- Research
- Open Access
- Published:
Exploring the relationship between dysfunctional metacognitive processes and orthorexia nervosa: the moderating role of emotion regulation strategies
BMC Psychiatry volume 23, Article number: 674 (2023)
Abstract
Background
Dysfunctional metacognitive processes and emotional dysregulation have been widely documented in the eating disorder literature. Despite numerous research and recent consensus suggesting the categorization of orthorexia nervosa as a form of eating disorder, no previous study has examined whether aberrant metacognitive processes also correlate with orthorexia nervosa tendencies. This paper investigates potential associations between symptoms of orthorexia nervosa and dysfunctional metacognitive processes while also exploring whether such relationships may be influenced by adaptive/maladaptive emotion regulation strategies.
Methods
We conducted a cross-sectional study in all Lebanese governorates. In total, 423 Lebanese adults completed an online questionnaire including the Teruel Orthorexia Scale, the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire, the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale – 16 Item Version, and the Metacognitions Questionnaire – Short Form.
Results
Higher physical activity, expressive suppression, emotion regulation difficulties, positive metacognitive beliefs about worry, and need to control thoughts were significantly associated with higher orthorexia nervosa. Additionally, the emotion regulation strategies moderated the relationships between two dysfunctional metacognitive processes and orthorexia nervosa. Specifically, cognitive self-consciousness was negatively related to orthorexia nervosa only in individuals with low (versus higher) emotion suppression (maladaptive emotion regulation strategy). In contrast, negative beliefs about worry uncontrollability and danger positively predicted orthorexia nervosa only in individuals with lower (versus high) cognitive reappraisal (adaptive emotion regulation strategy). Lower socio-economic status and having a university level of education compared to secondary level were significantly associated with lower orthorexia nervosa.
Conclusion
Our study provides the first empirical evidence for the existence of significant associations between dysfunctional metacognitive processes and orthorexia nervosa. It also highlights that these pathways are considerably modulated by individuals’ ability to regulate their emotions adaptively versus maladaptively. Our findings thus suggest that therapies aimed at improving thought monitoring and emotional regulation may be beneficial for individuals with symptoms of orthorexia nervosa.
Introduction
Over recent decades, societies have been placing a growing emphasis on the importance of healthy eating, to the point that discussions regarding the ideal type, timing, and quantity of food consumption have increasingly become a significant part of social discourse and judgment [1]. As a result, researchers have witnessed the emergence of a novel entity labelled “Orthorexia Nervosa”, which was first introduced by Bratman in 1997 [2]. Orthorexia nervosa derived from the Greek terms “Ortho” (i.e., righteous) and “Orexia” (i.e., appetite), thus alluding to an unhealthy obsession with clean and healthy eating [3, 4] whose ultimate goal is to achieve purity and health [5]. Individuals with orthorexia nervosa tend to exponentially devote their time to activities related to meal planning and consumption, which interferes with various life aspects; to exemplify, they may avoid social situations involving eating out with others and even experience physical complications such as extreme weight loss and malnutrition [6, 7]. Despite an emerging body of research attempting to determine its classification and diagnostic criteria, this entity has neither been assigned a standardized definition nor included in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5-TR) [8] or any other international disease classification [9, 10].
While orthorexia nervosa’s overlap with Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD) in terms of obsessive characteristics, intrusive thoughts, and repetitive behaviors is still controversial [11,12,13,14], orthorexia nervosa is specifically centered around an obsession with food choices, healthy eating, and strict dietary habits [15, 16]. In contrast, OCD encompasses a broader spectrum of obsessions and compulsions unrelated to food, such as cleanliness, symmetry, or fears of harm [8]. Furthermore, the underlying motivations and beliefs differ, as orthorexia nervosa is driven by a pursuit of health and wellness, while OCD is characterized by irrational fears and the need to alleviate anxiety through repetitive actions [17, 18]. Numerous papers have however indicated that orthorexia nervosa also shares similarities with some eating disorders, such as anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa [19], and may even arise during the post-recovery phase of an eating disorder [20]. Symptoms of orthorexia may also frequently mask symptoms of other eating disorders, such as anorexia nervosa; in fact, in clinical settings, the simultaneous presence of orthorexia nervosa alongside other well-established eating disorders is commonly observed [21, 22]. A recent consensus document on the definition and diagnostic criteria for orthorexia nervosa has also suggested its categorization as a mental health disorder falling within the DSM-5’s “Feeding and Eating Disorders” section (with 93.3% of experts agreement), indicating that it could be identified as a distinct form of eating disorder [9]. Nonetheless, further research remains vital to determine whether orthorexia nervosa is similar to or distinct from other types of eating disorders, as well as to deeply scrutinize its features and predictors, in order to gain a better understanding of this condition’s psychopathology and hence develop effective treatments.
In this context, a potentially crucial aspect that has been severely overlooked in research is whether dysfunctional metacognitive processes may contribute to the induction and/or perpetuation of orthorexia nervosa symptoms. Metacognition refers to the ability to reflect on and monitor one’s own thinking processes; it is described as a resolutive cognitive strategy involving multiple psychological aspects, such as perception, control, examination, and adjustment of an individual’s thinking processes [23]. Dysfunctional metacognitive processes, on the other hand, are maladaptive patterns of thinking often leading to negative psychological and behavioral outcomes [23, 24]. They are mainly classified into positive and negative metacognitive beliefs. As such, positive beliefs are postulated to serve as evaluative mechanisms that assess the efficacy of certain cognitive processes (e.g., beliefs in the effectiveness of anxiety and worrying thoughts, excessive cognitive self-consciousness, etc.), with the goal of guiding and adjusting one’s thoughts and emotions; whereas negative beliefs involve a pervasive conviction of insufficient control over one’s cognitive and affective states (e.g., worry uncontrollability and danger, low cognitive confidence, need to control thoughts, etc.); both potentially leading to maladaptive patterns of coping, thinking, and behaving [23,24,25]. As for disordered eating behaviors, these dysfunctional metacognitive processes, often involving ruminative ideation and excessive worry, may give rise to heightened preoccupations with food and eating behaviors [26, 27]. Indeed, dysfunctional metacognitive processes have been extensively documented in the eating disorder literature [26, 28,29,30,31]. Nevertheless, no previous research has examined whether maladaptive metacognitive thoughts also correlate with orthorexia nervosa tendencies.
