Skip to main content

Dysfunctional self-reported interoception predicts residual symptom burden of fatigue in major depressive disorder: an observational study

Abstract

Background

Fatigue is a core symptom of major depressive disorder (MDD) and is frequently refractory to antidepressant treatment, leading to unfavorable clinical/psychosocial outcomes. Dysfunctional self-reported interoception (i.e., maladaptive focus on the body’s physiological condition) is prevalent in MDD and could contribute to residual symptom burden of fatigue. Therefore, we explored (a.) cross-sectional correlations between both dimensions and investigated (b.) prospective associations between interoceptive impairments at admission and symptom severity of fatigue at the end of hospitalization.

Methods

This observational, exploratory study included 87 patients suffering from MDD who completed self-rating scales, the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, Version 2 (MAIA-2), and the Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI-20), at the beginning and end of hospitalization. Bivariate correlations (r) and hierarchical regression analyses were performed.

Results

The cross-sectional analysis showed moderate to large negative correlations between the MAIA-2 and MFI-20 dimensions except for the Not-Distracting scale. Symptoms of general, physical, and mental fatigue at the end of hospitalization were predicted by reduced body Trusting (β = -.31, p = .01; β = -.28, p = .02; β = -.31, p = .00, respectively). Increased Body Listening (β = .37, p = .00), Not-Worrying (β = .26, p = .02), and diminished Attention Regulation (β = -.32, p = .01) predicted higher mental fatigue.

Conclusions

Diminished body confidence at baseline identified patients at risk for post-treatment fatigue and could therefore serve as a target for improving antidepressant therapy. Body-centered, integrative approaches could address treatment-resistant fatigue in MDD. However, clinicians may also consider the potential adverse effect of increased Body Listening and Not-Worrying on mental fatigue in psychotherapeutic and counselling approaches. Due to the exploratory nature of this study, the results are preliminary and need to be replicated in pre-registered trials with larger sample sizes.

Peer Review reports

Background

Fatigue is a core symptom of major depressive disorder (MDD) [1, 2] which includes “physical (e.g., reduced activity, low energy, tiredness, decreased physical endurance, increased effort with physical tasks and with overcoming inactivity, general weakness, heaviness, slowness or sluggishness, nonrestorative sleep, and sleepiness); cognitive (e.g., decreased concentration, decreased attention, decreased mental endurance, and slowed thinking); and emotional dimensions (e.g., decreased motivation or initiative, decreased interest, feeling overwhelmed, feeling bored, aversion to effort, and feeling low)” (p. 186) [3]. Cumulative evidence points to a high prevalence of fatigue in MDD, even in patients who respond to treatment, because energy-related symptoms are poorly addressed by antidepressants [4, 5]. Additionally, fatigue is a common adverse effect of antidepressants [6]. Symptoms of fatigue typically occur in the prodromal, acute, and residual phase of MDD but are considerably underrecognized despite their clinical and psychosocial relevance [7]. Prospective studies have shown the long-term detrimental effects of residual symptoms of fatigue following treatment which predict chronicity, faster relapse, or a recurrent course of depression [8, 9], diminished psychosocial and vocational functioning [10], suicidal ideation/attempts [11], and increased healthcare utilization [11]. Therefore, there is a clinical need to address the extent of fatigue during the initial treatment of MDD, to identify targets for new interventions, and to characterize patients under risk [5].

Interoception involves the sensation, interpretation, and integration of signals representing the physiological condition of the entire body [12,13,14] as well as subjective beliefs (i.e., top-down predictions) about homeostatic states [15]. Interoception is primarily involved in the afferent part of homeostatic/allostatic feedback loops that are inextricably linked to urges and motivational behavior [16], e.g. dehydration triggers thirst, which motivates water seeking and encourages water drinking. Beyond physiological regulation, a growing body of research has shown that bodily sensations contribute to affect, cognition, and social functioning [16,17,18]. The continuous integration of interoceptive information from the entire body has been essentially linked to self-awareness, which is underpinned by a “common sensation” (German Gemeingefühl) of bodily feelings that are existentially important to human subjectivity [12, 13, 19]. The sensation of physiological changes is a central component of influential theories of emotion [20,21,22,23], and impairments in interoceptive processing have been identified as correlates of numerous mental disorders and psychosomatic disturbances [14, 24]. A state of dysfunctional interoception is also a typical characteristic of MDD, emphasizing its importance for the study of mood disorders [25,26,27,28,29,30,31].

Several facets of interoception have been distinguished [32, 33], one of these is cardiac interoceptive accuracy which is commonly assessed by the heartbeat counting task (but note the critical discussion on its construct validity [34,35,36]). Using this task in depression research, cumulative evidence points to blunted heartbeat perception accuracy in moderately depressed persons compared to healthy controls [25,26,27]. In the present study, we refer to another facet of interoception, i.e. self-reported interoception, which comprises an individual’s disposition to be focused on interoceptive states (i.e., bodily feelings) by distinguishing between clinically maladaptive and beneficial attention styles toward the body including their regulative consequences [37, 38]. In the tradition of phenomenological psychopathology, depression (latin deprimere, to press down) is described as a primarily bodily experienced disorder characterized by somatic feelings such as pain, numbness, flu-like symptoms, heaviness, constrictions in parts of the body, oppressions diffusely extending over the whole body (e.g., globus feeling in the throat, armor vest or tire feeling around the chest, pressure in the head, leaden paralysis, diffuse anxiety), or rigidity aggravating to stupor [39, 40]. Hence, the lived body becomes a resistant material corpse in depression – an interoceptive process which has been termed “corporealization of the lived body” [41]. Previous research has demonstrated the clinical relevance of abnormal self-reported interoception as an independent predictor of residual symptoms of depression after hospital treatment-as-usual [28] and of overall treatment response [42].

