Skip to main content

A meta-analysis on neural changes of cognitive training for mental disorders in executive function tasks: increase or decrease brain activation?

Abstract

Background

Cognitive impairment is often found in patients with psychiatric disorders, and cognitive training (CT) has been shown to help these patients. To better understand the mechanisms of CT, many neuroimaging studies have investigated the neural changes associated with it. However, the results of those studies have been inconsistent, making it difficult to draw conclusions from the literature. Therefore, the objective of this meta-analysis was to identify consistent patterns in the literature of neural changes associated with CT for psychiatric disorders.

Methods

We searched for cognitive training imaging studies in PubMed, Cochrane library, Scopus, and ProQuest electronic databases. We conducted an activation likelihood estimation (ALE) for coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies, conduct behavioral analysis of brain regions identified by ALE analysis, conduct behavioral analysis of brain regions identified by ALE analysis, and then created a functional meta-analytic connectivity model (fMACM) of the resulting regions.

Results

Results showed that CT studies consistently reported increased activation in the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and decreased activation in the left precuneus and cuneus from pre- to post- CT.

Conclusion

CT improves cognitive function by supporting language and memory function, and reducing neuronal resources associated with basic visual processing.

Peer Review reports

Background

The prevalence and disease burden of psychiatric disorders have remained incredibly high globally, with a total prevalence rate of about 30.5% [1]. Common to many psychiatric disorders is a substantial decline in cognitive function [2]. Cognitive training (CT), also known as cognitive remediation or cognitive remediation therapy, includes interventions in which patients repeatedly perform cognitive tasks in order to improve their cognitive abilities [3]. CT is an effective means of improving neuropsychological deficits in many different populations [4, 5].

The effects of CT on brain function have been studied extensively using neuroimaging techniques. Neuroimaging studies indicate that CT is associated with structural and functional alterations [6,7,8]. Most functional neuroimaging studies of CT focus on executive function. Executive functions are a collection of cognitive processes that help us to regulate our thoughts and behaviours to make plans, solve problems, and attain goals [9]. Major components of executive functioning include attention, inhibition, self-regulation, working memory, cognitive flexibility, planning, organization, problem-solving, and performance-monitoring [10]. A series of studies have shown that CT can improve executive function in patients with psychiatric disorders [11,12,13]. However, these studies do not always report concordant results, making it difficult to draw conclusions about the neural changes associated with CT. Recently, some meta-analysis and review suggests that cognitive training can improve the cognitive function of schizophrenia by activating the frontal brain regions [14, 15].

To infer a reliable conclusion from disparate results requires appropriate statistical methods. Meta-analysis is one method used by researchers to objectively identify reliable effects across the literature by pooling results across many studies to test for a significant convergence of findings [16]. Meta-analytic techniques can be used to build models and detect emergent properties of neural systems through large-scale data mining and computational modeling [17].

The objective of the present study was to use meta-analyses to identify neural changes associated with CT that are common across psychiatric disorders. Despite different neuropathology that may be a part of a specific disorder, it is important to understand common effects of CT on neural circuitry. If the results of this meta-analysis show both increased and decreased brain activation, we can infer that CT improves cognitive function through more than one pathway. Furthermore, the current study analyzed functional interactions between regions implicated in the meta-analysis, thereby identifying brain networks that are modified following CT, and providing clues regarding its mechanism of action.

Methods

Literature search and selection

We searched for studies from electronic databases including PubMed, Cochrane library, Scopus, and ProQuest. We used the following search criteria: Title/Abstract “cognitive training or cognitive remediation or cognitive rehabilitation or cognitive stimulation” and “MRI or magnetic resonance imaging or fMRI or functional magnetic resonance imaging or PET or positron emission tomography or SPECT or single photon emission computed /tomography”. Reviews were excluded.

