Skip to main content

Word recognition memory and serum levels of Borna disease virus specific circulating immune complexes in obsessive–compulsive disorder

Abstract

Background

Borna disease virus 1 (BoDV-1) is a non-segmented, negative-strand RNA virus that persistently infects mammals including humans. BoDV-1 worldwide occurring strains display highly conserved genomes with overlapping genetic signatures between those of either human or animal origin. BoDV-1 infection may cause behavioral and cognitive disturbances in animals but has also been found in human major depression and obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD). However, the impact of BoDV-1 on memory functions in OCD is unknown.

Method

To evaluate the cognitive impact of BoDV-1 in OCD, event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded in a continuous word recognition paradigm in OCD patients (n = 16) and in healthy controls (n = 12). According to the presence of BoDV-1-specific circulating immune complexes (CIC), they were divided into two groups, namely group H (high) and L (low), n = 8 each. Typically, ERPs to repeated items are characterized by more positive waveforms beginning approximately 250 ms post-stimulus. This “old/new effect” has been shown to be relevant for memory processing. The early old/new effect (ca. 300–500 ms) with a frontal distribution is proposed to be a neural correlate of familiarity-based recognition. The late old/new effect (post-500 ms) is supposed to reflect memory recollection processes.

Results

OCD patients were reported to show a normal early old/new effect and a reduced late old/new effect compared to normal controls. In our study, OCD patients with a high virus load (group H) displayed exactly these effects, while patients with a low virus load (group L) did not differ from healthy controls.

Conclusion

These results confirmed that OCD patients had impaired memory recollection processes compared to the normal controls which may to some extent be related to their BoDV-1 infection.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) is characterized by recurrent obsessions and compulsions which are time consuming, significantly interfering with daily living of the patient [1]. A disturbance of cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical (CSTC) regulatory loops has been repeatedly demonstrated by brain-imaging studies [2,3,4,5]. Additionally, the characteristic symptoms of OCD including chronic doubts, repetitive checking, ruminations, decreased behavioral flexibility and ritual behavior [6] have been related to neuropsychological deficits in the areas of learning and memory [7,8,9], visuospatial processing [7, 10] and executive functions [11,12,13].

A strategic memory deficit has been shown in verbal memory tasks [14,15,16,17]; however, more recent reports showed deficits on non-verbal memory [8, 18,19,20]. It has been pointed out that memory decisions in OCD patients are usually given with reduced confidence [21,22,23,24,25]. This phenomenon is known as memory bias [21, 22], thought to be responsible for the peculiar insecurity in OCD patients.

Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) studies reported the existence of abnormal sensory and cognitive information processing in patients with OCD [23, 26,27,28,29,30]. ERPs are tiny voltage fluctuations, which can be recorded non-invasively from the intact human scalp and have been widely used in studying memory [31,32,33] due to its high temporal resolution. Until now, there were few ERP reports of word memory recognition (the old/new effect) in OCD [34,35,36,37]. These studies revealed that the old/new effect has distinct topographical areas – the ‘parietal’ and ‘mid-frontal’ in relation to memory, namely recollection and familiarity [38]. Our previous ERP study demonstrated that severely ill OCD patients showed an impaired late old/new effect (conscious recollection) of the word memory compared to the control group, which suggested an impairment of working memory capacity in these patients due to the dysfunction in the frontal and cingulated cortex [36].

The aim of this study was to investigate a possible modulation of word recognition memory in OCD patients with Borna disease virus 1 (BoDV-1) infection. BoDV-1, an unique RNA virus with a non-segmented single strand genome of negative polarity [39, 40], dominates the species Mammalian 1 bornavirus within the family Bornaviridae [41]. BoDV-1 strains are characterized by a broad host spectrum [42], high genomic conservation (> 95% homology) of either animal or human strains [43] and global occurrence [44]. Human isolates could be retrieved from peripheral mononuclear cells (PBMCs) [45] and brain [46] of psychiatric patients. One isolate was originating from a patient with chronic OCD [45] and recently completely sequenced [43]. BoDV-1 infection has been further discussed in relation to OCD [29, 47, 48]. Recently, rare cases of human encephalitis attracted attention [49,50,51,52,53], resembling lethal forms of encephalomyelitis in animals [42, 54, 55]. However, clinical outcomes (encephalitis vs. psychiatric disease), and epidemiology (zoonotic vs. human-to-human transmission) are still controversial [44, 56]. Likewise, which diagnostic method should be applied remained a matter of debate.