Fatigue has been classified as a non-painful interoceptive feeling and ascribed to a state of dysfunctional interoception in MDD [14]. In a recent theory of fatigue, Stephan et al. conceptualized fatigue as an interoceptive response to a state of dyshomeostasis by referring to a hierarchical Bayesian framework [43]. Following this approach, dyshomeostasis involves a discrepancy between sensed (i.e., afferent inputs from viscera) and expected (i.e., homeostatic setpoints) physiological states over a prolonged period, leading to the occurrence of interoceptive prediction errors that signal failed efforts to regulate the internal milieu and thus could trigger the feeling of fatigue [43]. These mechanisms could explain the difference between fatigue and tiredness, the latter being seen as a deviation from homeostatic setpoints (e.g., elevated lactic acid concentrations after physical activity), which is successfully resolved by rest [43].

Previous studies investigating fatigue experience in MDD have primarily focused on monoamine neurotransmitters, neuronal circuits of energy-related systems, and on inflammatory processes, but without significant clinical consequences (for a review, see [3]). To the best of our knowledge, despite the theoretical proximity between interoception and fatigue [14, 43], associations between dysfunctional interoception and fatigue in depression have not been studied. Therefore, we sought to (1.) explore cross-sectional correlations between self-reported interoception and multidimensional fatigue, and (2.) to investigate prospective associations between interoceptive dysfunctions at admission and the occurrence of fatigue symptoms at hospital discharge in inpatients suffering from MDD.

Methods

The study was approved by the ethics committee of Ulm University (registration number: 13/17). We followed the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and obtained patient’s written informed consent.

Procedure and participants

This observational, exploratory study involved a secondary analysis of data which were gathered in a longitudinal, naturalistic trial investigating the effects of self-reported interoception on treatment-as-usual outcomes in hospitalized patients suffering from MDD. Details on the study design, procedure of participant recruitment, inclusion/exclusion criteria, a study flow chart, and a synopsis of treatment components have been reported in the companion paper, which investigated interoceptive predictors of overall change in depression severity over the course of inpatient treatment [28]. A total of 87 patients who have been consecutively admitted to the depression ward of the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy I (Ulm University, Center for Psychiatry Südwürttemberg, Weißenau, Germany) were included. The diagnosis of MDD was assessed by trained clinicians according to ICD-10 criteria [2]. Questionnaire data were collected within 48 h of hospital admission/discharge. Patients underwent guideline-based treatment-as-usual including psychotropic drugs, cognitive behavioral psychotherapy, depression-focused behavioral group therapy, mental health care nursing interventions (e.g., crisis intervention, progressive muscle relaxation according to Jacobson), dance movement therapy, physical activity and occupational therapy [28].

Measures

Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, Version 2 (MAIA-2)

The MAIA-2 [42, 44] is a self-administered questionnaire in the paper-and-pencil mode including 37 items to assess self-reported interoception on eight scales (brief scale descriptions, an item example, and internal consistency reliabilities for the present study are reported in brackets): 1.) Noticing (“awareness of uncomfortable, comfortable, and neutral body sensations”, e.g., “When I am tense I notice where the tension is located in my body”, McDonald’s ωpre = .58, ωpost = .73); 2.) Not-Distracting (“tendency not to ignore or distract oneself from sensations of pain or discomfort”, e.g., “I try to ignore pain”, ωpre = .67, ωpost = .70); 3.) Not-Worrying (“tendency not to worry or experience emotional distress with sensations of pain or discomfort”, e.g., “I can stay calm and not worry when I have feelings of discomfort or pain”, ωpre = .67, ωpost = .70); 4.) Attention Regulation (“ability to sustain and control attention to body sensations”, e.g., “I can maintain awareness of my inner bodily sensations even when there is a lot going on around me”, ωpre = .87, ωpost = .88); 5.) Emotional Awareness (“awareness of the connection between body sensations and emotional states”, e.g., “I notice that my body feels different after a peaceful experience”, ωpre = .86, ωpost = .88); 6.) Self-Regulation (“ability to regulate distress by attention to body sensations”, e.g., “I can use my breath to reduce tension”, ωpre = .76, ωpost = .84); 7.) Body Listening (“active listening to the body for insight”, e.g., “I listen to my body to inform me about what to do”, ωpre = .77, ωpost = .84); 8.) Trusting (“experience of one’s body as safe and trustworthy”, e.g., “I am at home in my body”, ωpre = .88, ωpost = .90). Analogous to Cronbach's α, the common rule of thumb cut-off value (ω ≥ .70) was considered adequate [45]. Participants were asked to circle one number on each item which applies to the current state (0 = never; 5 = always). Item scores (range: 0 = “never”; 5 = “always”) were averaged for each dimension taking into account reverse-scoring [42, 44].

Previous studies reported an eight-factor structure for MAIA and its revision MAIA-2, appropriate convergent/discriminant validity, and adequate internal consistency reliability except for two subscales (Noticing and Not-Worrying) [44, 46]. The instrument was also validated in a clinically depressed sample and showed good criterion validity to distinguish between treatment response groups [42]. A major strength of the MAIA-2 is its ability to differentiate between clinically maladaptive and beneficial attention styles towards interoceptive cues, except for the Noticing scale which is a unipolar measure of body awareness [37]. According to Mehling, maladaptive interoceptive attention, which is most evident in somatization or health anxiety, refers to a person’s dispositional tendency to focus on unpleasant body sensations in an anxious and hypervigilant manner associated with affective fragility [37]. For example, pain research has shown that pain-related anxiety and pain catastrophizing mediate the association between emotional instability and hypervigilance to pain, which in turn predicts increased pain intensity [47]. The initial conceptualization that low scores on the MAIA subscales correspond to maladaptive states of interoceptive attention [46] has recently been challenged by preliminary evidence showing that increased tendencies to Body Listening may also be clinically detrimental in some cases [48] (for further details, please see discussion below).

Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI-20)

The assessment of fatigue is limited to the reliance on validated self-report questionnaires either following an unidimensional or multidimensional approach that take into account physical, affective, and cognitive aspects [49]. The MFI-20 [49, 50] is a self-administered questionnaire in the paper-and-pencil mode including 20 items to assess subjective experience of multidimensional fatigue on five scales (brief scale descriptions, an item example, and internal consistency reliabilities for the present study are reported in brackets): 1.) General Fatigue (a person’s general expression about his/her functioning, e.g., “I feel fit”, ωpre = .61, ωpost = .86); 2.) Physical Fatigue (somatic sensations which are related to tiredness, e.g., “Physically I feel I am in an excellent condition”, ωpre = .79, ωpost = .88); 3.) Mental Fatigue (mental symptoms of fatigue such as lack of concentration and focus, e.g., “I can concentrate well”, ωpre = .77, ωpost = .87); 4.) Reduced Activity (reduction in daily activity, e.g., “I think I do a lot in a day”, ωpre = .81, ωpost = .87); 5.) Reduced Motivation (reduced motivation to start an activity, e.g., “I have a lot of plans”, ωpre = .69, ωpost = .75). Participants were asked to check one box on each item which applies to the current state (1 = yes, that is true; 5 = no, that is not true). Item scores (range: 1 = “yes, that is true”; 5 = “no, that is not true”) were summed up for each dimension taking into account reverse-scoring [49]. High scale scores indicate dimensional symptom burden of fatigue. Previous research has demonstrated the factorial validity of the MFI-20, construct and convergent validity, and good internal consistency reliability in a clinical sample [49].

Data analysis

The statistical analyses were performed in R 4.1.2 [51] using the packages car 3.0–12, lm.beta 1.5–1, MBESS 4.8.1, psych 2.1.9, tidyverse 1.3.1. The prevalence of post-treatment fatigue of any severity was estimated by referring to minimal clinically important difference scores, which yielded a two-point difference for each MFI-20 scale in a previous study (minimum sum-score = 4; cutoff: sum-score ≥ 6) [52]. Pearson’s product-moment correlations between self-reported interoception and multidimensional fatigue were investigated at two time points: a.) prior to (pre-treatment) and b.) at the end of hospital treatment (post-treatment). Multivariate associations were tested in a hierarchical regression analysis including two steps to determine the individual model contribution of self-reported interoception (block 1: inclusion of age, sex, body mass index, treatment duration, somatic comorbidity, antidepressant use, and pre-treatment fatigue; block 2: inclusion of MAIA-2 scales). Sociodemographic and clinical covariates were included according to general recommendations for observational studies [53] to account for potential confounding or suppression effects and for relevant predictors that have been shown to affect residual fatigue [5, 54]. The need to consider confounders when using the MAIA-2 has been discussed elsewhere [28, 55]. Multicollinearity was not detected (variance inflation factor for all predictors: < 5). The significance level was a priori set to α = .05. On the basis of previous research [28], G*Power 3.1.9.2 [56] was used to estimate the post hoc minimum sample size for hierarchical regression analysis which resulted in N = 85 (Cohen’s f2 = .20 [moderate effect], α = .05, 1-β = .80, number of tested predictors = 8; total number of predictors = 17).

Results

Participant characteristics are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Characteristics of included participants (N = 87)

During the course of treatment, the severity of fatigue decreased significantly in all dimensions of the MFI-20: General Fatigue, mean change ΔM (T0-T1) = -4.72 (95% confidence interval [CI] -5.60, -3.85), t(86) = -10.76, p < .01, Cohen’s d = -1.15; Physical Fatigue, ΔM = -4.26 (95% CI -5.15, -3.83), t(86) = -9.62, p < .01, d = -1.03; Mental Fatigue, ΔM = -4.57 (95% CI -5.41, -3.74), t(86) = -10.93, p < .01, d = -1.17; Reduced Motivation, ΔM = -4.56 (95% CI -5.34, -3.75), t(86) = -11.14, p < .01, d = -1.19; Reduced Activity, ΔM = -4.87 (95% CI -5.85, -3.89), t(86) = -9.87, p < .01, d = -1.06. At the end of hospital treatment, the prevalence of patients reporting fatigue of any severity on each dimension was: General Fatigue 89.66% (N = 78); Physical Fatigue 89.66% (N = 78); Mental Fatigue 91.95% (N = 80); Reduced Motivation 88.51% (N = 77); Reduced Activity 86.21% (N = 75). Besides, significant improvements were detected for all the MAIA-2 subscales, which were reported in the companion paper [28].

Table 2 shows main findings for the bivariate associations between self-reported interoception and multidimensional fatigue. Moderate to large negative cross-sectional correlations were found between the dimensions of MAIA-2 and MFI-20 except for the Not-Distracting scale. The correlation coefficients were consistently higher for the post-treatment condition compared to pre-treatment states.

Table 2 Correlations between self-reported interoception and multidimensional fatigue (N = 87)

In the multivariate analysis, symptoms of general, physical, and mental fatigue were prospectively predicted by baseline impairments in self-reported interoception (Table 3). Specifically, diminished Trusting in the body was associated with post-treatment symptoms of general, physical, and mental fatigue. Mental fatigue was also predicted by reduced capability to maintain attention to body sensations, the tendency towards a less worrying self-focus, and increased tendencies to listen to body sensations for insight. The MFI-20 dimensions Reduced Motivation and Reduced Activity were not predicted by baseline self-reported interoception. Longer duration of hospital treatment predicted reduced motivation at the end of treatment.

Table 3 Prediction of posttreatment residual fatigue by pretreatment self-reported interoception (N = 87)

Discussion

In the present study, we found moderate to large correlations between self-reported interoception and burden of fatigue in depressed patients. Additionally, we have identified prospective predictors of post-treatment fatigue in a guideline-based therapy of MDD, which may help characterize patients at risk and improve current treatments. The main findings are discussed in the following sections.