For the papers resulting from this search, we applied the following inclusion criteria [18]: (1) Participants included patients with a diagnosed psychiatric disorder (2) The paper was a study of executive function (3) A task was presented during image acquisition (4) The analysis focused on longitudinal changes in brain activation from pre- to post- CT (5) The analysis was a whole-brain voxel-wise analysis (6) The results were reported using stereotactic coordinates (Talairach/MNI) (x, y, z). The following exclusion criteria were applied: (1) Newspaper reports or abstract publications (2) Studies that were not in English (3) Resting-state fMRI, functional connectivity, voxel-based morphometry, or Region-of-Interest analyses. The entire search process is shown in the flowchart below (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of the Different Steps Conducted. In total, 16 studies remained. rs - fMRI = Resting-State functional MRI; FC = functional connectivity; VBM = voxel-based morphometric; ROI = region of interest

After applying these criteria, the resulting articles were coded into the BrainMap database [19] according to paradigm and direction of effect. The locations of the maximum voxel in each cluster were reported in Montreal Neuroimaging Institute (MNI) space. Studies reporting foci in Talairach space were converted to MNI space using the “Talairach to SPM” conversion function implemented in Ginger ALE.

Overlay analysis

To identify whether the regions activated by different tasks overlap, we performed a coordinate-based meta-analyses of executive functioning using the anatomic likelihood estimation (ALE) method per each task and completed within-group meta-analyses for each BrainMap domain to find domain-specific patterns of activation [20]. For the overlay analysis, we choose cluster-level of p = 0.05, minimum volume = 200, and threshold permutations = 1000.

ALE Meta-analysis

An ALE approach for coordinate-based meta-analysis was used to assess the overlap of foci reported in the individual studies by modeling them as 3-dimensional Gaussian probability distributions centered at the respective coordinates. The ALE method accommodates the spatial uncertainty associated with each single focus [21].

For brain areas reported with increased or decreased activations, two ALE meta-analyses were performed separately using P value = 0.05, cluster-level = 0.05 and threshold permutations =1000. First, a modeled activation map was created from the probabilities of all foci at each voxel. Then, a voxel-wise ALE map was created from the union of all modeled activation maps. The ALE map indicates the degree of convergence of results at each particular location in the brain [22]. A voxel-level FWE for meta-analyses via the ALE method was used for significance. The studies included in the map of increased activation to CT included 9 experiments in 8 papers reporting 37 foci. The studies included in the map of decreased activation to CT included 6 experiments in 5 papers with 27 foci.

Behavior analysis

In order to determine the behavior associated with each region within the same network, it is important to explicate the functional associations of individual regions across a wider anatomical scope that spans various networks. We used a plug-in software “Behavior Analysis” in the BrainMap database to map the functional correlates of behavioral phenotypes across regions. This method is based on the “Behavior Domain” describing behaviors and psychological processes. The behavior domain mainly includes cognition, action, perception, emotion, and internal concepts. When Z-score > 3, the behavioral domain was considered to be significantly associated with the activated brain region.

Functional Meta-analytic connectivity modeling (fMACM) procedure

Meta-analytic connectivity modeling (MACM) was used to identify functional co-activation of regions resulting from the ALE meta-analysis. MACM assesses the brain-wide co-activation patterns across a large number of functional neuroimaging results and identifies areas that show consistent co-activation with a seed region [23, 24]. For the current paper, we limited our search to fMACM (fMRI and PET analyses) in order to find task-dependent functional co-activations. Our seed regions included the 3 locations identified in our ALE analysis (left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), left precuneus, left cuneus). The ROIs were obtained by drawing a circle with a radius of 9 around the maximum point of activation. We set p value = 0.05.

Results

Literature search results

10 articles met all criteria and were coded into the BrainMap database according to paradigm and direction of effect. Characteristics of the 10 included studies are shown in Table 1. Of the 10 included studies, there were 6 schizophrenia, 2 anorexia nervosa, 1 attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and 1 mood disorder. Participants in these studies were aged from 11 to 41, including both patient and control groups. The studies involved 5 different executive function tasks: n-back, reasoning / problem solving, task switching, Go/No-Go, and visuospatial attention.