Moreover, emotion regulation difficulties and deficient emotion regulation strategies are important constructs that have been consistently demonstrated to be intricately related to eating disorders [32,33,34,35,36]. Emotion regulation refers to the use of cognitive and behavioral strategies to influence the emotional expression or response, with the aim of achieving a desired emotional state or outcome [37]. Such proficiency is essential for effectively managing aversive emotional experiences and requires several key cognitive-behavioral assets, namely a high level of emotional comprehension, awareness, and acceptance without judgment or avoidance; as well as the aptitude to engage in goal-directed behaviors, exercise impulse control, and use proficient emotion regulation strategies [38]. Alterations of any of these cognitive dimensions (i.e., emotion regulation difficulties) may contribute to the development and/or perpetuation of eating disorders as a maladaptive means to cope with negative emotions, and have been frequently observed in adults with such disorders compared to controls [33, 39, 40], as well as recently in orthorexia nervosa [41, 42].
On another hand, since emotion regulation implies the use of various approaches contingent on the stage and timing within the emotion production process [43], two emotion regulation strategies may be potentially enacted in response to adverse emotional experiences: expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal [44, 45]. Expressive suppression is a response-focused strategy often viewed as maladaptive, as it involves suppressing or reducing the behavioral and physiological expressions of ongoing negative emotions without circumventing their inner experience by the individual [37, 43, 46]. In contrast, cognitive reappraisal is an antecedent-focused emotion regulation strategy often seen as adaptive, as it interferes at the initial stages of emotional processing by modifying the perception and interpretation of an emotion-eliciting situation, hence preventing the development of negative emotions as well as their deleterious effects on individuals’ emotional experiences [37, 43, 46, 47]. Empirical investigations on the frequent use of expressive suppression as an emotion regulation strategy have consistently demonstrated its detrimental impact on various aspects of psychological well-being, as individuals experience unresolved aversive emotions and a feeling of discrepancy between their emotional experiences and behavioral expressions, potentially leading them to psychopathological issues including eating disorders [47,48,49,50,51,52]; whereas the opposite has been remarked for cognitive reappraisal, with no or even inverse relationships with eating disorder symptoms [51,52,53]. However, to the best of our knowledge, no study has attempted to explore the different relationships between expressive suppression/cognitive reappraisal and orthorexia nervosa to date.
In addition, newly emerging empirical evidence interestingly suggests the existence of a potential interplay between dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs and compromised emotion regulation capacities, which may act in concert to predict the onset and maintenance of various forms of psychopathology, including disordered eating behaviors [54,55,56,57]. In fact, since dysfunctional metacognitive appraisals serve as a mechanism for maladaptive ruminative thought processes and worry, it is posited that they also interfere in regulating emotions, thus frequently giving rise to overall deficits in emotion regulation and emotional difficulties, and resulting in heightened psychological issues and distress [24, 58, 59]. Indeed, the Self-Regulatory Executive Function model (S-REF) [59, 60] proposes a bidirectional causal relationship between dysfunctional metacognitive processes and emotional dysregulation, highlighting the influential role of dysfunctional metacognitive processes in the development of emotional disorder symptoms and psychopathological symptoms by fostering prolonged negative thinking in response to stress. Mutually, the model acknowledges the potential impact of emotions on metacognition, as emotion dysregulation may for instance reinforce dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs related to a perceived loss of control [59,60,61]. In line with this perspective, it is plausible to conjecture that deficient emotion regulation strategies (i.e., expressive suppression) and emotion regulation difficulties may positively moderate/strengthen the relationships between dysfunctional metacognitive processes and eating disorder symptoms like orthorexia nervosa tendencies, by affecting the extent to which people with orthorexia nervosa rely on food and eating behaviors to cope with negative emotions. Since individuals experiencing emotion regulation difficulties are particularly vulnerable to using food and eating behaviors as a means of regulating negative affect [62], ameliorations of negative emotions subsequent to indulging in such behaviors may reinforce these maladaptive strategies and potentially exacerbate the distorted thought patterns associated with dysfunctional metacognitive processes (i.e., excessive rumination, perceived loss of control, and worry towards eating patterns), thereby contributing to the maintenance and exacerbation of the eating disorder symptoms [27, 63]. Alternatively, it is also conceivable that the utilization of effective emotion regulation strategies, such as cognitive reappraisal, may negatively moderate/alleviate the impact of dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs on the development or exacerbation of orthorexia nervosa. By efficiently managing negative emotions linked to maladaptive metacognitive appraisals, individuals may possess higher capability to refrain from partaking in disordered food-related behaviors [51, 52] such as orthorexia nervosa tendencies. Nevertheless, no empirical investigations have been conducted to evaluate these postulations.
To this end, the current study aims to address the aforementioned gaps in the literature by examining the potential associations between symptoms of orthorexia nervosa and dysfunctional metacognitive processes, as well as both emotion regulation difficulties and strategies. Specifically, our study seeks to explore whether adaptive (cognitive reappraisal) and/or maladaptive (expressive suppression) emotion regulation strategies may influence the relationships between dysfunctional metacognitive processes and orthorexia nervosa. We hypothesize that, in addition to dysfunctional metacognitive processes and emotion regulation difficulties, only emotion suppression will be positively associated with orthorexia nervosa and reinforce the relationships between dysfunctional thought processes and orthorexia nervosa. Conversely, cognitive reappraisal is expected to mitigate such associations.
Methods
Participants
The sample consisted of 423 participants, with a mean age of 38.13 ± 11.03 years and 61.2% women. Other characteristics and description of the scores can be found in Table 1.
Study design and procedure
We conducted a cross-sectional study between June and July 2021 in all Lebanese governorates (Beirut, Mount Lebanon, North, South, and Bekaa). Due to the coronavirus pandemic outbreak, the data were gathered through snowball sampling using an online questionnaire. Prior to participation, study objectives and general instructions were delivered online for the individual subjects. We restricted multiple submissions from the same responder by using the “Restrict to 1 response” setting on Google Forms, where all IP addresses were examined. Participation was voluntary and anonymous. No credits were received for participation.