First, the hypothesized associations between the experience of fatigue and facets of self-reported interoception [14, 43] have been confirmed by the cross-sectional analysis. We found moderate to large correlations which pointed in the expected directions, suggesting more dysfunctional bodily self-focus in patients reporting higher fatigue burden. The results also showed links between abnormal interoception and facets of fatigue that go beyond the physical dimensions, such as mental fatigue or motivation loss, which may be understood against the background of the ecological embodiment paradigm that embeds mental processes and psychopathology in sensorimotor experiences [57]. Our findings are consistent with a growing body of research implicating a reconceptualization of depression as a dysfunctional response to impaired interoception [26,27,28, 39, 41, 42, 58,59,60], which also challenges the prevailing assumptions about depression as a “brain disorder” [61]. Although the pathophysiology of fatigue has been associated with a state of dyshomeostasis that manifests as systemic low-grade inflammation [62], there is preliminary evidence against an inflammatory involvement in the etiology of dysfunctional self-reported interoception [55]. Research into the causal factors of disturbed interoception is still in its infancy [14].

Besides, we found a consistent pattern showing stronger post-treatment correlations than for pre-treatment. Following Goodwin & Leech [63], several factors affect the magnitude of Pearson’s r which will be briefly discussed: a) “Is there a lack of variability in the data?” (p. 263–264). This is evident for the MFI-20 scales General Fatigue, Physical Fatigue, and Mental Fatigue, which show restricted ranges (minimum scorepre > minimum scorepost) and smaller pre-treatment standard deviations. The increase in variance (contributing to covariance and the magnitude of correlation coefficient) is consequently explained by significant reductions in fatigue severity over the course of treatment in some patients, whereas others showed relatively high fatigue burden. In contrast, the MAIA-2 scales are more homogenous over both time points regarding their variability. These patterns are also observed in the scatterplots. b) “Do the marginal distributions have dissimilar shapes?” (p. 264). The skewness’ of the variables included in the correlational analysis are excellent (± 1.00) except for Emotional Awarenesspost (skewness = -1.11; acceptable). Thus, difference between distributional shapes may not explain pre/post variability in r. c) “Is there a nonlinear or curvilinear relationship between the two variables? (p. 264). We checked scatterplots for all bivariate pairs and found no deviations from linearity. d) “Are there one or more outliers in the dataset?” (p. 264). When using boxplots together with scatterplots, we could not detect any influential outliers. e) “Are there other unique characteristics of the sample that might be responsible for an unusually low value of r?” (p. 264). The study included a relatively homogenous sample of severely depressed patients and did not recruit patients from ambulatory settings with lower fatigue symptom severity, which leads to selection bias. Pre-assessments could therefore be subject to insufficient effort responding on self-report measures (e.g., straight lining, random responding), which has been linked to psychiatric symptom burden, and inflates type II errors by decreasing the size of estimated correlations [64, 65]. f) “Is the measurement reliability for either variable (or both) low?” (p. 264). Carless responding may also affect internal consistency estimates [64], which could explain the lower pre-treatment scale reliabilities in our study. In summary, the unexpectedly low pre-treatment correlations are likely due to the interaction of lack of variability, pre-treatment sample characteristics, and measurement errors.

Second, diminished confidence in the body at baseline was identified as a prospective predictor of higher symptom severity of general, physical, and mental fatigue at the end of hospitalization. In particular, a lack of body trust has been shown as a common interoceptive abnormality in MDD [29, 66]. Additionally, a recent study has demonstrated the clinical importance of alleviating body mistrust in predicting overall response to treatment-as-usual in hospitalized MDD patients [42]. These findings are consistent with recently proposed theories which also ground the etiology of fatigue in interoceptive processes [43]. Stephan et al. [43] conceptualized fatigue and other symptoms of depression as a maladaptive response to chronic dyshomeostasis which cannot be resolved by allostatic regulation. Consequently, subjects develop beliefs about the uncontrollability and unpredictability of body sensations, which lead to “learned helplessness” [43] and probably to diminished body trust. The sense of low-self efficacy and diminished locus of control have a prominent history in depression research as contributing factors to MDD symptomatology [67]. These dysfunctional cognitions could also generalize to other situations [68] beyond the body, resulting in a global state of hopelessness [43]. However, previous research on the occurrence of learned helplessness has had a cognitive rather than somatic focus in terms of precipitating factors [67]. Moreover, the lack of body trust could reflect an existential dimension of the embodied human being (i.e., limited control over the body could lead to a threat to survival [43]), which can also be interpreted against the background of the phenomenological literature. Accordingly, the body is deprived of its predictable and sentient “material me” [12] and turns to an untrustworthy, inanimate corpse in depression [39, 41, 57], which can hardly enter into resonance with its environment [69], and loses sense of agency [70]. This fatigue-related “reification” of the body alters the person’s relation to the world, which is perceived through the felt body [40]: “it is the loss of an ordinarily taken for granted vitality that at the same time amounts to a draining away of practical possibilities from the experienced world” (p. 8).

Third, regarding residual symptoms of mental fatigue, we have shown that the ability to sustain attention to body sensations prospectively predicted less concentration difficulties after hospitalization. The close connection between targeted body focus and general ability to concentrate can be considered evident. We also found that increased listening to the body and an increased mindful cognitive appraisal of unpleasant body sensations at baseline is related to residual symptom burden in the mental fatigue dimension. These results contrast with the pattern of other interoceptive predictors studied, which show a negative relationship. It could be speculated to what extent too much focus on the body and an exaggerated mindful (i.e., non-judging) acknowledgment of unpleasant body sensations is associated with mental exhaustion. To the best of our knowledge, there is only one study reporting detrimental effects of heightened scores on the MAIA-2 Body Listening scale on clinical outcomes: Gioia et al. showed that increased tendency to listen to the body independently predicted the subsequent risk of suicidal ideation and nonsuicidal self-injury [48]. Preliminary findings thus suggest that increased active awareness of bodily sensations to gain insight, and possibly the adoption of an overly mindful cognitive style towards pain, may lead to unfavorable outcomes, calling into question the use of mindfulness-based intervention in depression therapy. These interventions are currently hyped [71], but have demonstrated unsatisfactory efficacy [72]. A growing body of research also points to rare but relatively severe and largely under-reported side effects [73, 74], which might also be better understood against the background of current evidence. Mindfulness training such as the “body scan” procedure focus on internal somatic signals, which at the same time also increases the tendency to listen to the body for insight and additionally modifies the cognitive appraisal of pain [75,76,77], which could be at the expense of mental energy. Clinicians should be aware of these effects to prevent an unfavorable outcome for the hospitalized patient.

