Skip to main content

Autistic and schizotypal traits exhibit similarities in their impact on mentalization and adult attachment impairments: a cross-sectional study

Abstract

Background

Deficits in mentalizing and attachment occur in the autism and schizophrenia spectrum, and their extended traits in the general population. Parental attachment and the broader social environment highly influence the development of mentalizing. Given the similarities in the symptomatology and neurodevelopmental correlates of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and schizophrenia (SCH), it is crucial to identify their overlaps and differences to support screening, differential diagnosis, and intervention.

Methods

This cross-sectional study utilized questionnaire data from 2203 adults (65.1% female, mean age[SD] = 37.98[9.66]), including participants diagnosed with ASD, SCH, and those exhibiting subclinical traits to investigate the associations between mentalizing, attachment, and perceived social support during adolescence across the autistic and schizotypy spectrum.

Results

It was revealed that both autistic and schizotypal traits have comparable effects on insecure adult attachment, primarily through challenges in mentalizing. The impact of mentalizing deficits on adult attachment slightly varies between autistic and schizotypal traits. Conversely, perceived social support during adolescence relates to improved mentalizing and secure adult attachment as a protective factor during development.

Conclusions

These outcomes highlight the significance of supportive therapeutic relationships and community care while suggesting directions for further research and collaborative treatments addressing ASD and SCH, considering the differential impact of mentalizing on adult attachment.

Peer Review reports

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by difficulties in reciprocal social interaction, communication, and restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior that can be detected early in life and persist to adulthood [1]. Schizophrenia (SCH) is a complex neuropsychiatric disorder defined by positive (delusions and hallucinations), negative (social and emotional withdrawal, apathy), and disorganized (thought, speech, and motor behavior disruption) symptoms [1]. Although the first psychotic episode in SCH typically occurs in early adulthood, subtle alterations in cognitive and motor functions are present much earlier, indicative of altered neurodevelopment [2, 3]. Both ASD and SCH can be conceptualized as extended endophenotypic dimensions, referred to as autistic [4, 5] and schizotypal [6, 7] traits. It means that the features of ASD and SCH are widely present in the general population, ranging from very mild forms to subclinical presentations and extreme cases requiring therapeutic intervention, reflecting a liability to disorder.

Recently, numerous studies have pointed out significant overlaps in the prevalence and symptomatology (e.g., social and communication discomfort, relationship disinterest, and difficulty interpreting social cues) of ASD and SCH, hindering differential diagnosis, especially in adulthood [8,9,10,11,12,13]. Notably, the prevalence of SCH and other psychotic disorders among individuals with ASD has been reported to range from 4 to 67% [14,15,16]. The reported prevalence rates of diagnosed ASD in individuals with psychotic disorders range from 0.78 to 52%, while the prevalence of autistic-like traits in this population ranges from 9.6 to 61% [11]. Moreover, the two disorders share genetic and neurodevelopmental correlates as well as environmental risk factors, and their co-occurrence is associated with worse clinical outcomes [17,18,19,20,21]. Some theories suggest a common vulnerability model for ASD and SCH, with aberrant salience, asociality and concrete thinking being the common underlying dimensions [22], while others conceptualize ASD and SCH as diametric disorders of the social brain [23, 24]. As autistic and schizotypal traits overlap significantly, and a great portion of clinically affected individuals are only diagnosed in adulthood, it is crucial to examine the two sets of features in the same sample of adults to support differential diagnosis, develop novel screening tools, and identify possible targets of early intervention. Here, our objective is to explore mentalizing and attachment, which could potentially unlock crucial insights into the partially shared developmental origins of ASD and SCH, as their underlying neural deviations affect the mentalizing network in the brain [19]. Our dimensional approach aligns with the broader movement in psychopathology research, as continuous measures have been shown to exceed the reliability and validity of discrete diagnostic categories [25, 26].

Mentalizing is the human-specific, social-cognitive ability to attribute different mental states (e.g., thoughts, goals, desires, intentions, emotions) to oneself and others [27], which is essential to navigating everyday social interactions. It is a multifaceted concept that encompasses four dimensions [28]: (1) mentalizing can be automatic or controlled; (2) it can refer to our own mental states or those of others; (3) it can be external or internal; and (4) cognitive or affective. The neural bases of mentalizing involve complex cortical and subcortical networks. Prefrontal cortical areas play a crucial role in cognitive-controlled mentalizing, whereas parietal cortical and subcortical circuits are involved in automatic-affective processes, with the amygdala being of central importance [29]. It is widely accepted that mentalizing deficits underlie social difficulties (e.g. in social-emotional reciprocity, non-verbal communication, and maintaining relationships) observed in autism [30,31,32]. Difficulties in mentalizing are also well-documented in SCH [33, 34], such as paranoid delusions which can be considered distortions of reality resulting from impaired mentalizing of the self [35]. Recent systematic reviews and meta-analyses have found that difficulties in cognitive mentalizing, emotional intelligence, and social skills in ASD are comparable to those observed in SCH [36,37,38]. However, differences emerge across age groups, with social and non-social cognitive abilities being more impaired in SCH than in ASD, particularly in the later years of young adulthood [39]. Furthermore, recent dimensional studies have found that autistic and schizotypal traits are similarly linked to poorer social functioning, particularly in the case of autistic traits and negative schizotypy [40, 41]. Moreover, previous studies have found that mentalization-based interventions can improve social functioning in individuals with psychotic disorders [42, 43], and there is preliminary evidence for the efficacy of these interventions in autism as well [44, 45].

The development of mentalizing is highly impacted by social and environmental factors, particularly relationships with other individuals [46], such as secure attachment with the primary caregiver, since internal working models [47] formed within this relationship provide the representational basis for understanding others. In this context, epistemic trust is a key concept, which refers to our innate ability to perceive others as reliable sources of social information. This facilitates the development of flexible mentalizing and stable adult attachments [28]. In adulthood, close relationships with others are described along the dimensions of proximity, dependence, and anxiety, and categorized into secure, avoidant, and anxious/ambivalent attachment styles [48, 49]. Securely attached adults maintain comfortably balanced closeness in intimate relationships, without being anxious about proximity or distance. Avoidant individuals, on the other hand, experience anxiety in intimacy, therefore they typically tend to avoid close relationships. Those with an anxious/ambivalent attachment style have a high need for proximity, while also frequently experiencing fear of abandonment and rejection. The current developmental psychopathology framework of mentalizing considers not only the early parent-child dyadic relationship but also the influence of family, peers, and the broader sociocultural environment [28]. To our knowledge, no study has specifically examined the relationship between mentalizing, adult attachment, and perceived social support. However, higher levels of perceived social support have been linked to increased positive affect, life satisfaction, and favorable outcomes of mental disorders [50,51,52]. Thus, we hypothesized that the amount of perceived social support received from one’s environment is an additional key factor in social-cognitive development, specifically playing a protective role against mentalizing difficulties and insecure attachment.