Minimal sample size calculation
According to the G-power software [64], 371 participants were needed to have enough statistical power, based on a minimal deviation of R2 from zero of 5%, a 5% risk of error, a 80% power, and 10 factors to be entered in the multivariate analyses.
Questionnaire and measures
The questionnaire was divided into three parts. In the first part, a written consent, confirming the approval of the participant to fill in the questionnaire was gathered. In the second part, respondents answered to questions assessing socio-demographic details (age, residency governorate, height, weight, etc.). The Household Crowding Index (HCI), reflecting the socio-economic status of the family, was calculated by dividing the number of people living in the house by the number of rooms in the house; a higher HCI reflects a lower socio-economic status [65]. In the last part of the study, participants completed a set of self-report measures, as follows:
The teruel orthorexia scale
The Teruel Orthorexia Scale (TOS), validated in Lebanon [66, 67], is a 17-item instrument that assesses orthorexia nervosa with two separate dimensions [68]: 9 items for Healthy Orthorexia or “HeOr” (e.g., “I mainly eat foods that I consider healthy”) and 8 items for Orthorexia Nervosa or “OrNe” (e.g., “Thoughts about healthy eating do not let me concentrate on other tasks”). Responses are provided on a four-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 = “strongly disagree” to 3 = “strongly agree”. Scores by dimension were computed as the sum of the item responses. In this study, the internal consistencies were ω = 0.85 for the TOS OrNe and ω = 0.85 for the TOS HeOr. In this paper, the TOS OrNe score were used.
The difficulties in emotion regulation scale – 16 Item Version (DERS-16)
It is a 16-item scale that assesses emotion regulation [69]. Items are graded using a 5-point Likert scale. Higher scores reflect increased emotion regulation difficulties. Within the scale are five subscales: emotional clarity (2 items; e.g., “I am confused about how I feel”), goals (3 items; e.g., “When I am upset, I have difficulty thinking about anything else”), impulse (3 items; e.g., “When I am upset, I feel out of control”), non-acceptance (3 items; e.g., “When I am upset, I feel like I am weak”), and strategies (5 items; e.g., “When I am upset, I believe that there is nothing I can do to make myself feel better”). The total score was used in this study (ω = 0.95). We used the validated Arabic version of this scale [70].
The emotion regulation questionnaire (ERQ)
Validated in Lebanon [71], it is a ten-item scale, with questions scored on a 7-point Likert scale [72]. Two dimensions derive from this scale: Expressive Suppression (e.g., “I control my emotions by not expressing them”) and Cognitive Reappraisal (e.g., “I control my emotions by changing the way I think about the situation I’m in”). Higher cognitive reappraisal scores are considered a positive/adaptive emotion regulation strategy (ω = 0.81), whereas higher emotional suppression is considered maladaptive within emotion regulation strategies (ω = 0.72).
The metacognitions questionnaire short form (MCQ-30)
Validated in Arabic [73], this tool [23] was used to assess dysfunctional metacognition processes. It consists of five dimensions, each composed of 6 items rated on a 4-point Likert scale from 1 (“Disagree”) to 4 (“Agree”): cognitive confidence, which concerns the beliefs a person holds about their cognitive abilities, including memory and attention (e.g., “I do not trust my memory”); positive beliefs about worry, which describes worrying as a problem-solving technique (e.g., “I need to worry in order to work well”); cognitive self-consciousness, which evaluates the tendency to constantly focus attention on one’s own thinking processes (e.g., “I constantly examine my thoughts”); negative beliefs about worry uncontrollability and danger, which describes worry as being uncontrollable and dangerous (e.g., “My worrying thoughts persist, no matter how I try to stop them”); and need to control thoughts, which assesses the belief of the person about thoughts having to be controlled/suppressed (e.g., “If I could not control my thoughts, I would not be able to function”). The McDonald’s ω values for the subscales were as follows: cognitive confidence (= 0.86), positive beliefs about worry (= 0.94), cognitive self-consciousness (= 0.84), negative beliefs about worry uncontrollability and danger (= 0.89) and need to control thoughts (= 0.78).
Statistical analyses
Since the data were collected using an online questionnaire, there were no missing values because responding to all questions was required. SPSS v.25 was used to compute data analyses. Prior to the analyses, normality of distribution of the TOS score was confirmed via a calculation of the skewness and kurtosis. Values between − 2 and + 2 are considered acceptable to prove normal univariate distribution [74]. These conditions consolidate the assumptions of normality in samples larger than 300 [75]. The scales’ internal consistency was evaluated using McDonald’s ω, and a threshold of 0.70 or higher was considered indicative of acceptable internal consistency [76]. McDonald’s ω was chosen as an alternative measure to Cronbach’s alpha due to concerns raised about the limitations associated with using Cronbach’s alpha [77]. Pearson’s test was used to correlate the TOS score with other continuous variables, whereas the Student’s t-test was used to compare two means. A set of linear regressions taking the TOS OrNe score as the dependent variable, was conducted as follows: in Step 1, socio-demographic variables were entered as covariates; in Step 2, DERS, Cognitive reappraisal, and Expressive suppression scores were entered, whereas in Step 3 the MCQ dimensions were entered. The PROCESS Macro v. 3.4 model 1 was then used to estimate significant interactions between each MCQ subscale score, taken as independent variables, and DERS and ERQ dimensions, taken as moderator variables. All variables were preliminarily standardized before computing interactions. Covariates were only entered in the linear regression if they were to show a p < .25 in the bivariate analysis [78]. A p < .05 was deemed statistically significant.