Fourth, congruent with previous research, included participants reported a high prevalence of fatigue of any severity at the end of hospital treatment. Fava et al. summarized the prevalence of residual fatigue symptoms across clinical trials and found high proportions for all treatment response groups (partial responders: 63–98%; remission: 22–49%; partial response + remission: 85–91%) [5]. Among the many subtypes of MDD [78], fatigue is most frequently reported by patients suffering from atypical depression, which is characterized by a somatic symptom profile (leaden paralysis, hypersomnia, appetite changes etc.) [79]. Some authors speculated whether fatigue has a different underlying etiology than other symptoms of depression, which could explain its poor response to established treatments [3, 7]. Therefore, in the present study, a symptom-specific analysis of differentiated fatigue dimensions was performed instead of predicting the overall depression severity to account for the heterogeneity of MDD [80].

This study is subject to several limitations. Due to its observational nature, causal conclusions should not be drawn based on the present study findings. The findings are based exclusively on self-report measures of interoception and should be regarded as exploratory with potential risk for type I error as the analysis plan was not pre-registered. The ecological validity of our results may be limited by selection bias because we recruited in an academic hospital and therefore included a substantial proportion of severely depressed inpatients. Rather than using DSM-5®-based SCID-5-CV interviews, diagnoses were assessed by trained psychiatrists/psychologists following ICD-10 criteria [2]. In addition, the study was not adequately powered to examine predictors of residual fatigue exclusively in those who responded or partially responded to treatment—a project that should be considered in the future. Conceptual concerns were also raised, emphasizing the difficulty of distinguishing fatigue from anhedonia [1] and antidepressant-related side effects [5, 6]. The latter is not possible in the context of this observational study, as this would require a randomized placebo-controlled trial providing sufficient internal validity to distinguish between medication-induced and depression-related fatigue. In this respect, our results are uncertain as to whether self-reported interoception predicts medication side effects, depression-associated residual fatigue, or both. However, this tends to be of secondary importance for everyday clinical practice, since a large proportion of patients with major depression are treated with antidepressants. The MFI-20 General Fatigue and Reduced Motivation scales, and the MAIA-2 Noticing, Not-Distracting and Not-Worrying scales demonstrated inappropriate pre-treatment internal consistency reliabilities (ωpre < .70), which may affect the validity of the findings regarding these scales. Future research should also include other facets of interoception, such as interoceptive accuracy, or composite measures such as the interoceptive state/trait prediction error. Furthermore, the individual effects of the treatment components in our naturalistic treatment-as-usual setting on self-reported interoception and multidimensional fatigue could not be identified in the current design. Randomized controlled trials are needed to address these challenges.

From a clinical standpoint, body sensations should be given greater consideration in clinical practice, both to identify patients at risk for adverse outcomes and to tailor personalized treatments. The prevention and treatment of residual symptoms of fatigue is not adequately addressed in available guidelines, although their clinical and psychosocial impact is increasingly recognized [4, 5]. Clinicians may consider discontinuing or reducing the dose of psychopharmaceuticals associated with residual fatigue, such as sedating tricyclic antidepressants, and should be aware of the high rate of treatment-related fatigue associated with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors [5, 54, 81]. Exercise like graded aerobic training could be a promising treatment because it alleviates symptoms of residual fatigue [54]. An interoceptive mechanism of action has been assumed for exercise therapy [82]. However, the patient’s consent to participate in exercise therapy could be hampered by the main features of MDD, such as severe fatigue, lack of interest, motivation loss, psychosomatic complaints, or comorbidity with physical diseases [83]. Others have therefore suggested the use of psychoregulatory massage therapies as complementary treatments for MDD, which have shown antidepressant, anxiolytic, analgesic, calming, and fatigue-reducing effects, probably via an interoceptive mechanism of action [84,85,86,87,88,89,90]. The frequent use of touch-based treatments in depressed patients may support their wider application [91]. Besides, the effectiveness of massage therapy in reducing fatigue has been demonstrated in patients suffering from chronic fatigue syndrome [92], cancer [93], and multiple sclerosis [94].