A variety of different methods to detect BoDV-1 infection had been developed in the past 30 years. Among them, BoDV-1 specific antibodies in serum via immune fluorescence (IFT) played a key role [57], but were complemented by direct viral components (proteins, RNA) in PBMCs and to a small extent in cerebrospinal fluid of psychiatric patients [47, 48, 58,59,60,61,62,63]. Isolation of infectious virus from RNA-positive PBMCs [45] and significantly higher frequencies of antibodies and/or RNA in psychiatric patients vs. healthy individuals have suggested a contributory role for Borna virus in the etiopathology of psychiatric disorders (reviewed by [47, 63]). Interestingly, BD virus recovered from a psychiatric patient was susceptible to amantadine [64]. Human isolates from a patient with bipolar depression and an OCD patient both were functionally different from laboratory viruses in vitro [43, 65]. Notably, in a double-blind placebo-controlled trial, BoDV-1 infected patients with major depressive and bipolar disorder largely benefitted from effective antiviral treatment with amantadine [66], thereby confirming previous open trials [67, 68]. Amantadine both provided anti-depressive and antiviral efficacy, as viral antigens and antibodies declined in parallel to clinical improvements, thereby strengthening the view on a viral involvement of the virus in neuropathology resulting in psychological events [66]. It was hypothesized from animal models that high amounts of free antigens are prerequisites for an interference of viral components with neurotransmitter systems, most probably with kainite KA1 receptor of glutamate [55, 69]. The impact of BoDV-1 infection on the sensitive balance of neuronal circuits and altered bioelectric activities in neurons, thus disturbing cognitive processes and memory, have been frequently demonstrated by molecular means and animal experiments [59, 70,71,72,73,74,75]. Persistence of high amounts of CICs which are circulating antigen–antibody complexes, and plasma antigen (pAG) correlated with the severity of depression [48, 66, 76]. Furthermore, higher CIC levels were associated with attention-related cognitive changing (increased ERP N1 component) in OCD patients, in particular in secondary visual areas and the anterior cingulated gyrus [29].

Notably, experimental evidence also demonstrated that BDV P-protein alone could disturb developmental processes in the brain [77] and impaired memory formation [74, 78]. Memory impairment has also been reported in other viral infection such as Theiler’s murine encephalomyelitis virus [79], Langat virus [80] and Herpes simplex virus [81]. Based on the likely contribution of BoDV-1 infection to memory dysfunction, this study aimed to investigate whether memory problems were modulated in BoDV-1 infected OCD patients using ERPs.

Methods

Subjects

The local ethical committee at the Hannover Medical School, Hanover, Germany, approved the study (see Additional file 1: study history and disclaimer). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Twenty native German speakers fulfilling the DSM-IV criteria [1] for OCD, participated in this study. The patients' pre-study treatment strategies were not changed during the study. Patients with comorbid current psychiatric diagnosis, drug abuse, medical or neurological disorder including tics or Tourette’s syndrome were excluded from the study. Four OCD subjects had to be excluded because of blink or movement artifact. Characteristics of the subjects (n = 16) including demographic data and scores on the Yale-Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS) [82], the Hamilton rating scale for depression (HAMD, 21-items) [83], and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) were shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Characteristics of the obsessive–compulsive disorder group