Although deficits in mentalizing are well-documented and consistently observed in individuals with both ASD and SCH, there have been only a few studies investigating attachment styles. In particular, there is a paucity of research examining the autism and schizophrenia spectrum together in adult populations. Two earlier meta-analyses found that children with ASD exhibit a higher proportion of insecure and disorganized attachment styles compared to their neurotypical peers [53, 54]. Another study [55] reported similar findings in a cross-sectional study of attachment styles among adults with ASD. In a non-clinical sample of adults, it was found that the relationship between autistic traits and attachment avoidance is mediated by lower empathy [56]. Similarly, autistic traits were specifically linked to attachment avoidance in a cross-sectional study of university students [57]. Finally, a recent study indicated that higher attachment anxiety and avoidance mediate the relationship between autistic traits and lower relationship satisfaction [58]. Previous meta-analyses have also highlighted a significant association between psychotic symptoms and insecure attachment styles [59]. Furthermore, psychotic experiences were linked to insecure attachment in clinical, high-risk, and non-clinical samples as well [60, 61]. Overall, it appears that difficulties in attachment and mentalizing might be considered transdiagnostic factors of autism and schizophrenia spectrum.

In this study, our main goal was to investigate the characteristics of attachment and mentalizing in relation to autistic and schizotypal traits, which may be key to understanding the partially common developmental roots of ASD and SCH as manifest psychiatric conditions. The relationship is likely bidirectional and rather complex: autistic and schizotypal traits might both influence and be influenced by attachment styles and mentalizing abilities, suggesting a dynamic interplay rather than a straightforward causal pathway. While secure attachments (both in childhood and adulthood) provide a foundational model for understanding others, mentalizing abilities can also influence the formation and quality of these attachments. However, given the cross-sectional nature of the study, causality cannot be established, and the research focuses on adult attachment styles as outcomes in this interaction. Figure 1 represents the proposed relationships between constructs based on the literature. Combining the research findings presented above, we proposed the following hypotheses: (1) Autistic traits contribute to higher levels of adult attachment anxiety and avoidance, mediated by difficulties in mentalizing [55,56,57,58]. Similarly, we presumed that (2) schizotypal traits are associated with higher levels of adult attachment anxiety and avoidance, mediated by difficulties in mentalizing [35, 59,60,61]. Furthermore, we hypothesized that (3) perceived social support in adolescence is negatively related to difficulties in mentalizing, as well as adult attachment anxiety, and avoidance [28, 50,51,52].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Proposed relationships between constructs. Note: Dashed, bidirectional arrows indicate correlational effects, while continuous, unidirectional arrows denote direct and indirect regression effects. We hypothesized that autistic traits contribute to higher levels of adult attachment anxiety and avoidance, mediated by difficulties in mentalizing. Similarly, we proposed that schizotypal traits (specifically, positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypy) predict adult attachment anxiety and avoidance via difficulties in mentalizing. Furthermore, we expected that perceived social support in adolescence is negatively associated with difficulties in mentalizing as well as adult attachment anxiety and avoidance

Methods

Procedure

The language of the study was Hungarian. For data collection, we used online self-report questionnaires, in a cross-sectional design. Our participants were recruited through various channels, including social media platforms (e.g., Facebook advertisements), outpatient units, and organizations working with ASD individuals (via flyers and caregiver recommendations). Potential participants could contact the research team via email or through a direct link and QR code that led to the questionnaire battery, which was created using formr.org [62], an online survey framework. The advertisements and flyers also included an email address (specifically created for this research project) that participants could use to ask questions and seek further (medical) assistance if needed. Participants completed the survey through the formr.org [62] website, as part of a larger battery of tests. The process began with providing voluntary, informed consent to participate, followed by the questionnaires. The procedure complied with the ethical standards of national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration.

Participants

A total of 2762 individuals agreed to complete the survey, out of which 2269 completed the Mentalization Questionnaire following the items relating to demographic data. Participants were recruited through social media platforms, outpatient units, and organizations working with ASD individuals, using convenience sampling. To ensure the high quality of our data and the authenticity of participants, we have constructed three criteria. First, we excluded participants who did not complete the Mentalization Questionnaire, as it was essential for measuring difficulties in mentalizing, a core variable of our mediation models. Second, we included only individuals aged between 18 and 65 years (N = 2225). Finally, we confirmed that all participants had completed at least eight years of elementary education or provided realistic data regarding their educational history, excluding those who reported more than 30 years of education (N = 2203). In order to explore the entire continuum of autistic and schizotypal traits, no participants were excluded based on their psychiatric diagnosis. Thus, the final sample consisted of 2203 participants, of whom 711 (32.3%) were male, 1434 (65.1%) were female, and 58 (2.6%) identified as another gender. Detailed demographics are presented in Table 1. Informed, voluntary consent was obtained from all participants. The study was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration, and it was approved by the Semmelweis University Regional and Institutional Committee of Science and Research Ethics (approval number: RKEB 158/2021).

Table 1 Detailed demographics of the final sample (N = 2203)

Measures

Autism spectrum quotient (AQ-50)

The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ-50; [63]) was utilized for the assessment of autistic traits. This self-report questionnaire was developed for screening purposes for adults in the average intelligence range. The AQ-50 measures the occurrence of autistic traits across five different domains (social skill, attention switching, attention to detail, communication, and imagination), each consisting of 10 items. Although a Hungarian adaptation has not yet been developed, the reliability and validity of AQ-50 have been demonstrated in numerous studies abroad [4, 65, 66]. In the original version, Baron-Cohen and colleagues [63] reported Cronbach’s α values ranging from 0.63 to 0.77 for the subscales. In the present study, the reliability indices of the AQ-50 demonstrated satisfactory results (Cronbach’s α = 0.887; McDonald’s ω = 0.889).

Multidimensional schizotypy scale – brief (MSS-B)

To examine schizotypal traits, we used the Multidimensional Schizotypy Scale – Brief (MSS-B; [67]). The questionnaire has three subscales corresponding to the main symptom domains of SCH (positive, negative, and disorganized). The total score of the questionnaire cannot be reliably applied, thus the use of separate scores for each of the three subscales is warranted (Cronbach’s α = 0.8–0.9). During the Hungarian adaptation, satisfactory reliability indices were found in a sample of healthy adults (Cronbach’s α = 0.76–0.87) [68]. In the present study, we obtained comparable reliability for the positive (Cronbach’s α = 0.781; McDonald’s ω = 0.779), negative (Cronbach’s α = 0.817; McDonald’s ω = 0.823), and disorganized (Cronbach’s α = 0.891; McDonald’s ω = 0.891) schizotypy scales.