Results
Bivariate analysis
The bivariate analysis results are summarized in Tables 2 and 3. Higher cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression, and physical activity index were significantly associated with higher orthorexia nervosa, whereas higher household crowding index was significantly associated with lower orthorexia nervosa. Lower cognitive confidence (i.e., higher scores on the cognitive confidence subscale) and increased positive beliefs about worry, negative beliefs about worry uncontrollability and danger, and need to control thoughts were significantly associated with higher orthorexia nervosa. Lower cognitive confidence and increased need to control thoughts were significantly associated with higher expressive suppression, whereas higher cognitive self-consciousness was significantly associated with higher cognitive reappraisal. On another hand, participants with a secondary level of education or less significantly had a higher mean orthorexia nervosa score compared to those who had a university education level.
Multivariate analyses
The results of a linear regression, taking the orthorexia nervosa score as the dependent variable, showed that higher physical activity index (Beta = 0.01), expressive suppression (Beta = 0.14), emotion regulation difficulties (Beta = 0.20), positive metacognitive beliefs about worry (Beta = 0.22), and need to control thoughts (Beta = 0.20) were significantly associated with higher orthorexia nervosa, whereas lower socio-economic status (i.e., higher household crowding index) (Beta=-0.27) and having a university level of education compared to secondary level (Beta=-0.29) were significantly associated with lower orthorexia nervosa (Table 4).
Interaction analyses
The results of the interaction analyses are summarized in Table 5 (Table 5). The interactions between (1) negative beliefs about worry uncontrollability and danger by cognitive reappraisal (Fig. 1), and (2) cognitive self-consciousness by expressive suppression (Fig. 2) were significantly associated with orthorexia nervosa; at low (Beta = 0.21; p = .001) and moderate (Beta = 0.13; p = .029) levels of cognitive reappraisal, having higher negative beliefs about worry uncontrollability and danger was significantly associated with higher orthorexia nervosa (Table 6). Furthermore, at low levels of expressive suppression, higher levels of cognitive self-consciousness (Beta = − 0.14; p = .039) were significantly associated with lower orthorexia nervosa (Table 7).
Discussion
In this paper, our main objectives were to explore the relationships between dysfunctional metacognitive processes and orthorexia nervosa, as well as to investigate the potential role of adaptive (cognitive reappraisal) and/or maladaptive (expressive suppression) emotion regulation strategies within these associations. Our results provided novel insights into the metacognitive correlates of orthorexia nervosa. The analysis has indeed indicated that the need to control thoughts and positive metacognitive beliefs about worry were significantly related to increased orthorexia nervosa tendencies. These findings are in line with previous literature describing dysfunctional metacognitive processes in eating disorders [30]. For instance, individuals with eating disorders commonly display elevated levels of positive metacognitions regarding the efficacy of anxious and worrying thoughts [30]. Specifically, these metacognitions pertain to the anguish of gaining weight and the desire to maintain a specific body shape, which are also generally observed in people with eating disorders [79,80,81]. In the case of orthorexia nervosa, individuals may instead exhibit preoccupations with the fear of consuming an unhealthy diet and causing harm to their health, which can lead to an irrational and debilitating fixation on consuming only healthy foods.
Furthermore, the need to control thoughts has also been consistently established as a susceptibility factor for dysfunctional eating behaviors [26, 30, 82, 83]. Namely, individuals are inclined to engage in detrimental eating patterns in furtherance of regulating their thoughts, compensating for their low perception of control over internal emotions and external circumstances (a key cognitive factor in eating disorders) [84], and hence, evading unfavorable consequences. Resorting to disordered eating behaviors thus reflects their desire of perceiving some degree of self-control [84]. The current study thus speculates that this phenomenon may also be observed within the context of orthorexia nervosa, whereby individuals exhibit a pattern of restrictive eating behaviors that is characterized by the consumption of only foods deemed “healthy”, mainly as a means of sensing a subjective control over their health. Moreover, since people engaging in maladaptive eating behaviors commonly display a strong preoccupation with regulating and suppressing thoughts pertaining to their body weight and shape, this persistent focus leads to an even more inflated attentiveness to these body concerns, which may in turn contribute to maintaining the eating disorder [56]. In line with the previous statement, it has also been demonstrated that orthorexic behaviors can progressively intensify over time [85], contributing to the maintenance and chronicity of the disorder.
Additionally, our study demonstrated that emotion regulation difficulties and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (i.e., expressive suppression) were significantly associated with orthorexia nervosa among Lebanese adults, consistent with previous findings [41, 42]. Precisely, people with orthorexia nervosa showed a tendency to reject emotional responses, employ limited and unsuitable strategies for regulating their emotions, and experience difficulties in adopting goal-directed behaviors and controlling impulsive actions when experiencing distress [41]. Researchers suggested that when individuals are persuaded of the lack of alternative strategies that can effectively improve their emotional well-being [86, 87] (observed with deficient emotion regulation strategies), extreme and obsessive orthorexic eating behaviors may offer a sense of control over their emotions, thereby serving as a coping strategy to manage difficult emotional experiences [41]. Inappropriate self-imposed restrictive eating may then provide a feeling of security and reassurance over threatening situations and adverse emotional states, such as self-criticism related to body shape [88, 89] or unhealthy eating in the case of orthorexia nervosa. In this context, emotion regulation difficulties and expressive suppression have been documented across the broad spectrum of eating disorders as well [39, 40, 50, 51].
Remarkably, and as hypothesized, our analyses revealed that adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies exert key moderating effects in the relationships between dysfunctional metacognitive processes and orthorexia nervosa. Prior research has observed that in response to negative emotional expressions, people endeavor to regulate their thoughts [82], particularly the cognitive manifestations of underlying struggles with coping with distress [26]. This suggests that when emotion regulation strategies are deficient in coping with internal distress, dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs arise and fail to be counteracted, hence resulting in compensatory psychopathological disorders (e.g., eating disorders). Additional research has also emphasized the complex interplay between metacognitive and emotional processes in contributing to disordered eating behaviors [55,56,57].