BoDV-1 infection monitoring

BoDV-1-infection was investigated by collecting anticoagulated blood. BoDV-1-specific circulating immune complexes (CICs) were detected by an enzyme immune assay (EIA) with specifically immobilized anti-p40/p24 monoclonal antibodies. Antibodies (Ab) and plasma antigens (pAG) were also measured using EIA, according to the published protocol [48] at the Robert Koch-Institute, Berlin, Germany during 2002–2005 (see Additional file 1: Study history and disclaimer). The values of CICs, pAG, and Ab were given in Table 2 as extinction values at 405 nm, measured in the starting dilutions for each test as indicated. Follow-up samples of study patients were tested at weeks 2, 4, 6, 12, and 16 to allow for evaluating the course of infection and the CIC values were given in Table 3. Based on previous longitudinal studies, the expressions of BoDV-1-specific CICs in the blood plasma are indicating an acute activated infection state.

Table 2 Virological results of the obsessive–compulsive disorder group
Table 3 Infection follow-up of the obsessive–compulsive disorder group based on circulating immune complexes (BoDV-1 CIC)

Two subgroups of patients were formed with regards to BoDV-1-specific CIC levels given in Table 2 (initial sample, week 0): group H (for “high”; n = 8, 5 women) with relatively high levels of BoDV-1-specific CICs and group L (for “low”; n = 8, 3 women) with relatively low levels. Psychometric and virological parameters of the two subgroups were shown in Table 4. Twelve healthy control subjects (7 women, aged 20–48 years, mean age ± SD, 33.2 ± 10.3) were recruited from the student population and the staff of the Hannover Medical School and were matched closely with respect to age, education and handedness. They were diagnosed and found to be not infected with BoDV-1 (data not shown). There was no significant difference of age between OCD patients and the control group (T = 0.02; p = 0.99) and among OCD group H, group L and the control group [F(1, 2) = 0.10; p = 0.91].

Table 4 Characteristic comparisons between the two OCD groups with higher BoDV-1-specific CIC levels (group H) and with lower BoDV-1-specific CIC levels (group L)

Stimuli and procedure

Three hundred German words (150 nouns, 86 verbs and 64 adjectives) were selected according to Meier [84] and the Beck inventory [85]. Mean log-frequency was –3.05. Word length varied between 4 and 11 letters (mean 6.7), subtending a visual angle of 1.7° vertically and 3.0° horizontally. Five randomized word lists of approximately equal length were constructed. Within each list, 83.3% of the words were repeated after 10 to 15 intervening items. Words were repeated only once. Each word was presented at the fixation point on a computer monitor located 110 cm from the subject.

The subjects´ task was to discriminate between first- and second- word presentation by pressing one of two buttons with the left or right index finger, while giving equal importance to speed and accuracy. The button assignment was counterbalanced between subjects. The words were presented serially in white color on a uniform dark blue background of a video monitor with duration of 300 ms (milliseconds) each with an inter-stimulus interval of 2 s (± 0.3 s). Subjects were requested and trained to fixate a dot in the center of the screen and not to move their eyes.

Recording

The EEG was recorded from 31 electrodes including all standard sites of the International 10/20 system [84]. All electrodes were referenced to the right mastoid. The horizontal EOG was recorded bilaterally with electrodes at the outer ocular canthi and the vertical EOG was registered using an electrode below the right eye referenced to the right outer ocular canthus. The signals were amplified using 10 s time constant and processed with a band pass filter between 0.1 and 100 Hz (half amplitude low and high frequency cut-offs), digitized at a rate of 250 Hz and stored on the hard disk. Automated artefact rejection was performed off-line to eliminate data epochs contaminated by blinks, saccades and muscle activity and amplifier saturation. The mean rejection rate was 16% (7–28% for individual subjects).

Data analysis

ERPs were averaged off-line (BrainVision Analyzer Version 2.0) for correctly detected first and second presentations of the words for each session and participant, as analyzed previously [27, 86]. The ERPs and behavioral performances were analyzed using repeated measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) using a 5% confidence level. Behavioral data were quantified by measuring reaction time (RT, ms) and hit rates (HR, %) (Pressing the correct button for first and second presentation).