Mentalization questionnaire (MZQ)

The Mentalization Questionnaire (MZQ; [69]) measures various difficulties in mentalizing along four subscales (refusing self-reflection, emotional awareness, psychic equivalence mode, and regulation of affect). In the original study, the MZQ demonstrated high reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.81). The questionnaire was adapted to Hungarian on a sample of individuals affected by psychotic disorders, confirming the original four-factor structure (Cronbach’s α = 0.7–0.9) [70]. In the present study, we obtained satisfactory reliability scores (Cronbach’s α = 0.859; McDonald’s ω = 0.865).

Adult attachment scale (AAS)

The Adult Attachment Scale (AAS; [48]) measures differences in attachment security in intimate relationships during adulthood. The original questionnaire can be divided into subscales of close, depend, and anxiety. However, in the Hungarian version, the alternative factors of attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety proved reliable (Cronbach’s α = 0.77–0.87) [71]. In the present study, the subscales of attachment avoidance (Cronbach’s α = 0.857; McDonald’s ω = 0.871) and attachment anxiety (Cronbach’s α = 0.883; McDonald’s ω = 0.886) demonstrated satisfactory reliability.

Multidimensional scale of perceived social support (MSPSS)

The measurement of perceived social support during adolescence was conducted using a slightly modified version of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS; [72]). In the original instructions, participants were asked to assess the perceived adequacy of social support in a general context. However, in our questionnaire, we assessed the level of perceived social support received during adolescence, specifically between the ages of 14 and 18, aiming to evaluate the extent to which family members, friends, and significant others were perceived as providing psycho-social resources to the individual. The reliability indices of the questionnaire were found to be satisfactory both in the original study (Cronbach’s α = 0.88) and in the Hungarian adaptation (Cronbach’s α = 0.91) [73]. In the present study, the reliability of the MSPSS also demonstrated excellent values (Cronbach’s α = 0.946; McDonald’s ω = 0.946).

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted using JASP 0.16.1 [74]. Data visualization was performed with R [75], RStudio [76] and the packages corrplot [77], ggdist [78], ggthemes [79], Hmisc [80], patchwork [81], and psych [82]. The variables consisted of aggregated scores of the questionnaires (for distributions see Fig. 4). Based on Shapiro-Wilk tests (all p < 0.001) and Q-Q plots, the distribution of the variables did not appear to be normal. Consequently, Spearman’s rank correlation was employed to create a correlation matrix (Fig. 5) between variables, and false discovery rate (FDR) correction was applied to mitigate Type I errors.

We attempted to test our hypotheses using two multivariate mediation models. The separate testing of the two models was justified due to the high multicollinearity between AQ-50 scores and the negative and disorganized subscales of MSS-B, which likely would have resulted in a negative suppressor effect between the predictor variables. In Model A (Fig. 2), we tested the mediating effect of difficulties in mentalizing (MZQ total score), with autistic traits (AQ-50 total score) and perceived social support in adolescence (MSPSS total score) included as predictors, and adult attachment anxiety and avoidance (AAS scores) as outcome variables. To control for potential confounding effects, gender, age, and years of education were included as covariates.

In Model B (Fig. 3), predictor variables included positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypy subscale scores of the MSS-B, and MSPSS total score as a measure of perceived social support in adolescence. Similarly to Model A, the mediator was difficulties in mentalizing (MZQ total score), and outcome variables included adult attachment anxiety and avoidance (AAS scores). Gender, age, and years of education were controlled.

In both models, we used robust indirect effect size indicators to assess the mediating role of difficulties in mentalizing between predictor and outcome variables. A robust Maximum Likelihood Ratio (MLR) method was used to estimate model parameters due to the non-normal distribution of the data.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Model A to test the mediating effects of difficulties in mentalizing. Note: Arrows indicate hypothesized direct and indirect effects. Input variables include autistic traits and perceived social support in adolescence. Outcome variables include adult attachment anxiety and avoidance. Control variables such as gender, age, and years of education are not shown in the figure to maintain readability. We hypothesized that autistic traits contribute to higher levels of adult attachment anxiety and avoidance via difficulties in mentalizing. Conversely, perceived social support in adolescence was assumed to be negatively associated with difficulties in mentalizing as well as attachment anxiety and avoidance

Fig. 3
figure 3

Model B to test the mediating effects of difficulties in mentalizing. Note: Arrows indicate hypothesized direct and indirect effects. Input variables include positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypal traits and perceived social support in adolescence. Outcome variables include adult attachment anxiety and avoidance. Control variables such as gender, age, and years of education are not shown in the figure to maintain readability. We hypothesized that positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypal traits contribute to higher levels of adult attachment anxiety and avoidance via difficulties in mentalizing. Conversely, perceived social support in adolescence was assumed to be negatively associated with difficulties in mentalizing as well as attachment anxiety and avoidance

Results

Table 2 shows the main descriptive statistics of all the questionnaire scores. The visualized distributions of the aggregated questionnaire scores can be seen in Fig. 4. A correlation matrix between variables is displayed in Fig. 5.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics of the questionnaire scores
Fig. 4
figure 4

Distribution plots of questionnaire scores (N = 2203). Note: Boxes show the interquartile range, vertical lines indicate the median and the whiskers show the maximum and minimum values. Abbreviations: AQ-50 = Autism-Spectrum Quotient; MZQ = Mentalization Questionnaire; AAS = Adult Attachment Scale; MSS-B = Multidimensional Schizotypy Scale – Brief; MSPSS = Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support

Fig. 5
figure 5

Correlation matrix of observed variables (N = 2203). Note: Numbers represent Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients (rS). Blue shades indicate positive, while red shades indicate negative correlations. Darker colors represent higher coefficients. Empty squares denote non-significant correlations; *** FDR-corrected p < 0.001. Importantly, moderate to strong positive correlations were observed between AQ-50 and AAS anxiety, AAS avoidance, MSS-B negative, and MSS-B disorganized, which suggested a significant overlap between autistic and schizotypal dimensions. High multicollinearity was observed between AQ-50 and MSS-B negative and disorganized subscales, which justified the separate testing of the two mediation models. MSPSS exhibited a weak to moderate negative correlation with AQ-50, MZQ, AAS anxiety, AAS avoidance, and MSS-B subscales, suggesting the protective nature of perceived social support in adolescence. Abbreviations: AQ-50 = Autism-Spectrum Quotient; MZQ = Mentalization Questionnaire; AAS = Adult Attachment Scale; MSS-B = Multidimensional Schizotypy Scale – Brief; MSPSS = Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support