Table 1 Characteristics of the 16 induced studies

Overlay analysis of task-specific maps

Five coordinate-based meta-analyses of executive functioning were performed using the ALE method for each executive function imaging task. Table 2 lists the studies included in each task-wise map. According to the results, n-back activated right Mid Frontal Gyrus (BA6), left Cingulate Gyrus (BA32), left Inf Parietal Lobule (BA40), right Mid Frontal Gyrus (BA9), right Inf Parietal Lobule (BA40), left Precentral Gyrus (BA9), left Sub-Gyrus (BA6), and right Precuneus (BA7); Reasoning activated right Insula (BA13), right Inf Frontal Gyrus (BA9), left Sup Parietal Lobule (BA7), right Sup Parietal Lobule (BA7), and right Precuneus (BA31); Task switching activated left Sup Frontal Gyrus (BA6), right Insula (BA13), left Inf Parietal Lobule (BA40), left Precentral Gyrus (BA6); Go-NoGo activated left Med Frontal Gyrus (BA6) and right Med Frontal Gyrus (BA6); and visuospatial attention activated left Sup Parietal Lobule (BA7), right Precuneus (BA7), and left Med Frontal Gyrus (BA6).

Table 2 Studies from the BrainMap database for the 5 executive functioning tasks reported in the included publications

We overlayed the 5 resulting task-specific maps, and projected them onto a brain template. The resulting overlay map is shown in Fig. 2. The results demonstrated that overlay regions of the 5 tasks included R Insula (BA13), Cingulate Gyrus (BA32), L Med Frontal Gyrus (BA32), L Sup Frontal Gyrus (BA6), L Inf Parietal Lobule (BA40), and L Sup Parietal Lobule (BA7). Hence, we confirmed that these 5 tasks can be combined into a meta-analysis of executive function.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Significant overlay regions of 5 executive functioning tasks in included publications. Task 1 = n-back (yellow), Task 2 = reasoning / problem Solving (violet), Task 3 = task switching (light blue), Task 4 = Go/No-Go (red), Task 5 = visuospatial attention (blue). Imaging a, overlay region R Insula: center = (34, 22, 2), r = 8; Imaging b, overlay region Cingulate Gyrus: center = (2, 18, 40), r = 9; Imaging c, overlay region L Inf Parietal Lob: center = (− 36, − 48, 46), r = 10; L Sup Parietal Lob: center = (− 21, − 63, 51), r = 9; Imaging d, overlay region L Sup Frontal Gyrus: center = (− 2, 15, 50), r = 6; L Med Frontal Gyrus: center = (− 3, 9, 50), r = 6

ALE meta-analysis results for change from pre- to post-CT

Two ALE meta-analysis were performed separately on brain areas displaying increased or decreased activation with P value = 0.05, cluster-level = 0.05 and threshold permutations =1000. The results showed that the left IFG (MNI -52, 8, 26; BA9) was significant for increased activation (maximum ALE value = 0.048), and the left precuneus (MNI -8, − 70, 60; BA7; maximum ALE value = 0.006) and the left cuneus (MNI -8, − 100, 12; BA17; maximum ALE value = 0.008) were significant for decreased activation. Results are shown in Fig. 3.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Areas of increased brain activation (red overlay) or decreased brain activation (blue overlay) after cognitive training