Conclusions

The burden of residual fatigue in depressed patients is not sufficiently addressed by current treatment guidelines, resulting in high prevalence rates even in treatment responders. Decreased confidence in the body at admission predicted increased fatigue severity at the end of hospital treatment and could therefore serve as a target for improving therapy. Despite increasing evidence, which recognizes the impact of fatigue on prognosis, daily functioning, and quality of life in MDD, targeted interventions are scarce and could be complemented by body-focused treatments. However, clinicians may also be aware of the potential negative effects of a) an increased tendency to listen to one’s body and b) the adoption of an overly mindful cognitive style towards pain on mental fatigue in terms of therapeutic plan, secondary prevention, and lifestyle counselling. Due to the exploratory nature of this study, our findings are preliminary and need to be replicated in pre-registered trials with larger sample sizes.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Billones RR, Kumar S, Saligan LN. Disentangling fatigue from anhedonia: a scoping review. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10:273. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-00960-w.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. WHO. Manual of the international classification of diseases and related health problems. 10th ed. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Arnold LM. Understanding fatigue in major depressive disorder and other medical disorders. Psychosomatics. 2008;49:185–90. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.psy.49.3.185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Paykel ES, Ramana R, Cooper Z, Hayhurst H, Kerr J, Barocka A. Residual symptoms after partial remission: an important outcome in depression. Psychol Med. 1995;25:1171–80. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291700033146.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Fava M, Ball S, Nelson JC, Sparks J, Konechnik T, Classi P, et al. Clinical relevance of fatigue as a residual symptom in major depressive disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31:250–7. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Fava M, Hoog SL, Judge RA, Kopp JB, Nilsson ME, Gonzales JS. Acute efficacy of fluoxetine versus sertraline and paroxetine in major depressive disorder including effects of baseline insomnia. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2002;22:137–47. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004714-200204000-00006.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Demyttenaere K, de Fruyt J, Stahl SM. The many faces of fatigue in major depressive disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2005;8:93–105. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145704004729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Moos RH, Cronkite RC. Symptom-based predictors of a 10-year chronic course of treated depression. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1999;187:360–8. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005053-199906000-00005.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Kennedy N, Paykel ES. Residual symptoms at remission from depression: impact on long-term outcome. J Affect Disord. 2004;80:135–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00054-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Swindle R, Kroenke K, Braun L. Energy and improved workplace productivity in depression. In: Farquhar I, Sorkin AL, Summers K, editors. Investing in health: the social and economic benefits of health care innovation. Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Limited; 2001. p. 323–41.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  11. Tranter R, O’Donovan C, Chandarana P, Kennedy S. Prevalence and outcome of partial remission in depression. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2002;27:241–7.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Craig AD. How do you feel - now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:59–70. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:655–66. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn894.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Khalsa SS, Adolphs R, Cameron OG, Critchley HD, Davenport PW, Feinstein JS, et al. Interoception and mental health: a roadmap. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2018;3:501–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Paulus MP, Feinstein JS, Khalsa SS. An active inference approach to interoceptive psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2019;15:97–122. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095617.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Tsakiris M, Critchley H. Interoception beyond homeostasis: affect, cognition and mental health. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2016;371:1708. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Wiens S. Interoception in emotional experience. Curr Opin Neurol. 2005;18:442–7. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wco.0000168079.92106.99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Piech RM, Strelchuk D, Knights J, Hjälmheden JV, Olofsson JK, Aspell JE. People with higher interoceptive sensitivity are more altruistic, but improving interoception does not increase altruism. Sci Rep. 2017;7:15652. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-14318-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Fuchs T. Leib, Raum, Person: Entwurf einer phänomenologischen Anthropologie. 2nd ed. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta; 2018.

  20. Damasio AR. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Phil Trans R Soc B. 1996;351:1413–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. James W. What is an emotion? Mind. 1884;9:188–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Schachter S, Singer J. Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychol Rev. 1962;69:379–99. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046234.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Barrett LF. The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2017;12:1–23. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Cameron OG. Interoception: the inside story–a model for psychosomatic processes. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:697–710.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Eggart M, Lange A, Binser MJ, Queri S, Müller-Oerlinghausen B. Major depressive disorder is associated with impaired interoceptive accuracy: a systematic review. Brain Sci. 2019;9:131. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9060131.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Harshaw C. Interoceptive dysfunction: toward an integrated framework for understanding somatic and affective disturbance in depression. Psychol Bull. 2015;141:311–63. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Paulus MP, Stein MB. Interoception in anxiety and depression. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214:451–63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0258-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Eggart M, Valdés-Stauber J. Can changes in multidimensional self-reported interoception be considered as outcome predictors in severely depressed patients? A moderation and mediation analysis. J Psychosom Res. 2021;141:110331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2020.110331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Dunne J, Flores M, Gawande R, Schuman-Olivier Z. Losing trust in body sensations: interoceptive awareness and depression symptom severity among primary care patients. J Affect Disord. 2021;282:1210–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.12.092.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Lyons N, Strasser A, Beitz B, Teismann T, Ostermann T, Anderle L, Michalak J. Bodily maps of emotion in major depressive disorder. Cogn Ther Res. 2021;45:508–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-020-10195-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Lyons N, Michaelsen MM, Graser J, Bundschuh-Müller K, Esch T, Michalak J. Bodily experience in depression: using focusing as a new interview technique. Psychopathology. 2021;54:150–8. https://doi.org/10.1159/000514128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Garfinkel SN, Critchley HD. Interoception, emotion and brain: new insights link internal physiology to social behaviour. Commentary on: “Anterior insular cortex mediates bodily sensibility and social anxiety” by Terasawa et al. (2012). Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2013;8:231–4. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss140.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Murphy J, Catmur C, Bird G. Classifying individual differences in interoception: implications for the measurement of interoceptive awareness. Psychon Bull Rev. 2019;26:1467–71. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01632-7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Zamariola G, Maurage P, Luminet O, Corneille O. Interoceptive accuracy scores from the heartbeat counting task are problematic: evidence from simple bivariate correlations. Biol Psychol. 2018;137:12–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.06.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Desmedt O, Luminet O, Corneille O. The heartbeat counting task largely involves non-interoceptive processes: evidence from both the original and an adapted counting task. Biol Psychol. 2018;138:185–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.09.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Ring C, Brener J. Heartbeat counting is unrelated to heartbeat detection: a comparison of methods to quantify interoception. Psychophysiology. 2018;55:e13084. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13084.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Mehling WE. Differentiating attention styles and regulatory aspects of self-reported interoceptive sensibility. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2016;371:20160013. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0013.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Mehling WE, Gopisetty V, Daubenmier J, Price CJ, Hecht FM, Stewart A. Body awareness: construct and self-report measures. PLoS One. 2009;4:e5614. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0005614.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Fuchs T. Corporealized and disembodied minds: a phenomenological view of the body in melancholia and schizophrenia. Philos Psychiatr Psychol. 2005;12:95–107.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Ratcliffe M. A bad case of the flu? The comparative phenomenology of depression and somatic illness. J Conscious Stud. 2013;20:198–218.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Fuchs T. The Phenomenology of shame, guilt and the body in body dysmorphic disorder and depression. J Phenomenol Psychol. 2002;33:223–43. https://doi.org/10.1163/15691620260622903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Eggart M, Todd J, Valdés-Stauber J. Validation of the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA-2) questionnaire in hospitalized patients with major depressive disorder. PLoS One. 2021;16:e0253913. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0253913.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Stephan KE, Manjaly ZM, Mathys CD, Weber LAE, Paliwal S, Gard T, et al. Allostatic self-efficacy: a metacognitive theory of dyshomeostasis-induced fatigue and depression. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:550. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00550.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Mehling WE, Acree M, Stewart A, Silas J, Jones A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, Version 2 (MAIA-2). PLoS One. 2018;13:e0208034. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208034.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. George D, Mallery P. SPSS for windows step by step: a simple guide and reference, 11.0 update. 4th ed. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Mehling WE, Price C, Daubenmier JJ, Acree M, Bartmess E, Stewart A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). PLoS One. 2012;7:e48230. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048230.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Goubert L, Crombez G, van Damme S. The role of neuroticism, pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear in vigilance to pain: a structural equations approach. Pain. 2004;107:234–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2003.11.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Gioia AN, Forrest LN, Smith AR. Diminished body trust uniquely predicts suicidal ideation and nonsuicidal self-injury among people with recent self-injurious thoughts and behaviors. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2022;52:1205–16. https://doi.org/10.1111/sltb.12915.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Smets EM, Garssen B, Bonke B, de Haes JC. The Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI) psychometric qualities of an instrument to assess fatigue. J Psychosom Res. 1995;39:315–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3999(94)00125-o.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Hinz A, Weis J, Brähler E, Härter M, Geue K, Ernst J. Fatigue in cancer patients: comparison with the general population and prognostic factors. Support Care Cancer. 2020;28:4517–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00520-019-05260-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2021. https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s00180-020-01034-7.pdf.