Figure 6 shows the results of Model A mediation analysis. A significant and strong relationship was observed between autistic traits and difficulties in mentalizing. In contrast, a significant weak negative association was found between the level of perceived social support in adolescence and difficulties in mentalizing. A strong significant association was found between difficulties in mentalizing and adult attachment anxiety, and a significant moderate positive relationship was found with attachment avoidance. Additionally, autistic traits showed a significant positive moderate direct relationship with adult attachment avoidance. Weak negative direct associations were found between perceived social support during adolescence and adult attachment anxiety and avoidance.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Results of mediation Model A. Note: Difficulties in mentalizing mediated the associations between autistic traits and perceived social support in adolescence as input variables and adult attachment anxiety and avoidance as outcome variables. A higher prevalence of autistic traits was linked to more severe difficulties in mentalizing, which were related to higher levels of adult attachment anxiety and avoidance. Conversely, higher levels of perceived social support in adolescence were associated with less severe difficulties in mentalizing, related to a more moderate level of adult attachment anxiety and avoidance. Standardized regression coefficients (β) are shown on the arrows. Continuous lines indicate significant, and dotted lines indicate non-significant regression effects. R2 = explained variance of the variable; *** p < 0.001

We found a moderate significant indirect effect of autistic traits and a small significant indirect effect of perceived social support in adolescence on adult attachment anxiety through difficulties in mentalizing. It is important to highlight that there was full mediation between autistic traits and attachment anxiety through difficulties in mentalizing. There were also significant weak indirect associations between adult attachment avoidance and autistic traits, as well as perceived social support in adolescence. Indirect effect size indicators are detailed in Table 3.

Table 3 Detailed total, direct, and indirect effect size indicators of mediation analyses

To summarize results yielded by Model A, difficulties in mentalizing mediated the effects of autistic traits and perceived social support during adolescence on adult attachment anxiety and avoidance. Specifically, more prevalent autistic traits were associated with more difficulties in mentalizing, which, in turn, corresponded to higher levels of adult attachment anxiety and avoidance. On the other hand, a greater amount of perceived social support during adolescence was related to fewer difficulties in mentalizing, resulting in lower levels of attachment anxiety and avoidance in adulthood.

Results from Model B mediation analysis are shown in Fig. 7. Positive schizotypal traits showed a significant weak direct association with difficulties in mentalizing as well as adult attachment avoidance. Negative schizotypy exhibited a significant weak positive relationship with difficulties in mentalizing, and a significant moderate positive direct relationship with attachment avoidance. However, the association between negative schizotypy and attachment anxiety did not prove to be significant. Disorganized schizotypal traits exerted significant weak direct effects on adult attachment anxiety, and significant moderate positive effects on difficulties in mentalizing. A somewhat unexpected finding was the significant negative direct relationship between adult attachment avoidance disorganized schizotypal traits. Nevertheless, considering the strength and direction of the total effect, this can be considered a statistical artifact. Additionally, we found significant weak negative direct associations between perceived social support during adolescence and difficulties in mentalizing, as well as adult attachment anxiety and avoidance.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Results of mediation Model B. Note: More prevalent positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypal traits were associated with higher levels of adult attachment avoidance (and attachment anxiety for positive and disorganized schizotypy), partially through the mediating effects of difficulties in mentalizing. Conversely, higher levels of perceived social support in adolescence were associated with less severe difficulties in mentalizing, as well as lower attachment anxiety and avoidance. Standardized regression coefficients (β) are shown on the arrows. Continuous lines indicate significant, and dotted lines indicate non-significant regression effects. R2 = explained variance of the variable; *** p < 0.001; ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05

Positive and disorganized schizotypy, as well as perceived social support during adolescence, showed a significant weak association with adult attachment anxiety, mediated by difficulties in mentalizing. Adult attachment avoidance showed weak indirect relationships with positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypal traits, as well as perceived social support in adolescence. Specific indirect effect size indicators are detailed in Table 3.

Overall, in Model B, difficulties in mentalizing fully mediated the effect of positive schizotypy on adult attachment anxiety. Furthermore, partial mediation was observed through mentalizing in the case of disorganized schizotypy and perceived social support in adolescence. Similarly, partial mediation was found through mentalizing between positive and negative schizotypy, perceived social support during adolescence as predictor variables, and adult attachment avoidance as the outcome variable. A full mediation was observed between disorganized schizotypy and attachment avoidance through difficulties in mentalizing. Specifically, higher levels of positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypy were associated with more severe difficulties in mentalizing, thereby leading to more prevalent adult attachment avoidance (and attachment anxiety for positive and disorganized schizotypal traits). In contrast, higher perceived social support was associated with less prevalent difficulties in mentalizing, resulting in lower adult attachment anxiety and avoidance.

Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate the similarities between autism and schizophrenia spectrum by exploring difficulties in mentalizing and attachment, which may be key to uncovering their shared developmental roots. By taking a dimensional perspective, we measured autistic and schizotypal features as extended, disorder-liability traits of ASD and SCH. Our results provided evidence that autistic and schizotypal traits are associated with impairments in mentalizing and insecure adult attachment in comparable ways.

Previous research has consistently found deficits in mentalizing and a higher prevalence of insecure attachment styles in adulthood in both ASD and SCH [28, 57, 61]. Further, perceived social support during social development was suggested to be a key protective factor [51, 52]. Our results show that autistic and schizotypal traits might be risk factors for insecure adult attachment (higher anxiety and avoidance), through difficulties in mentalizing. More specifically, while autistic traits seem to mainly contribute to adult attachment anxiety through challenges in mentalizing, negative schizotypy relates to attachment avoidance, and disorganized schizotypal traits are linked to attachment anxiety. Conversely, perceived social support in adolescence was negatively linked to impairments in mentalizing and insecure adult attachment, serving as a protective factor during social-cognitive development.

Autistic traits were found to be linked to difficulties in mentalizing and insecure adult attachment, consistent with prior research revealing mentalizing impairments in autistic individuals [30, 83, 84]. Importantly, we found a stronger direct than indirect link between autistic traits and adult attachment avoidance. Conversely, the link between autistic traits and attachment anxiety was more indirect, with mentalizing fully mediating this relationship. This aligns partially with previous findings on healthy adults, which demonstrated the mediating effect of lower empathy between autistic traits and attachment avoidance [56]. However, this previous study only found a direct association between autistic traits and attachment anxiety. The divergence in findings may stem from methodological differences, as the previous study exclusively assessed one facet of mentalizing, empathy, using the unidimensional Empathy Quotient–Short [85], which focuses on inferring the emotional states of others [27]. In contrast, we employed the comprehensive Mentalization Questionnaire [69], covering four domains of mentalizing impairments. Additionally, the prior study employed a smaller sample size and different measures, specifically, the Broad Autism Phenotype Questionnaire [86] for autistic traits and the Experiences in Close Relationships–Revised [87] for attachment. Another study demonstrated a specific relationship between autistic traits and adult attachment avoidance in a normative sample [57], consistent with our findings. Here, difficulties in mentalizing fully explained the relationship between autistic traits and attachment anxiety, whereas attachment avoidance showed only partial mediation. This suggests that individuals with more prevalent autistic traits experience increased anxiety in interpersonal relationships due to difficulties in mentalizing.