In particular, among those with higher negative beliefs about worry uncontrollability and danger, participants with lower levels of cognitive reappraisal (adaptive emotion regulation strategy) displayed higher orthorexia nervosa tendencies in our sample. Our finding shows that in the absence of adaptive emotion regulation strategies, individuals experience amplified psychopathological symptoms (e.g., orthorexia nervosa) in response to negative metacognitive beliefs about the uncontrollability and danger of worrying thoughts. These negative beliefs have been identified as crucial factors in eating disorders [26, 28, 81]. Actually, research has suggested that persistent negative thoughts related to food and/or body image may become central to negative self-evaluations over time [28, 79]. Consequently, individuals with eating disorders may engage in complex metacognitive processes making them regard their negative beliefs as uncontrollable and harmful [28]; which may, in turn, prompt them to indulge in compensatory and restrictive eating behaviors as a way of circumventing negative outcomes such as weight gain (or an unhealthy body in the case of orthorexia nervosa). In contrast, among people manifesting heightened levels of cognitive self-consciousness, those having low levels of expressive suppression (maladaptive emotion regulation strategy) in our sample exhibited less symptoms of orthorexia nervosa. Our finding implies that in the absence of maladaptive emotion regulation strategies, the inclination to consistently direct one’s attention towards their own cognitive processes results in no or less negative and obsessive thinking patterns related to food and eating behaviors.
Our analysis also indicated that higher physical activity was associated with more orthorexia nervosa tendencies, in line with previous literature [90]. According to some concepts, individuals exhibiting disordered eating behaviors may engage in excessive exercise as a means of avoiding or coping with their emotions [91]. Finally, our study showed that lower socio-economic status and higher educational level (university level compared to secondary level) were related to less orthorexia nervosa tendencies. Prior reports have suggested that individuals from socio-economically disadvantaged backgrounds, particularly in the context of the current economic crisis in Lebanon, may prioritize meeting basic survival needs and obtaining adequate nutrition over concerns related to their weight or the quality and healthfulness of their diets [36]. In addition, despite some evidence suggesting that people with higher levels of education may be more susceptible to exhibit orthorexia nervosa tendencies, owing primarily to greater awareness and concern about adequate nutrition and health [92], our study found an opposite relationship. Consistent with our finding, some researchers have however revealed that individuals with lower levels of education may be at greater risk for developing orthorexia nervosa [11], potentially due to limited access to reliable and accurate information about nutrition and health. Overall, further research is still necessary to gain a better understanding of the association between educational level and orthorexia nervosa, as this relationship may be influenced by a variety of factors including the type of education and university major (e.g., nutrition students may be more susceptible to orthorexia nervosa compared to other students) [93].
Clinical implications
Dysfunctional metacognitive processes in individuals with orthorexia nervosa have yet to be explored in the literature. Through shedding light on the relationships among dysfunctional metacognitive processes, emotion regulation strategies, and orthorexia nervosa, our study has filled a significant gap in orthorexia nervosa research; which could help to better comprehend, identify, and manage this pathology. Our findings specifically endorse that comparably to individuals with other eating and psychopathological disorders, people with orthorexia nervosa may benefit from effective treatment strategies to manage dysfunctional thought patterns and negative emotions; such as metacognitive therapy, cognitive behavioral therapy, mindfulness-based interventions, or acceptance and commitment therapy; in order to enhance their awareness of their cognitive and affective states, and to foster the development of more adaptive coping strategies in response to them [94,95,96,97,98]. Building upon these perspectives, our study provides the groundwork for future work aiming at advancing research into the psychopathology, risk factors, and evidence-based therapeutic approaches for orthorexia nervosa.
Limitations
The results of our study should be interpreted in light of certain limitations, such as the cross-sectional design that impedes the establishment of causal relationships between the variables. Future cohort studies are warranted to reproduce our findings through temporal frameworks. In addition, all data were collected via the snowball sampling technique, which limits the generalizability of our findings to the whole Lebanese population. Indeed, due to potential difficulties some individuals may face in accessing internet services, addressing this issue in future research could improve the study findings’ overall representativeness. A residual confounding bias is also possible; additional variables could be considered as potential mediators and moderators of the relationships between dysfunctional metacognition and orthorexia nervosa in future research. In addition, evaluations of general health, mental health, and/or eating disorders must be considered in future research, in order to control for these comorbidities and test whether participants with a history of eating disorders, for instance, would be more inclined to participate in the survey, potentially impacting the research findings.
Conclusion
Our study provides the first empirical evidence for the existence of significant associations between dysfunctional metacognitive processes and orthorexia nervosa. It also highlights that these pathways are considerably modulated by individuals’ ability to regulate their emotions adaptively versus maladaptively. These results further support that orthorexia nervosa shares features with other eating disorders and can be treated as a particular form of feeding and eating disorders. Our findings thus suggest that therapies aimed at improving thought monitoring and emotional regulation may be beneficial for individuals with symptoms of orthorexia nervosa.
Data Availability
All data generated or analyzed during this study are not publicly available, but are available upon a reasonable request from the corresponding author (SH).
References
Rangel C, Dukeshire S, MacDonald L. Diet and anxiety. An exploration into the Orthorexic Society. Appetite. 2012;58(1):124–32.
Bratman S. The health food eating disorder. Yoga J. 1997;42:50.
Haddad C, Obeid S, Akel M, Honein K, Akiki M, Azar J, Hallit S. Correlates of orthorexia nervosa among a representative sample of the lebanese population. Eat Weight Disord. 2019;24(3):481–93.
Brytek-Matera A, Staniszewska A, Hallit S. Identifying the Profile of Orthorexic Behavior and normal eating behavior with cluster analysis: a cross-sectional study among polish adults. Nutrients 2020, 12(11).
Brytek-Matera A. Orthorexia nervosa–an eating disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder or disturbed eating habit. Archives of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy. 2012;1(1):55–60.
Oberle CD, Samaghabadi RO, Hughes EM. Orthorexia nervosa: Assessment and correlates with gender, BMI, and personality. Appetite. 2017;108:303–10.
Fidan T, Ertekin V, Işikay S, Kırpınar I. Prevalence of orthorexia among medical students in Erzurum, Turkey. Compr Psychiatr. 2010;51(1):49–54.
Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th Ed text revision. (no Title) 2022.
Donini LM, Barrada JR, Barthels F, Dunn TM, Babeau C, Brytek-Matera A, Cena H, Cerolini S, Cho HH, Coimbra M, et al. A consensus document on definition and diagnostic criteria for orthorexia nervosa. Eat Weight Disord. 2022;27(8):3695–711.