Behavioral analysis of regions that reliably change from pre- to post-CT

As shown in Fig. 4, the left IFG ROI was significantly related to the cognition behavioral domain. In the cognition subdomain of language, there were several significant relations between the left IFG with semantics (Z = 6.913), phonology (Z = 5.659), speech (Z = 5.517), and syntax (Z = 4.07). The left IFG ROI was also significantly related to the memory subdomain of cognition (working) (Z = 5.51) and the action behavioral domain (imagination) (Z = 3.647). The left precuneus and cuneus ROIs were significantly related to the behavioral domains of perception including the subdomain of vision (motion) (Z = 5.831), (shape) (Z = 3.73), and the cognition behavioral domain (spatial) (Z = 4.757).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Histogram of the results of the behavioral analysis for regions implicated in CT. a. shows the behavioral domain Z-scores of regions of increased activation and b. shows the behavioral domain Z-scores of regions of decreased activation. Significant outcomes are on the dashed line. Blue: perception, Green: cognition, Red: action

Results of the functional Meta-analytic connectivity modeling analysis

To find the task-dependent functional co-activations of the three regions altered by CT (left IFG, left precuneus, left cuneus), they were set as ROIs and three fMACM were constructed. For each region, we constructed fMACM that identified task-based co-activations across 73 neuroimaging studies of healthy participants (the least number of studies common to all 3 regions). These studies included 106 experiments with 842 subjects and 1631 locations for the left IFG analysis; 94 experiments with 1109 subjects and 1583 locations were used for left precuneus analysis; and 93 experiments with 1185 subjects and 1940 locations were used for left cuneus analysis. Results are shown in Fig. 5. The left IFG displayed significant co-activation with left cingulate gyrus (BA24), left insula (BA13), left superior parietal lobule (BA7), left middle frontal gyrus (BA46), right IFG (BA9), right insula (BA13), and right thalamus. The left precuneus demonstrated significant co-activation with left superior frontal gyrus (BA6), left IFG (BA6), right precuneus (BA7), left inferior parietal lobule (BA6), left insula (BA13), and left precentral gyrus (BA6). No significant co-activation was found between the left cuneus with other brain regions.

Fig. 5
figure 5

a The fMACA with left IFG as ROI, LIFG: left inferior frontal gyrus, LCG: left cingulate gyrus, LInsula: left insula, LSPL: left superior parietal lobule, LMFG: left middle frontal gyrus, RInsula: right insula, RIFG: right inferior frontal gyrus, RTha: right thalamus; b The fMACA with left precuneus as ROI, LPCu: left precuneus, LSFG: left superior frontal gyrus, RPCu: right precuneus, LIP: left inferior parietal lobule, LIFG: left inferior frontal gyrus, LInsula: left insula, LPG: left precentral gyrus

fMACM between the 3 altered brain regions and the 5 executive task-activated regions

Similarly, we constructed a fMACM between the 3 brain regions altered by CT and 5 executive task-activated regions. As mentioned above, the overlapping areas activated by the 5 executive tasks included R Insula, Cingulate Gyrus, L Med Frontal Gyrus, L Sup Frontal Gyrus, L Inf Parietal Lobule, and L Sup Parietal Lobule. We also analyzed the mean volume of 5 executive task-activated regions in the 3 seed regions (sphere, radius = 9 mm). The results showed all 5 executive task-activated regions were significantly related to the task-dependent functional coactivation regions of L Inf Frontal Gyrus (ALE value > 0.01). The results are shown in Fig. 6.

Fig. 6
figure 6

This figure shows the fMACM between the 3 brain regions altered by CT and the 5 executive task-activated regions. Red overlay = left IFG. Light blue overlay =5 executive task-activated ROIs, R Insula (34, 22, 2), Cingulate Gyrus (2, 18, 40), LIPL = L Inf Parietal Lobule (− 36, − 48, 46), LSPL = L Sup Parietal Lobule (− 21, − 63, 51), LSFG = L Sup Frontal Gyrus (− 2, 15, 50), LMFG = L Med Frontal Gyrus (− 3, 9, 50). Arrows are on behalf of co-activations in the fMACM with direction from activation ROI to co-activation overlay

Discussion

In this study, we conducted a meta-analysis to identify brain regions that are consistently reported in functional neuroimaging studies of CT for patients with psychiatric disorders. The main finding was that CT is associated with increases in activation in the left IFG and decreases in activation in left precuneus and cuneus from pre- to post- CT. This suggests that the response to brain activity may be a potential mechanism for improved cognitive function.