The three dimensions of schizotypy (positive, negative, and disorganized) showed distinct associations with mentalizing and adult attachment. Positive schizotypy showed a weak association with adult attachment anxiety and avoidance through difficulties in mentalizing, consistent with the findings of a recent meta-analysis [88]. Positive schizotypy is also directly linked to adult attachment anxiety and avoidance, aligning with previous literature [61]. However, a large-scale network analysis [68] failed to identify positive traits as a central component in the relationship between schizotypy, mentalizing, and attachment. These results are unexpected, given that in cases of clinical SCH, paranoid delusions and hallucinations have been linked to deficits in mentalizing [34, 89, 90]. However, this inconsistency may be resolved by the “happy schizotypy” concept [91], which posits a subgroup characterized by positive schizotypal traits, high adaptation and creativity, and low scores on negative and disorganized dimensions [92]. Since our participants mostly lacked a psychiatric diagnosis of SCH, we can assume that positive schizotypal traits entail fewer mentalizing difficulties in their case.

Negative schizotypal traits were found to be related to adult attachment avoidance through difficulties in mentalizing, aligning with previous literature [88, 93, 94]. Furthermore, a study identified an association between negative schizotypy and avoidant attachment in samples of healthy young adults from the USA and Spain [95]. Additionally, a recent network analysis of autistic and schizotypal traits in the context of social functioning revealed a substantial overlap between autistic traits and negative schizotypy [40], both of which were found to be similarly linked to difficulties in mentalizing in our models. It is conceivable that individuals characterized by high levels of negative schizotypy exhibit difficulties in the motivational aspect of social engagement in the context of mentalizing and attachment, with decreased responsiveness to social stimuli. Disorganized schizotypy was associated with both adult attachment anxiety and avoidance, mediated by difficulties in mentalizing. Previous research suggested that disorganized traits are a central component of schizotypy [96], are associated with poorer cognitive and emotional control functions [97], and inversely relate to mentalizing [68]. Consequently, individuals with higher levels of disorganized schizotypy may experience difficulties in maintaining and tracking conversations with others, as well as integrating social cues from various sources (e.g., gestures, facial expressions, tone of voice, and verbal content). This may further complicate multi-actor interactions, leading to cumulative mentalizing errors without successfully coping with their emotional consequences due to impaired affective control functions. Repeated negative experiences in social interactions can result in attachment anxiety, and, particularly when associated with negative traits, avoidance of intimate relationships.

Perceived social support in adolescence showed a negative relationship with both difficulties in mentalizing and insecure adult attachment (anxiety and avoidance). This finding is consistent with previous literature that identified social support as a protective factor in the outcome of mental disorders [52]. Perceived social support enhances positive emotions, reduces negative ones, and contributes to overall life satisfaction, thereby contributing to subjective well-being [51]. Some authors suggest that social support directly impacts general mental health, while others consider it a specific resource that promotes coping with stress [73]. Within the mentalizing approach, it appears that perceived social support provides an environment that facilitates epistemic trust, which is our pre-programmed ability to perceive others as reliable sources of information, thereby expanding our knowledge of the world [28]. Epistemic trust contributes to the development of flexible mentalizing, and accordingly, perceived social support in adolescence can be considered a protective factor against adult difficulties in mentalizing and insecure attachment.

Summarizing the above results, both autistic and schizotypal traits appear to play a crucial role in the development of mentalizing difficulties and insecure adult attachment. The results might suggest a developmental trajectory in which autistic and schizotypal traits reflect atypical neurodevelopment [4,5,6,7], a liability for ASD and SCH, respectively. These features might influence social-cognitive development throughout life, resulting in ineffective mentalizing, further consolidating attachment anxiety, and avoidance [28]. However, it is an area for future longitudinal research to confirm these possible trajectories, as our study utilized cross-sectional data. Our findings seem to align with previous research on the neural underpinnings of ASD and SCH, highlighting their similarities. Mentalizing difficulties and anxious-avoidant attachment in ASD may be due to atypical amygdala functioning and connectivity [83, 98], as the neural circuits connecting the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex play a key role in balanced mentalizing [29]. Similarly, individuals with SCH exhibit altered threat processing [99] and decreased emotion recognition, also linked to atypical amygdala functioning [100, 101]. We could hypothesize that individuals with more prevalent autistic and schizotypal traits may experience inadequate amygdala activation, less controlled mentalizing, increased anxiety, and mentalizing errors in interpersonal situations. In the long term, this may result in the development of more insecure attachment styles, specifically, anxious attachment, in adulthood. However, as our study did not include neuroimaging to measure amygdala activation, we propose these notions as areas for future research that should combine self-report and neural measures. Additionally, it may be beneficial to conduct separate analyses for the clinical population after further data collection, as the proposed relationships could exhibit different patterns (e.g. quadratic) across the autistic and schizotypal spectrum.

Our findings are an important contribution to the literature which considers psychopathological phenomena from a dimensional perspective. This approach has shown greater reliability and validity compared to categorical, diagnosis-based measures [26]. However, a limitation lies in the cross-sectional nature of the study, which hinders the possibility to draw causal inferences and calls for longitudinal and experimental designs to validate our results. Additionally, the use of online self-report questionnaires may have biased the final composition of our sample, as individuals at the more severe end of psychopathology dimensions may have failed to fill out the questionnaires completely, resulting in dropout due to incomplete scores. Furthermore, self-reports might be biased by the participants’ subjective views of themselves. Moreover, the use of online assessment methods, and socioeconomic makeup of our sample further limit the generalizability of our results, as individuals with greater support needs (potentially lacking internet access) could not be included in our study. Considering the diagnostic composition of our sample, there is an overrepresentation of individuals with ASD compared to the general population. Additionally, our sample is likely skewed due to the recruitment of patients primarily through outpatient units, where individuals are more likely to be actively seeking help. The gender demographics of our sample also do not reflect those of the autistic population, with our sample predominantly consisting of females, whereas autistic populations typically have a male-to-female ratio of two to three times higher [31]. Additionally, elevated AQ-50 and MSS-B scores indicate autistic and schizotypal traits rather than being specific to ASD or SCH, cautioning against concluding that our findings are specific to these conditions [7, 63]. While we acknowledge that some findings may overlap with general psychopathological frameworks [102], our goal was to contribute to a more detailed understanding of how these features manifest uniquely or similarly within these conditions. Furthermore, our models did not differentiate between difficulties in dimensions of mentalizing (e.g., cognitive or affective), suggesting another area for future research.