Varga M, Dukay-Szabó S, Túry F, van Furth Eric F. Evidence and gaps in the literature on orthorexia nervosa. Eat Weight Disorders-Studies Anorexia Bulimia Obes. 2013;18:103–11.
Donini LM, Marsili D, Graziani MP, Imbriale M, Cannella C. Orthorexia nervosa: a preliminary study with a proposal for diagnosis and an attempt to measure the dimension of the phenomenon. Eat Weight Disord. 2004;9(2):151–7.
Koven NS, Abry AW. The clinical basis of orthorexia nervosa: emerging perspectives. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:385–94.
Bundros J, Clifford D, Silliman K, Neyman Morris M. Prevalence of Orthorexia nervosa among college students based on Bratman’s test and associated tendencies. Appetite. 2016;101:86–94.
Hallit S, Azzi V, Malaeb D, Obeid S. Any overlap between orthorexia nervosa and obsessive-compulsive disorder in Lebanese adults? Results of a cross-sectional study and validation of the 12-item and 4-item obsessive-compulsive inventory (OCI-12 and OCI-4). BMC Psychiatry. 2022 Jul 14;22(1):470. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-04119-3.
Oberle CD, Watkins RS, Burkot AJ. Orthorexic eating behaviors related to exercise addiction and internal motivations in a sample of university students. Eat Weight Disord. 2018;23(1):67–74.
Cena H, Barthels F, Cuzzolaro M, Bratman S, Brytek-Matera A, Dunn T, Varga M, Missbach B, Donini LM. Definition and diagnostic criteria for orthorexia nervosa: a narrative review of the literature. Eat Weight Disord. 2019;24(2):209–46.
Meier M, Kossakowski JJ, Jones PJ, Kay B, Riemann BC, McNally RJ. Obsessive-compulsive symptoms in eating disorders: a network investigation. Int J Eat Disord. 2020;53(3):362–71.
Novara C, Pardini S, Maggio E, Mattioli S, Piasentin S. Orthorexia Nervosa: over concern or obsession about healthy food? Eat Weight Disord. 2021;26(8):2577–88.
Zickgraf HF, Ellis JM, Essayli JH. Disentangling orthorexia nervosa from healthy eating and other eating disorder symptoms: Relationships with clinical impairment, comorbidity, and self-reported food choices. Appetite. 2019;134:40–9.
Segura-Garcia C, Ramacciotti C, Rania M, Aloi M, Caroleo M, Bruni A, Gazzarrini D, Sinopoli F, De Fazio P. The prevalence of orthorexia nervosa among eating disorder patients after treatment. Eat Weight Disorders-Studies Anorexia Bulimia Obes. 2015;20:161–6.
McComb SE, Mills JS. Orthorexia nervosa: a review of psychosocial risk factors. Appetite. 2019;140:50–75.
Atchison AE, Zickgraf HF. Orthorexia nervosa and eating disorder behaviors: a systematic review of the literature. Appetite. 2022;177:106134.
Wells A, Cartwright-Hatton S. A short form of the metacognitions questionnaire: properties of the MCQ-30. Behav Res Ther. 2004;42(4):385–96.
Wells A. Emotional disorders and metacognition: innovative cognitive therapy. John Wiley & Sons; 2002.
Wells A, Carter K. Further tests of a cognitive model of generalized anxiety disorder: metacognitions and worry in GAD, panic disorder, social phobia, depression, and nonpatients. Behav Ther. 2001;32(1):85–102.
McDermott C, Rushford N. Dysfunctional metacognitions in anorexia nervosa. Eat Weight Disorders-Studies Anorexia Bulimia Obes. 2011;16:e49–e55.
Startup H, Lavender A, Oldershaw A, Stott R, Tchanturia K, Treasure J, Schmidt U. Worry and rumination in anorexia nervosa. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2013;41(3):301–16.
Woolrich RA, Cooper MJ, Turner HM. Metacognition in patients with anorexia nervosa, dieting and non-dieting women: a preliminary study. Eur Eat Disorders Review: Prof J Eat Disorders Association. 2008;16(1):11–20.
Vann A, Strodl E, Anderson E. Thinking about internal states, a qualitative investigation into metacognitions in women with eating disorders. J Eat Disorders. 2013;1(1):1–11.
Davenport E, Rushford N, Soon S, McDermott C. Dysfunctional metacognition and drive for thinness in typical and atypical anorexia nervosa. J Eat Disord. 2015;3:24.
Palmieri S, Mansueto G, Ruggiero GM, Caselli G, Sassaroli S, Spada MM. Metacognitive beliefs across eating disorders and eating behaviours: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2021;28(5):1254–65.
Harrison A, Sullivan S, Tchanturia K, Treasure J. Emotional functioning in eating disorders: attentional bias, emotion recognition and emotion regulation. Psychol Med. 2010;40(11):1887–97.
Lavender JM, Wonderlich SA, Engel SG, Gordon KH, Kaye WH, Mitchell JE. Dimensions of emotion dysregulation in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a conceptual review of the empirical literature. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015;40:111–22.
Monell E, Clinton D, Birgegard A. Emotion dysregulation and eating disorders-Associations with diagnostic presentation and key symptoms. Int J Eat Disord. 2018;51(8):921–30.
Mallorquí-Bagué N, Vintró‐Alcaraz C, Sánchez I, Riesco N, Agüera Z, Granero R, Jiménez‐Múrcia S, Menchón JM, Treasure J, Fernández‐Aranda F. Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic feature among eating disorders: Cross‐sectional and longitudinal approach. Eur Eat Disorders Rev. 2018;26(1):53–61.
Gerges S, Hallit S, Malaeb D, Obeid S. Maladaptive cognitive Schemas as Predictors of Disordered Eating: examining the Indirect pathway through emotion regulation difficulties. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(18):11620.
McRae K, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation. Emotion. 2020;20(1):1.
Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2004;26:41–54.