Generally speaking, decreasing activation putatively reflects reduced recruitment of neuronal resources, whereas increasing activation is thought to reflect the recruitment and evolution of additional neuronal substrates. Decreasing activation may presumably relate to the setting of more efficient task representations with repeated experience, whereas increased activation may be related to the establishment and development of task-specific representations with continued practice [25].

Behavioral analysis shows the left IFG is related to language, working memory, and imagination. Previous reviews and meta-analyses suggest the left IFG participates in semantic processing, phonology, syntactic processes, cognitive control, and working memory [25]. Some recent studies have also shown the left IFG regulates strategic semantic access via top-down signals acting upon temporal storage areas [26], and this function is affected by prior knowledge [27]. A recent meta-analysis of schizophrenia also found an increase activation in left IFG after cognitive training [14]. Cognitive training can improve the cognitive function of patients with bipolar disorder by increasing the resting cerebral blood flow in the left IFG [28]. Left IFG increased activation mainly improves verbal fluency and verbal working memory [29, 30]. All of this evidence supports the idea that left IFG is important for the core cognitive activity of language and working memory. Furthermore, this suggests that CT does indeed increase cognitive function through increased IFG activation.

Behavioral analysis of the left precuneus and left cuneus reveal that they are frequently activated while processing visual motion and shape. Research has proven that the precuneus is involved in a wide spectrum of highly integrated tasks including visuo-spatial imagery and episodic memory retrieval [31]. The cuneus, traditionally, has been linked to the processing of visual information, and also plays a supporting role in the integration of sensory information with cognitive processes such as attention, learning, and memory [32]. Studies indicate that the precuneus and cuneus are located in the default mode network (DMN) [33,34,35,36,37] where they play an important role [38, 39]. Precuneus and cuneus are related to cognitive dysfunction in patients with mental disorders [40, 41]. Decreased activation of left precuneus after cognitive training is associated with cognitive improvement [42]. Typically, relative DMN activity decreases during task performance [43], suggesting that CT improves cognitive function by reducing neurological resources for vision.

Furthermore, we conducted a fMACM to identify patterns of connectivity that have been frequently associated with the 3 seed regions (left IFG, left precuneus and left cuneus). We found that the left IFG was consistently co-activated with the left precuneus. This finding supports the notion that left precuneus reliably underlies visual processing for a wide spectrum of tasks and its activity supports the core cognitive function of left IFG. Consistent with the results of a recent study by our colleagues, we found that when performing a memory task, the bilateral anterior cingulate gyrus and precuneus are activated, while the left IFG and left cuneus are deactivated [44].

In the module of the left IFG, the left IFG and left superior parietal lobule belong to central-executive network (CEN), and the left IFG is one of its key nodes [45,46,47]. The CEN is involved in information manipulation and decision-making behaviors [46, 47]. Moreover, research has shown that during performance of cognitive tasks, the CEN typically displays increased activation [45, 48].

In the module of the left precuneus, the left precuneus, left superior frontal gyrus, and left inferior parietal lobule belong to DMN [45, 49]. The DMN is associated with emotional processing, self-referential mental activity, the recollection of prior experiences, and rumination [50,51,52,53]. As such, during performance of cognitive tasks, DMN is often deactivated; the DMN is “task negative”, i.e., it turns off when tasks are performed [43, 50, 54]. In psychiatric disorders, many studies have shown that poor cognitive performance is associated on an item-by-item basis (i.e., during event-related fMRI) with a failure to “turn down” the DMN [55,56,57]. That is, when the DMN is not shut off during task performance, the task is done poorly; when the DMN is shut off, the task is done well. As mentioned above, the left precuneus is a key node of DMN. This may mean that improvements in cognitive function are achieved by a rebalance within the DMN [58].