Our findings have important practical implications for the support of individuals with ASD and SCH spectrum disorders. For example, the characteristics of attachment highly influence the patient-therapist collaboration in psychotherapy [103], as well as adherence to pharmacotherapy [104, 105]. Considering the specific mentalizing and attachment characteristics of individuals enables more personalized and effective healthcare, as these interventions necessarily occur through social interactions. Additionally, mentalizing can serve as a target of novel therapeutic interventions which aim to ameliorate the social functioning of individuals, as there is evidence for the efficacy of mentalization-based interventions in both ASD [44, 45] and SCH [42, 43].

Conclusions

In conclusion, our findings on the effect of mentalizing and social support on adult attachment emphasize the essential role of supportive therapeutic relationships and community care in autism and schizophrenia spectrum conditions. Overall, this study has contributed to a deeper understanding of autistic and schizotypal dimensions as well as their overlaps and could take us one step further towards understanding the brain correlates and developmental roots of ASD and SCH.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Abbreviations

AAS:

Adult Attachment Scale

AQ-50:

Autism-Spectrum Quotient

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

FDR:

False discovery rate

MLR:

Maximum likelihood ratio

MSPSS:

Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support

MSS-B:

Multidimensional Schizotypy Scale – Brief

MZQ:

Mentalization Questionnaire

SCH:

Schizophrenia spectrum disorder

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical Manual of Mental disorders: DSM-5-TR. 5th ed. text rev: Author; 2022.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Isohanni M, Jones PB, Moilanen K, Rantakallio P, Veijola J, Oja H, Koiranen M, Jokelainen J, Croudace T, Järvelin M-R. Early developmental milestones in adult schizophrenia and other psychoses. A 31-year follow-up of the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort. Schizophr Res. 2001;52:1–19.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. McCutcheon RA, Reis Marques T, Howes OD. Schizophrenia—An overview. JAMA Psychiatry. 2020;77:201–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Ruzich E, Allison C, Smith P, Watson P, Auyeung B, Ring H, Baron-Cohen S. Measuring autistic traits in the general population: a systematic review of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) in a nonclinical population sample of 6,900 typical adult males and females. Mol Autism. 2015;6:2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Wainer AL, Ingersoll BR, Hopwood CJ. The structure and nature of the broader autism phenotype in a non-clinical sample. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2011;33:459–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Ettinger U, Meyhöfer I, Steffens M, Wagner M, Koutsouleris N, Genetics. Cognition, and Neurobiology of Schizotypal personality: a review of the overlap with Schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry. 2014;5:18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Nelson MT, Seal ML, Pantelis C, Phillips LJ. Evidence of a dimensional relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37:317–27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. De Crescenzo F, Postorino V, Siracusano M, Riccioni A, Armando M, Curatolo P, Mazzone L. Autistic symptoms in Schizophrenia Spectrum disorders: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:78.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Dinsdale NL, Hurd PL, Wakabayashi A, Elliot M, Crespi BJ. How are autism and Schizotypy Related? Evidence from a Non-clinical Population. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e63316.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Ford TC, Apputhurai P, Meyer D, Crewther DP. Confirmatory factor analysis of autism and schizophrenia spectrum traits. Personal Individ Differ. 2017;110:80–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Kincaid DL, Doris M, Shannon C, Mulholland C. What is the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder and ASD traits in psychosis? A systematic review. Psychiatry Res. 2017;250:99–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. King BH, Lord C. Is schizophrenia on the autism spectrum? Brain Res. 2011;1380:34–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Ferrara M, Curtarello EMA, Simonelli G, Yoviene Sykes LA, Fusar-Poli L, Folesani F, Belvederi Murri M, Aguglia E, Politi P, Grassi L. First-episode psychosis and autism spectrum disorder: a scoping review and a guide to overcome diagnostic challenges. Int J Ment Health. 2024;53:4–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Hossain MM, Khan N, Sultana A, Ma P, McKyer ELJ, Ahmed HU, Purohit N. Prevalence of comorbid psychiatric disorders among people with autism spectrum disorder: an umbrella review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Psychiatry Res. 2020;287:112922.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Gomes DR, Silveira F, Freitas R. Psychosis in autism spectrum disorder: a clinical review. Eur Psychiatry. 2023;66:S754–5.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Varcin KJ, Herniman SE, Lin A, Chen Y, Perry Y, Pugh C, Chisholm K, Whitehouse AJO, Wood SJ. Occurrence of psychosis and bipolar disorder in adults with autism: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2022;134:104543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Boada L, Lahera G, Pina-Camacho L, Merchán-Naranjo J, Díaz-Caneja CM, Bellón JM, Ruiz-Vargas JM, Parellada M. Social Cognition in Autism and Schizophrenia Spectrum disorders: the same but different? J Autism Dev Disord. 2020;50:3046–59.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Chisholm K, Lin A, Abu-Akel A, Wood SJ. The association between autism and schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a review of eight alternate models of co-occurrence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;55:173–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Sugranyes G, Kyriakopoulos M, Corrigall R, Taylor E, Frangou S. Autism Spectrum disorders and Schizophrenia: Meta-Analysis of the neural correlates of Social Cognition. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e25322.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Schalbroeck R, Foss-Feig JH, Jutla A, Ziermans TB. Integrating neuropsychological research on autism and psychosis to improve clinical outcomes. Nat Rev Psychol. 2023;2:723–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Nair A, Jolliffe M, Lograsso YSS, Bearden CE. A review of Default Mode Network Connectivity and Its Association with Social Cognition in adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder and early-onset psychosis. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:614.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Chirica MG, Zhu Y, Mu W, Zhou H, Gong J, Chan RCK, Kwapil TR, Berenbaum H. Exploring phenotypic overlap across schizotypy and autism spectrum conditions in American and Chinese young adults. Schizophr Res. 2024;267:359–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Crespi B, Badcock C. Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain. Behav Brain Sci. 2008;31:241–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Abu-Akel AM, Apperly IA, Wood SJ, Hansen PC. Autism and psychosis expressions diametrically modulate the right temporoparietal junction. Soc Neurosci. 2017;12:506–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Borsboom D, Rhemtulla M, Cramer AOJ, van der Maas HLJ, Scheffer M, Dolan CV. Kinds versus continua: a review of psychometric approaches to uncover the structure of psychiatric constructs. Psychol Med. 2016;46:1567–79.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Markon KE, Chmielewski M, Miller CJ. The reliability and validity of discrete and continuous measures of psychopathology: a quantitative review. Psychol Bull. 2011;137:856–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Allen JG, Fonagy P, Bateman AW. Mentalizing in clinical practice. American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Luyten P, Campbell C, Allison E, Fonagy P. The Mentalizing Approach to Psychopathology: state of the art and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2020;16:297–325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Luyten P, Fonagy P. The neurobiology of mentalizing. Personal Disord Theory Res Treat. 2015;6:366–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Baron-Cohen S. Theory of mind and autism: a review. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation. Elsevier; 2000. pp. 169–84.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Lai M-C, Lombardo MV, Baron-Cohen S. Autism Lancet. 2014;383:896–910.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Velikonja T, Fett A-K, Velthorst E. Patterns of Nonsocial and social cognitive functioning in adults with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76:135–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2009;109:1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Frith CD. Schizophrenia and theory of mind. Psychol Med. 2004;34:385–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Debbané M, Salaminios G, Luyten P, Badoud D, Armando M, Solida Tozzi A, Fonagy P, Brent BK. Attachment, Neurobiology, and Mentalizing along the psychosis Continuum. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:406.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Fernandes JM, Cajão R, Lopes R, Jerónimo R, Barahona-Corrêa JB. Social Cognition in Schizophrenia and Autism Spectrum disorders: a systematic review and Meta-analysis of direct comparisons. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:504.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Barlati S, Minelli A, Ceraso A, Nibbio G, Carvalho Silva R, Deste G, Turrina C, Vita A. Social Cognition in a Research Domain Criteria Perspective: a Bridge between Schizophrenia and Autism Spectra disorders. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:806.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Oliver LD, Moxon-Emre I, Lai M-C, Grennan L, Voineskos AN, Ameis SH. Social Cognitive performance in Schizophrenia Spectrum disorders compared with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review, Meta-analysis, and Meta-regression. JAMA Psychiatry. 2021;78:281–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Ozbek SU, Sut E, Bora E. Comparison of social cognition and neurocognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2023;155:105441.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Hajdúk M, Straková A, Januška J, et al. Connections between and within extended psychosis and autistic phenotypes and social relationships in the general population. J Psychiatr Res. 2023;157:36–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Isvoranu A-M, Ziermans T, Schirmbeck F, et al. Autistic symptoms and Social Functioning in Psychosis: A Network Approach. Schizophr Bull. 2022;48:273–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Brent BK, Holt DJ, Keshavan MS, Seidman LJ, Fonagy P. Mentalization-based treatment for psychosis: linking an attachment-based model to the psychotherapy for impaired mental state understanding in people with psychotic disorders. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci. 2014;51:17–24.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Weijers J, Ten Kate C, Viechtbauer W, Rampaart LJA, Eurelings EHM, Selten JP. Mentalization-based treatment for psychotic disorder: a rater-blinded, multi-center, randomized controlled trial. Psychol Med. 2021;51:2846–55.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Holopainen A, De Veld DMJ, Hoddenbach E, Begeer S. Does theory of mind training enhance Empathy in Autism? J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49:3965–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Krämer K, Vetter A, Schultz-Venrath U, Vogeley K, Reul S. Mentalization-based treatment in groups for adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front Psychol. 2021;12:708557.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Carpendale JIM, Lewis C. Constructing an understanding of mind: the development of children’s social understanding within social interaction. Behav Brain Sci. 2004;27:79–151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss, Vol. 1: Attachment. Basic Books; 1969.