Svaldi J, Griepenstroh J, Tuschen-Caffier B, Ehring T. Emotion regulation deficits in eating disorders: a marker of eating pathology or general psychopathology? Psychiatry Res. 2012;197(1–2):103–11.
Brockmeyer T, Skunde M, Wu M, Bresslein E, Rudofsky G, Herzog W, Friederich H-C. Difficulties in emotion regulation across the spectrum of eating disorders. Compr Psychiatr. 2014;55(3):565–71.
Vuillier L, Robertson S, Greville-Harris M. Orthorexic tendencies are linked with difficulties with emotion identification and regulation. J Eat Disorders. 2020;8(1):1–10.
Obeid S, Hallit S, Akel M, Brytek-Matera A. Orthorexia nervosa and its association with alexithymia, emotion dysregulation and disordered eating attitudes among lebanese adults. Eat Weight Disorders-Studies Anorexia Bulimia Obes 2021:1–10.
Gross JJ. The extended process model of emotion regulation: elaborations, applications, and future directions. Psychol Inq. 2015;26(1):130–7.
Gross JJ. Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1998;74(1):224.
Gross JJ. Emotion regulation in adulthood: timing is everything. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2001;10(6):214–9.
Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: a transdiagnostic examination. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(10):974–83.
Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(2):217–37.
Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2003;85(2):348.
Görlach MG, Kohlmann S, Shedden-Mora M, Rief W, Westermann S. Expressive suppression of emotions and overeating in individuals with overweight and obesity. Eur Eat Disorders Rev. 2016;24(5):377–82.
Samuel L, Cohen M. Expressive suppression and emotional eating in older and younger adults: an exploratory study. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2018;78:127–31.
Naumann E, Svaldi J. Influence of suppression and reappraisal on eating-related symptoms and ruminative thinking in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Behav Res Ther. 2021;141:103851.
Embaye J, Bassi G, Dingemans AE, Doesborg L, Snoek FJ, de Wit M. Associations between disordered eating behaviour, diabetes distress and emotion regulation strategies in adults with type 1 diabetes: results from a dutch-italian cross‐sectional study. Diabet Med 2023:e15122.
Murayama Y, Ohya A. A cross-sectional examination of the simultaneous association of four emotion regulation strategies with abnormal eating behaviours among women in Japan. J Eat Disorders. 2021;9:1–11.
Mazloom M, Yaghubi H, Mohammadkhani S. Post-traumatic stress symptom, metacognition, emotional schema and emotion regulation: a structural equation model. Pers Indiv Differ. 2016;88:94–8.
Nejati S, Rezaei AM, Moradi M, Rajezi Esfahani S. Metacognitive beliefs and emotion regulation strategies: obese women with negative and positive body images. J Res Health. 2017;7(3):826–33.
Laghi F, Bianchi D, Pompili S, Lonigro A, Baiocco R. Metacognition, emotional functioning and binge eating in adolescence: the moderation role of need to control thoughts. Eat Weight Disorders-Studies Anorexia Bulimia Obes. 2018;23:861–9.
Aloi M, Rania M, Carbone EA, Caroleo M, Calabrò G, Zaffino P, Nicolò G, Carcione A, Coco GL, Cosentino C. Metacognition and emotion regulation as treatment targets in binge eating disorder: a network analysis study. J Eat Disorders. 2021;9(1):1–9.
Wells A, Matthews G. Attention and emotion (classic edition): a clinical perspective. Psychology Press; 2014.
Wells A, Matthews G. Modelling cognition in emotional disorder: the S-REF model. Behav Res Ther. 1996;34(11–12):881–8.
Wells A, Matthews G. Attention and emotion: a clinical perspective. Hove, UK: Erlbaum; 1994.
Capobianco L, Heal C, Bright M, Wells A. What comes first Metacognition or negative emotion? A test of temporal precedence. Front Psychol. 2019;10:2507.
Meule A, Richard A, Schnepper R, Reichenberger J, Georgii C, Naab S, Voderholzer U, Blechert J. Emotion regulation and emotional eating in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Eat Disord. 2021;29(2):175–91.
Smyth JM, Wonderlich SA, Heron KE, Sliwinski MJ, Crosby RD, Mitchell JE, Engel SG. Daily and momentary mood and stress are associated with binge eating and vomiting in bulimia nervosa patients in the natural environment. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2007;75(4):629.
Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G. Statistical power analyses using G* power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–60.
Melki IS, Beydoun HA, Khogali M, Tamim H, Yunis KA. National Collaborative Perinatal neonatal N: Household crowding index: a correlate of socioeconomic status and inter-pregnancy spacing in an urban setting. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004;58(6):476–80.
Mhanna M, Azzi R, Hallit S, Obeid S, Soufia M. Validation of the arabic version of the Teruel Orthorexia Scale (TOS) among lebanese adolescents. Eat Weight Disord 2021.
Awad E, Obeid S, Sacre H, Salameh P, Strahler J, Hallit S. Association between impulsivity and orthorexia nervosa: any moderating role of maladaptive personality traits? Eat Weight Disord 2021.
Barrada JR, Roncero M. Bidimensional structure of the orthorexia: development and initial validation of a new instrument. Anales De Psicología/Annals of Psychology. 2018;34(2):283–91.
Bjureberg J, Ljotsson B, Tull MT, Hedman E, Sahlin H, Lundh LG, Bjarehed J, DiLillo D, Messman-Moore T, Gumpert CH, et al. Development and validation of a brief version of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale: the DERS-16. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2016;38(2):284–96.
Fekih-Romdhane F, Kanj G, Obeid S, Hallit S. Psychometric properties of an arabic translation of the brief version of the difficulty in emotion regulation scale (DERS-16). BMC Psychol. 2023;11(1):72.
Kahwagi RM, Zeidan RK, Haddad C, Hallit R, Sacre H, Kheir N, Salameh P, Obeid S, Hallit S. Emotion regulation among lebanese adults: validation of the emotion regulation questionnaire and association with attachment styles. Perspect Psychiatr Care. 2021;57(2):809–20.
Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85(2):348–62.