ERPs were quantified as mean amplitudes in specified time windows. These data were entered into repeated measures’ analyses of variance (ANOVA). Separate ANOVAs were conducted for F3, Fz, F4 and C3, Cz, C4 in the time range of 250–500 ms (early old/new effect), for C3, Cz, C4 and P3, Pz, P4 in the time window of 450–650 ms, and for F3, Fz, F4 and C3, Cz, C4 from 650- 950 ms (both time windows for the late old/new effect), due to the repetition effect tending to be largest over frontal and parietal areas [87]. The ANOVAs comprised of the factors group (OCD patients vs. control group; patient’s group H vs. group L vs. control group), repetition (first vs. second presentation), and electrode location (left vs. middle vs. right). The factor group was treated as a between-subject variable, whereas the other factors were within-subject factors. All analyses were adjusted for non-sphericity with the Greenhouse–Geisser epsilon coefficient [88] whenever more than 1 degree of freedom was present in the numerator.

Results

Virological results

OCD patients were virologically categorized according to high infection values based on CICs (H-group, patients 1–8) and low infection values (L-group, patients 9–16). As shown in Table 4, the mean CIC values of the H-group were 433.25 ± 305.7 which differed significantly from those of the L-group (111.13 ± 48.51, p < 0.01). Within the L-group were three patients (No. 9, 10, 12) who turned out to be negative at the time of sampling (week 0), because of values below the cut -off of 100 in all three tests. However, these patients developed CICs on week 2, lasting until week 6 (No. 9 and 10) up to week 16 (No. 12) (Table 3), thus confirming infection. Table 3 further clarified that activation levels (CIC) could be stable for a long time (at least 4 weeks up to 16 weeks), as suggested in continuously symptomatic OCD. Continuous CIC levels accounted for more than half of our OCD patients, namely for 9 out of 16 patients (56.3%), but discontinuous virus activation was also found (7 out of 16 patients; 43.7%). Of the healthy volunteers, all tested negative for CICs, pAG and Ab.

Behavioral results

Reaction times (RTs) to repeated words were significantly longer than to first presentations in the OCD patients and the control group [F (1, 26) = 53.35, p < 0.0001]. There was no interaction of group X repetition in either the comparison of the whole patient sample to controls [F (1, 26) = 1.41, p = 0.25] or for the patient subgroups and the control group [F (2, 25) = 1.18, p = 0.32] (Table 5). Repeated words were classified less accurately than first presentations [F (1, 26) = 40.47, p < 0.0001]. There were no interaction effects of group X repetition for the comparison of the entire patient group and the control group [F (1, 26) = 0.93, p = 0.34] as well as for the analysis including the patient subgroups and the control group [F (2, 25) = 0.74, p = 0.48] (Table 5).

Table 5 Reaction times (RT, ms) and hit rates (HR, %) (mean ± SD) for correctly recognized words in the different patient and control groups

ERP results

The grand-average ERPs of the OCD patients (n = 16) and the control group (n = 12) elicited by correctly recognized first (new) and second (old) word presentations are shown for selected electrodes in Fig. 1. In both groups ERPs to repeated and correctly classified “old” items were more frequently positive than for new items, which represented the old/new effect. This old/new effect had an onset of about 220 ms post-stimulus. The early part of the old/new effect was indicated by the ERPs between 250 and 500 ms at the frontal electrodes for both groups [factor repetition: F (1, 26) = 34.78, p < 0.001]. But there was no difference for this early old/new effect between both groups [interaction group X repetition effect: F (1, 26) = 0.13, p = 0.73]. The ANOVA also revealed a significant late old/new effect with the centro-frontal maximum measured between 450 and 650 ms (factor repetition in both groups at the central electrodes: [F (1, 26) = 13.15, p < 0.01]. This effect appears to be more pronounced over the left side for both groups (an interaction of the factors, repetition X electrode [F (1, 26) = 10.27; p < 0.01]. There was no interaction group X repetition effect [F (1, 26) = 1.3, p = 0.25] and no main group differences [F (1, 26) = 2.18, p = 0.15] of the mean amplitudes between both groups at the central electrodes. But the single electrode statistical analysis showed the group difference at C3 [F (1, 26) = 4.27, p < 0.05], not at Cz [F (1, 26) = 1.55, p = 0.23] and at C4 [F (1, 26) = 1.08, p = 0.31]. From 650 to 950 ms, the factors of the within-group or between-group ANOVA showed no difference between both groups. The followed individual analysis for each group disclosed that the OCD group displayed no late old/new effect at the parietal electrodes, but at C3, as compared to the control group (Table 6).