  48. Collins NL. Working models of attachment: implications for explanation, emotion, and Behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;71:810–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Hazan C, Shaver P. Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987;52:511–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Kaplan BH, Cassel JC, Gore S. Social support and health. Med Care. 1977;15:47–58.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Siedlecki KL, Salthouse TA, Oishi S, Jeswani S. The relationship between Social Support and Subjective Well-Being Across Age. Soc Indic Res. 2014;117:561–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Wang J, Mann F, Lloyd-Evans B, Ma R, Johnson S. Associations between loneliness and perceived social support and outcomes of mental health problems: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry. 2018;18:156.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Rutgers AH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, Berckelaer-Onnes IA. Autism and attachment: a meta-analytic review. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45:1123–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. van IJzendoorn MH, Rutgers AH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Swinkels SHN, van Daalen E, Dietz C, Naber FBA, Buitelaar JK, van Engeland H. Parental sensitivity and attachment in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: comparison with Children with Mental Retardation, with Language Delays, and with typical development. Child Dev. 2007;78:597–608.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Taylor EL, Target M, Charman T. Attachment in adults with high-functioning autism. Attach Hum Dev. 2008;10:143–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Lamport D, Turner LA. Romantic attachment, Empathy, and the Broader Autism Phenotype among College Students. J Genet Psychol. 2014;175:202–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Gallitto E, Leth-Steensen C. Autistic traits and adult attachment styles. Personal Individ Differ. 2015;79:63–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Beffel JH, Cary KM, Nuttall AK, Chopik WJ, Maas MK. Associations between the broad autism phenotype, adult attachment, and relationship satisfaction among emerging adults. Personal Individ Differ. 2021;168:110409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Korver-Nieberg N, Berry K, Meijer CJ, de Haan L. Adult attachment and psychotic phenomenology in clinical and non-clinical samples: a systematic review. Psychol Psychother Theory Res Pract. 2013;87:127–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Boldrini T, Pontillo M, Tanzilli A, Giovanardi G, Di Cicilia G, Salcuni S, Vicari S, Lingiardi V. An attachment perspective on the risk for psychosis: clinical correlates and the predictive value of attachment patterns and mentalization. Schizophr Res. 2020;222:209–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Carr SC, Hardy A, Fornells-Ambrojo M. Relationship between attachment style and symptom severity across the psychosis spectrum: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2018;59:145–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Arslan RC, Walther MP, Tata CS. Formr: a study framework allowing for automated feedback generation and complex longitudinal experience-sampling studies using R. Behav Res Methods. 2020;52:376–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger Syndrome/High-Functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:5–17.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Woodbury-Smith M, Robinson J, Wheelwright S, Baron-Cohen S. Screening adults for Asperger Syndrome using the AQ: a preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical practice. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005;35:331–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, Cath DC, Boomsma DI. Factor structure, reliability and Criterion Validity of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): a study in Dutch Population and Patient groups. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:1555–66.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Wakabayashi A, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Tojo Y. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) in Japan: a cross-cultural comparison. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36:263–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Gross GM, Kwapil TR, Raulin ML, Silvia PJ, Barrantes-Vidal N. The multidimensional schizotypy scale-brief: scale development and psychometric properties. Psychiatry Res. 2018;261:7–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Polner B, Farkas K, Keri S. A network model of the aetiology of schizotypal personality: evidence for the central role of mentalization, disorganization, and PTSD-symptoms (preprint). PsyArXiv. 2021. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/328an.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Hausberg MC, Schulz H, Piegler T, Happach CG, Klöpper M, Brütt AL, Sammet I, Andreas S. Is a self-rated instrument appropriate to assess mentalization in patients with mental disorders? Development and first validation of the mentalization questionnaire (MZQ). Psychother Res. 2012;22:699–709.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Fekete K, Török E, Kelemen O, Makkos Z, Csigó K. A mentalizáció dimenziói pszichotikus zavarokban [Dimensions of mentalization in psychotic disorders]. Neuropsychopharmacol Hung. 2019;21:5–11.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Őri D, Kapornai K, Baji I, Kiss E. A Collins–Read Felnőtt Kötődési Skála Pszichometriai jellemzőinek vizsgálata [Psychometric properties of the Hungarian adult attachment Scale]. Ideggyógy Szle. 2021;74:87–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Zimet GD, Dahlem NW, Zimet SG, Farley GK. The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. J Pers Assess. 1988;52:30–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Papp-Zipernovszky O, Kékesi MZ, Jámbori S. A Multidimenzionális Észlelt Társas Támogatás Kérdőív magyar nyelvű validálása [Validation of the Hungarian version of Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support]. Mentálhig És Pszichoszomatika. 2017;18:230–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Team JASP. JASP. 2022. https://jasp-stats.org/.