Fekih-Romdhane F, Azzi V, Obeid S, Sarray El Dine A, Malaeb D, Soufia M, Hallit S. Psychometric properties on an arabic translation of the short form of the metacognition questionnaire (MCQ-30) in a non-clinical adult sample. Article under review.
George D. SPSS for windows step by step: a simple study guide and reference, 17.0 update, 10/e. Pearson Education India; 2011.
Mishra P, Pandey CM, Singh U, Gupta A, Sahu C, Keshri A. Descriptive statistics and normality tests for statistical data. Ann Card Anaesth. 2019;22(1):67–72.
Dunn TJ, Baguley T, Brunsden V. From alpha to omega: a practical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency estimation. Br J Psychol. 2014;105(3):399–412.
McNeish D. Thanks coefficient alpha, we’ll take it from here. Psychol Methods. 2018;23(3):412.
Bursac Z, Gauss CH, Williams DK, Hosmer DW. Purposeful selection of variables in logistic regression. Source Code Biol Med. 2008;3:17.
Sassaroli S, Bertelli S, Decoppi M, Crosina M, Milos G, Ruggiero G. Worry and eating disorders: a psychopathological association. Eat Behav. 2005;6(4):301–7.
Konstantellou A, Reynolds M. Intolerance of uncertainty and metacognitions in a non-clinical sample with problematic and normal eating attitudes. Eat Behav. 2010;11(3):193–6.
Safdari S, Khoramdel K, Kamranian E. The role of metacognitive beliefs in eating disorders. J Social Issues & Humanities. 2013;1:96–9.
Olstad S, Solem S, Hjemdal O, Hagen R. Metacognition in eating disorders: comparison of women with eating disorders, self-reported history of eating disorders or psychiatric problems, and healthy controls. Eat Behav. 2015;16:17–22.
Palomba D, Venturini M, Rausa M, Contin SA, Penolazzi B, Schumann R, Ballardini D. Reduced sympathetic activity and dysfunctional metacognition in patients with anorexia nervosa: a preliminary study. J Evidence-Based Psychotherapies. 2017;17(1):1.
Sassaroli S, Gallucci M, Ruggiero GM. Low perception of control as a cognitive factor of eating disorders. Its independent effects on measures of eating disorders and its interactive effects with perfectionism and self-esteem. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2008;39(4):467–88.
Nevin SM, Vartanian LR. The stigma of clean dieting and orthorexia nervosa. J Eat Disorders. 2017;5:1–10.
Dignon A, Beardsmore A, Spain S, Kuan A. Why I won’t eat’ patient testimony from 15 anorexics concerning the Causes of their disorder. J Health Psychol. 2006;11(6):942–56.
Reid M, Burr J, Williams S, Hammersley R. Eating disorders patients’ views on their disorders and on an outpatient service: a qualitative study. J Health Psychol. 2008;13(7):956–60.
Dunkley DM, Grilo CM. Self-criticism, low self-esteem, depressive symptoms, and over-evaluation of shape and weight in binge eating disorder patients. Behav Res Ther. 2007;45(1):139–49.
Whiteside U, Chen E, Neighbors C, Hunter D, Lo T, Larimer M. Difficulties regulating emotions: do binge eaters have fewer strategies to modulate and tolerate negative affect? Eat Behav. 2007;8(2):162–9.
Brytek-Matera A, Pardini S, Szubert J, Novara C. Orthorexia nervosa and disordered eating attitudes, self-esteem and physical activity among young adults. Nutrients. 2022;14(6):1289.
Fairburn C, Cooper, z., Shafran R. (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: A “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment Behavior Research and Therapy, 41:509–528.
Villa M, Opawsky N, Manriquez S, Ananías N, Vergara-Barra P, Leonario-Rodriguez M. Orthorexia nervosa risk and associated factors among chilean nutrition students: a pilot study. J Eat Disorders. 2022;10(1):6.
Malmborg J, Bremander A, Olsson MC, Bergman S. Health status, physical activity, and orthorexia nervosa: a comparison between exercise science students and business students. Appetite. 2017;109:137–43.
Bostanov V, Keune PM, Kotchoubey B, Hautzinger M. Event-related brain potentials reflect increased concentration ability after mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: a randomized clinical trial. Psychiatry Res. 2012;199(3):174–80.
Delgado-Pastor LC, Ciria LF, Blanca B, Mata JL, Vera MN, Vila J. Dissociation between the cognitive and interoceptive components of mindfulness in the treatment of chronic worry. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2015;48:192–9.
Ruiz FJ, Odriozola-González P. Comparing cognitive, metacognitive, and acceptance and commitment therapy models of depression: a longitudinal study survey. Span J Psychol. 2015;18:E39.
Galbiati A, Sforza M, Scarpellino A, Salibba A, Leitner C, D’Este G, Mombelli S, Ferini-Strambi L, Castronovo V. Thinking about thinking in Insomnia Disorder: the Effect of cognitive-behavioral therapy for Insomnia on Sleep-Related Metacognition. Front Psychol. 2021;12:705112.
Tecuta L, Schumann R, Ballardini D, Tomba E. The need to control thoughts in eating disorder outpatients: a longitudinal study on its Modification and Association with Eating Disorder Symptom Improvement. J Clin Med. 2022;11(8):2205.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank all participants.
Funding
None.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
VA and SH designed the study; SG drafted the manuscript; SH carried out the analysis and interpreted the results; SG and DM collected the data. DB, FL, SP, MS and SO reviewed the paper for intellectual content; all authors reviewed the final manuscript and gave their consent.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Ethics approval for this study was obtained from the ethics committee of the School of Pharmacy at the Lebanese International University (2023RC-014-LIUSOP). Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects; the online submission of the soft copy was considered equivalent to receiving a written informed consent. All experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Gerges, S., Azzi, V., Bianchi, D. et al. Exploring the relationship between dysfunctional metacognitive processes and orthorexia nervosa: the moderating role of emotion regulation strategies. BMC Psychiatry 23, 674 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-05183-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-05183-z
Keywords
- Eating disorder
- Orthorexia Nervosa
- Emotion regulation
- Dysfunctional thoughts
- Metacognition
- Lebanon