In addition, we can see that the left insula appears in both left IFG module and left precuneus module. The insula is implicated in disparate cognitive, affective, and regulatory functions, and it is a key node of the salience network (SN )[59, 60]. The function of SN is to segregate the most relevant among internal and extrapersonal stimuli in order to guide behavior [60]. We can speculate that SN plays a balanced role in both the left IFG and left precuneus fMACM modules. Meanwhile, previous studies have proposed that there is a complementary neurobiological relationship between the DMN and the CEN [45, 48, 52, 60, 61].

The results showed that cognitive training altered the activation of left IFG, left precuneus, and left cuneus. The 5 executive task overlay regions included R Insula, Cingulate Gyrus, L Inf Parietal Lobule, L Sup Parietal Lobule, L Sup Frontal Gyrus, and L Med Frontal Gyrus. We conducted a fMACM between the 3 brain regions altered by CT and the 5 executive task-activated regions and showed that all 5 executive task activation regions significantly coactivated with L Inf Frontal Gyrus. We noted that the brain regions altered by CT were not within the range of brain regions activated by the 5 executive tasks, but brain regions activated by the 5 executive tasks co-activated with L Inf Frontal Gyrus. This may indicate that changes in brain activation result from cognitive training rather than from the task itself, and associated changes lead to an improvement in cognitive function.

Conclusions

Taken together within the context of our meta-analyses, we might conclude that CT improves cognitive function by supporting language and memory function, and reducing neuronal resources associated with basic visual processing. Perhaps the DMN and CEN play a role in balancing the neurological resources related to this change since CT helps “turn off” the DMN, allowing patients to turn their attention outward rather than inward.

Limitations

There are several limitations of this study. First, the number of the included studies was comparatively small for ALE meta-analysis. Only 16 studies matched our screening criteria, which limits our power to detect consistently activated and deactivated regions. Secondly, we included 5 different executive function tasks in our screening criteria. Although we performed a validation and showed that the activation of the 5 tasks was overlapping, using a single task for meta-analysis improves accuracy. Thirdly, different mental disorders have different neuropathology that might affect the outcomes of CT, and for this study we combined several psychiatric disorders in order to have enough studies for the meta-analysis. Future studies will benefit from addressing these limitations.

Availability of data and materials

The data used for this meta-analysis are publicly available in the research studies. The full dataset can be requested from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

CT:

Cognitive training

ALE:

Activation likelihood estimation

fMACM:

Functional meta-analytic connectivity model

IFG:

Left inferior frontal gyrus

MNI:

Montreal Neuroimaging Institute

MACM:

Meta-analytic connectivity modeling

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

SCZ:

Schizophrenia

SAD:

Schizoaffective disorder

ADHD:

Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

CRT:

Cognitive remediation therapy

CR:

Cognitive remediation

FAR:

Facial Affect Recognition

DMN:

Default mode network

CEN:

Central-executive network

SN:

Salience network

LIFG:

Left inferior frontal gyrus

LCG:

Left cingulate gyrus

LInsula:

left insula

LSPL:

Left superior parietal lob

LMFG:

Left middle frontal gyrus

RInsula:

Right insula

RIFG:

Right inferior frontal gyrus

RTha:

Right thalamus

LPCu:

Left precuneus

LSFG:

Left superior frontal gyrus

RPCu:

Right precuneus

LIP:

Left inferior parietal lobe

LInsula:

Left insula

LPG:

Left precentral gyrus

References

  1. Huang Y, et al. The China mental health survey (CMHS): I. background, aims and measures. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;51(11):1559–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Polo-Lopez R, Salaberria K, Echeburua E. Effectiveness of a psychological support program for relatives of people with mental disorders compared to a control group: a prandomized controlled trial. Behav Res Ther. 2015;68:13–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Taya F, et al. Brain enhancement through cognitive training: a new insight from brain connectome. Front Syst Neurosci. 2015;9:44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Fiszdon JM, et al. Cognitive remediation for individuals with psychosis: efficacy and mechanisms of treatment effects. Psychol Med. 2016;46(16):3275–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Jahn FS, et al. Cognitive training with fully immersive virtual reality in patients with neurological and psychiatric disorders: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Psychiatry Res. 2021;300:113928.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Draganski B, et al. Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature. 2004;427(6972):311–2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Duda BM, Sweet LH. Functional brain changes associated with cognitive training in healthy older adults: a preliminary ALE meta-analysis. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;14(4):1247–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Deng L, et al. The effect of cognitive training on the brain's local connectivity organization in healthy older adults. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):9033.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Downes M, Bathelt J, De Haan M. Event-related potential measures of executive functioning from preschool to adolescence. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2017;59(6):581–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Eslinger PJ, Flaherty-Craig CV, Chakara FM. Rehabilitation and management of executive function disorders. Handb Clin Neurol. 2013;110:365–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Vianin P, et al. Increased activation in Broca's area after cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2014;221(3):204–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Mak M, et al. The efficacy of computer-based cognitive training for executive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2019;279:62–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Passarotti AM, et al. A preliminary study on the functional benefits of computerized working memory training in children with pediatric bipolar disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Front Psychol. 2019;10:3060.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Wei YY, et al. Correlation between brain activation changes and cognitive improvement following cognitive remediation therapy in schizophrenia: an activation likelihood estimation Meta-analysis. Chin Med J. 2016;129(5):578–85.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Matsuda Y, et al. Neural changes following cognitive remediation therapy for schizophrenia. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2019;73(11):676–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Salimi-Khorshidi G, et al. Meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a comparison of image-based and coordinate-based pooling of studies. Neuroimage. 2009;45(3):810–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Fox PT, et al. Meta-analysis in human neuroimaging: computational modeling of large-scale databases. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2014;37:409–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Chase HW, et al. The neural basis of drug stimulus processing and craving: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70(8):785–93.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Vanasse TJ, et al. BrainMap VBM: an environment for structural meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39(8):3308–25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Niendam TA, et al. Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2012;12(2):241–68.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Eickhoff SB, et al. Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. Neuroimage. 2012;59(3):2349–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Goodkind M, et al. Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(4):305–15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Dogan I, et al. Functional connectivity modeling of consistent cortico-striatal degeneration in Huntington's disease. Neuroimage Clin. 2015;7:640–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Robinson JL, et al. Metaanalytic connectivity modeling: delineating the functional connectivity of the human amygdala. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(2):173–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Karni A, et al. The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95(3):861–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Zhao W, et al. Transcranial magnetic stimulation over left inferior frontal and posterior temporal cortex disrupts gesture-speech integration. J Neurosci. 2018;38(8):1891–900.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Di Liberto GM, Lalor EC, Millman RE. Causal cortical dynamics of a predictive enhancement of speech intelligibility. Neuroimage. 2018;166:247–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Venza EE, et al. Enhancing executive function and neural health in bipolar disorder through reasoning training. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1676.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Jaywant A, et al. The structural and functional Neuroanatomy of post-stroke depression and executive dysfunction: a review of neuroimaging findings and implications for treatment. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2022;35(1):3–11.

  30. Wexler BE, et al. Preliminary evidence of improved verbal working memory performance and normalization of task-related frontal lobe activation in schizophrenia following cognitive exercises. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(10):1694–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. 2006;129(Pt 3):564–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Martel M, et al. Unravelling the effect of experimental pain on the corticomotor system using transcranial magnetic stimulation and electroencephalography. Exp Brain Res. 2017;235(4):1223–31.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Brewer JA, et al. Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(50):20254–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:1–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Pan P, et al. Aberrant spontaneous low-frequency brain activity in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis of resting-state fMRI studies. Ageing Res Rev. 2017;35:12–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Raichle ME, et al. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(2):676–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Smith SM, et al. Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(31):13040–5.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Fransson P, Marrelec G. The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: evidence from a partial correlation network analysis. Neuroimage. 2008;42(3):1178–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Gusnard DA, et al. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(7):4259–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Soldevila-Matias P, et al. Precuneus and insular hypoactivation during cognitive processing in first-episode psychosis: systematic review and meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Rev Psiquiatr Salud Ment (Engl Ed). 2020;S1888–9891(20):30100–2.

