the Ministry of Social and Family Development for funding this research study, the patients and their family members for their participation, and the National University Hospital’s Emergency Medicine Department and REACH (West) for their assistance in participant recruitment.

Funding

The Singapore Ministry of Community Development, Youth and Sports Family Research Fund awarded the study grant (grant number 2010-3).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

S.H.Y. Toh designed the research study, with support from M.J.S. Wan and J.C.M. Wong. N. Nyein collected all relevant data with supervision from J.C.M. Wong. S.H.Y. Toh led the manuscript development and direction with substantial support from J.C.M. Wong and M.J.S. Wan. S.H.Y. Toh designed the figures, analyzed all data, and interpreted the full results in consultation with L.M. Kroneman and J.C.M. Wong. All authors made substantive suggestions for revision. All authors approved the final submitted version.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sean H.Y. Toh.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the National Healthcare Group (NHG) Domain Specific Review Board (DSRB) under the reference code “2010/00567”. All authors confirmed that all methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Written Informed Consent was obtained from all the subjects before the interview. As the subjects were under 18, written informed consent was obtained from the father or mother of participants in a face-to-face position after explaining the purpose of the study. Consent was given prior to commencing study procedures.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Conflict of interests

All authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Toh, S.H., Wan, M.J., Kroneman, L.M. et al. Temperament and adolescent suicide attempts: a case-control study with multi-ethnic Asian adolescents. BMC Psychiatry 23, 434 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-04914-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-04914-6

Keywords