  75. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. 2022. https://www.R-project.org/.

  76. Posit team. RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R. 2023. http://www.posit.co/.

  77. Wei T, Simko V. R package ’corrplot’: Visualization of a Correlation Matrix. 2021. https://github.com/taiyun/corrplot.

  78. Kay Mggdist. Visualizations of Distributions and Uncertainty. 2023. https://mjskay.github.io/ggdist/.

  79. Arnold Jggthemes. Extra Themes, Scales and Geoms for ggplot2. 2021. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ggthemes.

  80. Harrell F Jr, Hmisc. Harrell Miscellaneous. 2022. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=Hmisc.

  81. Pedersen T, patchwork. The Composer of Plots. 2022. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=patchwork.

  82. Revelle W. psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research. 2022. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych.

  83. Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Bullmore ET, Wheelwright S, Ashwin C, Williams SCR. The amygdala theory of autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24:355–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Spek AA, Scholte EM, Van Berckelaer-Onnes IA. Theory of mind in adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40:280–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Wakabayashi A, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Goldenfeld N, Delaney J, Fine D, Smith R, Weil L. Development of short forms of the Empathy Quotient (EQ-Short) and the Systemizing Quotient (SQ-Short). Personal Individ Differ. 2006;41:929–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Hurley RSE, Losh M, Parlier M, Reznick JS, Piven J. The broad autism phenotype questionnaire. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:1679–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Fraley RC, Waller NG, Brennan KA. An item response theory analysis of self-report measures of adult attachment. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78:350–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Bora E. Theory of mind and schizotypy: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2020;222:97–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Finn ES, Corlett PR, Chen G, Bandettini PA, Constable RT. Trait paranoia shapes inter-subject synchrony in brain activity during an ambiguous social narrative. Nat Commun. 2018;9:2043.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Montag C, Dziobek I, Richter IS, Neuhaus K, Lehmann A, Sylla R, Heekeren HR, Heinz A, Gallinat J. Different aspects of theory of mind in paranoid schizophrenia: evidence from a video-based assessment. Psychiatry Res. 2011;186:203–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Mohr C, Claridge G. Schizotypy — do not worry, it is not all Worrisome. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41:S436–43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  92. Hori H, Teraishi T, Sasayama D, Matsuo J, Kinoshita Y, Ota M, Hattori K, Kunugi H. A latent profile analysis of schizotypy, temperament and character in a nonclinical population: Association with neurocognition. J Psychiatr Res. 2014;48:56–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Berry K, Wearden A, Barrowclough C, Liversidge T. Attachment styles, interpersonal relationships and psychotic phenomena in a non-clinical student sample. Personal Individ Differ. 2006;41:707–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Meins E, Jones SR, Fernyhough C, Hurndall S, Koronis P. Attachment dimensions and schizotypy in a non-clinical sample. Personal Individ Differ. 2008;44:1000–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Sheinbaum T, Bedoya E, Ros-Morente A, Kwapil TR, Barrantes-Vidal N. Association between attachment prototypes and schizotypy dimensions in two independent non-clinical samples of Spanish and American young adults. Psychiatry Res. 2013;210:408–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Polner B, Faiola E, Urquijo MF, Meyhöfer I, Steffens M, Rónai L, Koutsouleris N, Ettinger U. The network structure of schizotypy in the general population. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2021;271:635–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Kerns JG. Schizotypy facets, cognitive control, and emotion. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115:418–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Hennessey T, Andari E, Rainnie DG. RDoC-based categorization of amygdala functions and its implications in autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;90:115–29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  99. Williams LM. Dysregulation of Arousal and Amygdala-Prefrontal Systems in paranoid Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:480–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Aleman A, Kahn R. Strange feelings: do amygdala abnormalities dysregulate the emotional brain in schizophrenia? Prog Neurobiol. 2005;77:283–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Bjorkquist OA, Olsen EK, Nelson BD, Herbener ES. Altered amygdala-prefrontal connectivity during emotion perception in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2016;175:35–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, et al. The p factor: one General psychopathology factor in the structure of Psychiatric disorders? Clin Psychol Sci. 2014;2:119–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. Daniel SIF. Adult attachment patterns and individual psychotherapy: a review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26:968–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Bennett JK, Fuertes JN, Keitel M, Phillips R. The role of patient attachment and working alliance on patient adherence, satisfaction, and health-related quality of life in lupus treatment. Patient Educ Couns. 2011;85:53–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Nanjappa S, Chambers S, Marcenes W, Richards D, Freeman R. A theory led narrative review of one-to-one health interventions: the influence of attachment style and client-provider relationship on client adherence. Health Educ Res. 2014;29:740–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by the ÚNKP-22-2-III-ELTE-328 New National Excellence Program of the Ministry for Culture and Innovation from the source of the National Research, Development and Innovation Fund (to DS); by the Hungarian National Research, Development and Innovation Office Grant OTKA PD 146424 (to KF); by the Ministry of Innovation and Technology of Hungary from the National Research, Development and Innovation Fund, financed under the TKP2021-EGA-25 funding scheme (to ÁV and KF); by the ANR grant awarded within the framework of the Inserm CPJ (ANR-22-CPJ1-0042-01) (to DN); and the National Brain Research Program by Hungarian Academy of Sciences (project NAP2022-I-1/2022) (to DN).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

D.S.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Writing - Original Draft, Visualization. Á.V.: Formal analysis, Investigation, Resources, Data Curation, Review & Editing, Visualization. D.N.: Review & Editing. K.F.: Conceptualization, Investigation, Resources, Data Curation, Writing - Review & Editing, Supervision, Project administration.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kinga Farkas.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the Semmelweis University Regional and Institutional Committee of Science and Research Ethics (approval number: RKEB 158/2021). Informed consent was obtained from all subjects and/or their legal guardian(s).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sörnyei, D., Vass, Á., Németh, D. et al. Autistic and schizotypal traits exhibit similarities in their impact on mentalization and adult attachment impairments: a cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry 24, 654 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-024-06048-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-024-06048-9

Keywords