  41. Yuan M, et al. Altered regional and integrated resting-state brain activity in general social anxiety disorder patients before and after group cognitive behavior therapy. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2018;272:30–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Schneider I, et al. Cognitive remediation therapy modulates intrinsic neural activity in patients with major depression. Psychol Med. 2020;50(14):2335–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Fox MD, et al. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102(27):9673–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Guo L, et al. Cognitive functioning in temporal lobe epilepsy: a BOLD-fMRI study. Mol Neurobiol. 2017;54(10):8361–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V. A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(34):12569–74.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Fujisawa TX, et al. Neural basis of psychological growth following adverse experiences: a resting-state functional MRI study. PLoS One. 2015;10(8):e0136427.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Uddin LQ, et al. Dynamic reconfiguration of structural and functional connectivity across core neurocognitive brain networks with development. J Neurosci. 2011;31(50):18578–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Greicius MD, et al. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(1):253–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Li W, et al. Subregions of the human superior frontal gyrus and their connections. Neuroimage. 2013;78:46–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Andrews-Hanna JR, Smallwood J, Spreng RN. The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014;1316:29–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Andrews-Hanna JR, et al. Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain's default network. Neuron. 2010;65(4):550–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Raichle ME. The brain's default mode network. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2015;38:433–47.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Ford JM. Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012;8:49–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Manoliu A, et al. Aberrant dependence of default mode/central executive network interactions on anterior insular salience network activity in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(2):428–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Anderson NE, et al. Psychopathic traits associated with abnormal hemodynamic activity in salience and default mode networks during auditory oddball task. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2018;18(3):564–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Figueroa CA, et al. Aberrant default-mode network-hippocampus connectivity after sad memory-recall in remitted-depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2017;12(11):1803–13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Hanlon FM, et al. Hemodynamic response function abnormalities in schizophrenia during a multisensory detection task. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37(2):745–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Yuan S, et al. Grey matter concentration revealed by voxel-based morphometry in individuals with cognitive vulnerability to depression. Zhong Nan Da Xue Xue Bao Yi Xue Ban. 2017;42(6):667–72.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Dun WH, et al. Abnormal structure and functional connectivity of the anterior insula at pain-free periovulation is associated with perceived pain during menstruation. Brain Imaging Behav. 2017;11(6):1787–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214(5-6):655–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Fox MD, et al. Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(26):10046–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for the comments provided by the assistant professor Amy S. Garrett (Department of psychiatry, University of Texas Health Science Center, San Antonio). And we would like to thank Nanjing Brain Hospital, affiliated with Nanjing Medical University of China for their funding and support.

Funding

This study was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (WC, 81571344; ZY, 81871344); Natural Science Foundation of Jiangsu Province (BK20161109,WC); the Natural Science Foundation of the Higher Education Institutions of Jiangsu Province, China (Grant no. 18KJB190003); key research and development program (Social Development) project of Jiangsu province (BE20156092015). The funders had no role in the design of the study, data collection, data analysis, interpretation of the data, or writing of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Research design and data analysis: WC, technical and method support: WQ, ZY and KH, FPT, literature search and screening: WHQ, YST, Manuscript first draft: WC, LJY, Supervisor: ZN. All the authors have approved the final article.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chun Wang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This article is a review of existing literature, there is no ethics approval and consent to participate.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Li, J.Y., Wu, H., Yuan, S. et al. A meta-analysis on neural changes of cognitive training for mental disorders in executive function tasks: increase or decrease brain activation?. BMC Psychiatry 22, 155 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-03796-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-03796-4

Keywords