  52. Purcell A, Fleming J, Bennett S, Burmeister B, Haines T. Determining the minimal clinically important difference criteria for the multidimensional fatigue inventory in a radiotherapy population. Support Care Cancer. 2010;18:307–15. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00520-009-0653-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Lin DY, Psaty BM, Kronmal RA. Assessing the sensitivity of regression results to unmeasured confounders in observational studies. Biometrics. 1998;54:948. https://doi.org/10.2307/2533848.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Marin H, Menza MA. The management of fatigue in depressed patients. Essent Psychopharmacol. 2005;6:185–92.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Eggart M, Valdés-Stauber J, Müller-Oerlinghausen B, Heinze M. Exploring associations between C-reactive protein and self-reported interoception in major depressive disorder: a Bayesian analysis. Brain Sci. 2023;13:353. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13020353.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:175–91. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Fuchs T, Schlimme JE. Embodiment and psychopathology: a phenomenological perspective. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2009;22:570–5. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283318e5c.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Wiebking C, de Greck M, Duncan NW, Tempelmann C, Bajbouj M, Northoff G. Interoception in insula subregions as a possible state marker for depression-an exploratory fMRI study investigating healthy, depressed and remitted participants. Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9:82. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00082.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Barrett LF, Quigley KS, Hamilton P. An active inference theory of allostasis and interoception in depression. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2016;371:20160011. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0011.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Scheffers M, van Duijn MAJ, Beldman M, Bosscher RJ, van Busschbach JT, Schoevers RA. Body attitude, body satisfaction and body awareness in a clinical group of depressed patients: an observational study on the associations with depression severity and the influence of treatment. J Affect Disord. 2019;242:22–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.08.074.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Banner NF. Mental disorders are not brain disorders. J Eval Clin Pract. 2013;19:509–13. https://doi.org/10.1111/jep.12048.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Lee C-H, Giuliani F. The role of inflammation in depression and fatigue. Front Immunol. 2019;10:1696. https://doi.org/10.3389/fimmu.2019.01696.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Goodwin LD, Leech NL. Understanding correlation: factors that affect the size of r. J Exp Educ. 2006;74:249–66. https://doi.org/10.3200/JEXE.74.3.249-266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. DeSimone JA, DeSimone AJ, Harms PD, Wood D. The differential impacts of two forms of insufficient effort responding. Appl Psychol. 2018;67:309–38. https://doi.org/10.1111/apps.12117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Dupuis M, Meier E, Rudaz D, Strippoli MPF, Castelao E, Preisig M, et al. Psychiatric symptoms and response quality to self-rated personality tests: evidence from the PsyCoLaus study. Psychiatry Res. 2017;252:118–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.02.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Flasinski T, Dierolf AM, Rost S, Lutz APC, Voderholzer U, Koch S, et al. Altered interoceptive awareness in high habitual symptom reporters and patients with somatoform disorders. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1859. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01859.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Abramson LY, Seligman ME, Teasdale JD. Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. J Abnorm Psychol. 1978;87:49–74. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.87.1.49.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev. 1977;84:191–215. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.84.2.191.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Rosa H. Resonanz: Eine Soziologie der Weltbeziehung. 1st ed. Berlin: Suhrkamp; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Slaby J, Paskaleva A, Stephan A. Enactive emotion and impaired agency in depression. J Conscious Stud. 2013;20:33–55.

    Google Scholar 

  71. van Dam NT, van Vugt MK, Vago DR, Schmalzl L, Saron CD, Olendzki A, et al. Mind the hype: a critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2018;13:36–61. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617709589.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Coronado-Montoya S, Levis AW, Kwakkenbos L, Steele RJ, Turner EH, Thombs BD. Reporting of positive results in randomized controlled trials of mindfulness-based mental health interventions. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0153220. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0153220.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Shapiro DH. Adverse effects of meditation: a preliminary investigation of long-term meditators. Int J Psychosom. 1992;39:62–7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Schlosser M, Sparby T, Vörös S, Jones R, Marchant NL. Unpleasant meditation-related experiences in regular meditators: prevalence, predictors, and conceptual considerations. PLoS One. 2019;14:e0216643. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0216643.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Bornemann B, Herbert BM, Mehling WE, Singer T. Differential changes in self-reported aspects of interoceptive awareness through 3 months of contemplative training. Front Psychol. 2015;5:1504. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01504.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. de Lima-Araujo GL, de Sousa Júnior GM, Mendes T, Demarzo M, Farb N, Barros de Araujo D, de Sousa MBC. The impact of a brief mindfulness training on interoception: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One. 2022;17:e0273864. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0273864.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Fissler M, Winnebeck E, Schroeter T, Gummersbach M, Huntenburg JM, Gaertner M, Barnhofer T. An investigation of the effects of brief mindfulness training on self-reported interoceptive awareness, the ability to decenter, and their role in the reduction of depressive symptoms. Mindfulness. 2016;7:1170–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0559-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Fried EI, Nesse RM. Depression is not a consistent syndrome: an investigation of unique symptom patterns in the STAR*D study. J Affect Disord. 2015;172:96–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.10.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Juruena MF, Bocharova M, Agustini B, Young AH. Atypical depression and non-atypical depression: is HPA axis function a biomarker? A systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2018;233:45–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.052.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Fried EI, Nesse RM. Depression sum-scores don’t add up: why analyzing specific depression symptoms is essential. BMC Med. 2015;13:72. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-015-0325-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Papakostas GI, Nutt DJ, Hallett LA, Tucker VL, Krishen A, Fava M. Resolution of sleepiness and fatigue in major depressive disorder: a comparison of bupropion and the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:1350–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.015.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Mehling WE, Chesney MA, Metzler TJ, Goldstein LA, Maguen S, Geronimo C, et al. A 12-week integrative exercise program improves self-reported mindfulness and interoceptive awareness in war veterans with posttraumatic stress symptoms. J Clin Psychol. 2018;74:554–65. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22549.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Knapen J, Vancampfort D, Moriën Y, Marchal Y. Exercise therapy improves both mental and physical health in patients with major depression. Disabil Rehabil. 2015;37:1490–5. https://doi.org/10.3109/09638288.2014.972579.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Eggart M, Queri S, Müller-Oerlinghausen B. Are the antidepressive effects of massage therapy mediated by restoration of impaired interoceptive functioning? A novel hypothetical mechanism. Med Hypotheses. 2019;128:28–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2019.05.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Moyer CA, Rounds J, Hannum JW. A meta-analysis of massage therapy research. Psychol Bull. 2004;130:3–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.1.3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Müller-Oerlinghausen B, Berg C, Scherer P, Mackert A, Moestl H-P, Wolf J. Effects of slow-stroke massage as complementary treatment of depressed hospitalized patients. Results of a controlled study (SeSeTra). Dtsch Med Wochenschr. 2004;129:1363–8. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2004-826874.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Arnold MM, Müller-Oerlinghausen B, Hemrich N, Bönsch D. Effects of psychoactive massage in outpatients with depressive disorders: a randomized controlled mixed-methods study. Brain Sci. 2020;10:676. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10100676.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Baumgart S, Müller-Oerlinghausen B, Schendera CF. Wirksamkeit der Massagetherapie bei Depression und Angsterkrankungen sowie bei Depressivität und Angst als Komorbidität – Eine systematische Übersicht kontrollierter Studien. Phys Med Rehab Kuror. 2011;21:167–82. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0031-1279760.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Baumgart S, Baumbach-Kraft A, Lorenz J. Effect of psycho-regulatory massage therapy on pain and depression in women with chronic and/or somatoform back pain: a randomized controlled trial. Brain Sci. 2020;10:721. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10100721.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Müller-Oerlinghausen B, Eggart M, Norholt H, Gerlach M, Kiebgis GM, Arnold MM, Uvnäs MK. Berührungsmedizin – ein komplementärer therapeutischer Ansatz unter besonderer Berücksichtigung der Depressionsbehandlung. Dtsch Med Wochenschr. 2022;147:e32–40. https://doi.org/10.1055/a-1687-2445.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Wu P, Fuller C, Liu X, Lee HC, Fan B, Hoven CW, et al. Use of complementary and alternative medicine among women with depression: results of a national survey. Psychiatr Serv. 2007;58:349–56. https://doi.org/10.1176/ps.2007.58.3.349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Field TM, Sunshine W, Hernandezreif M, Quintino O, Schanberg S, Kuhn C, Burman I. Massage therapy effects on depression and somatic symptoms in chronic fatigue syndrome. J Chronic Fatigue Syndr. 2011;3:43–51. https://doi.org/10.1300/J092v03n03_03.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Kinkead B, Schettler PJ, Larson ER, Carroll D, Sharenko M, Nettles J, et al. Massage therapy decreases cancer-related fatigue: results from a randomized early phase trial. Cancer. 2018;124:546–54. https://doi.org/10.1002/cncr.31064.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Heidari Z, Shahrbanian S, Chiu C. Massage therapy as a complementary and alternative approach for people with multiple sclerosis: a systematic review. Disabil Rehabil. 2022;44:5758–69. https://doi.org/10.1080/09638288.2021.1949051.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank all patients who participated in the study and would like to express our gratitude to the team of the hospital ward 2011 (Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy I, Ulm University, Center for Psychiatry Südwürttemberg, Weißenau, Germany).

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. Funded by the Brandenburg Medical School publication fund supported by the German Research Foundation and the Ministry of Science, Research and Cultural Affairs of the State of Brandenburg.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JVS and ME designed and coordinated the study. ME collected the data, performed statistical data analysis, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MH, JVS and BMOe supervised the project and reviewed/edited the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Eggart.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the ethics committee of Ulm University (registration number: 13/17). We followed the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and obtained patient’s written informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Eggart, M., Valdés-Stauber, J., Müller-Oerlinghausen, B. et al. Dysfunctional self-reported interoception predicts residual symptom burden of fatigue in major depressive disorder: an observational study. BMC Psychiatry 23, 667 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-05168-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-05168-y

Keywords