Fig. 1
figure 1

ERPs comparing OCD patients and controls. Grand average ERP waveforms to correctly detected first and second presentations of the words for the OCD group (n = 16) and the normal controls (n = 12) at F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, and P4. The second presentations of words elicited a more positive-shift starting at about 220 ms after the onset of the stimulus and extending for the rest of the recording epoch. This old/new effect is widespread and has a left frontocentral maximum. The patients showed reduced late old/new effect (ca. 450–650 ms) compared to the controls. FP = first presentation, SP = second presentation

Table 6 ANOVA results for the old/new effect (Repetition) and its interactions with the factor electrode (in parentheses) during two latency intervals at the different electrode sites for OCD group, group H, group L, and the control group

Figure 2 disclosed the grand-average ERPs for the OCD group H (n = 8), OCD group L (n = 8) and the control group (n = 12) at the selected electrode sites. For the early portion of the old/new effect (250–500 ms post-stimulus), there was a significant repetition effect [F (1, 25) = 25.37, p < 0.0001], interaction effects of repetition X electrode [F (2, 25) = 8.08, p = 0.004], and repetition electrode X group [F (2, 25) = 3.3, p < 0.05]. In these three groups, the early old/new effect differed between groups for the different electrodes. The between-group differences of the mean amplitudes were present at the different electrodes. There was a significant difference observed for the between-group effect at the central areas. The pair-wise comparison of any two groups showed that the ERP amplitudes were significantly more frequently positive in the group H than in group L and control group at the central area. No significant differences were found between group L and the control group (Table 7). As shown by the individual analysis for each group, the control group and group H showed the early old/new effect at the fronto-central electrodes, while group L did not (Table 6).

Fig. 2
figure 2

ERPs comparing OCD groups H and L and controls. Grand average ERP waveforms to correctly detected first and second presentations of the words for the OCD group H (n = 8), group L (n = 8) and the normal controls (n = 12) at F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, and P4. Group H showed early old/new effect (ca. 250–500 ms) and partially late old/new effect only over the left hemisphere in contrast to controls. Significant early and late old/new difference can be observed for the whole recording epoch between group L and the controls. FP = first presentation, SP = second presentation

Table 7 Between-subject differences of mean amplitude measures for the ERP components at the different electrode sites for group H, group L and the control group

From 450 to 650 ms, the within-group ANOVA disclosed the late portion of the old/new effect to be statistically significant [at the central area, F (1,25) = 10.99, p < 0.01]. The difference of the late old/new effect between groups was more pronounced over the left hemisphere. The central areas were indexed by an interaction of the factors, repetition X electrode X group [F (2, 25) = 4.77, p < 0.05]. Only at C3, the single electrode statistical analysis showed that there is an interaction effect of repetition X group [F (2, 25) = 3.24, p = 0.05]. These effects still remained at the central areas in the later interval of 650–950 ms [the interaction effect of repetition X electrode X group: F (2, 25) = 4.563, p = 0.01; the interaction of repetition X group at C3: [F (2, 25) = 3.669, p = 0.04]. As seen in the individual group analyses (Table 6), group H showed the late old/new effect only at the left centro-parietal areas in contrast to the control group (bilateral) while group L showed none. In addition, there were significant differences of the mean amplitudes between these groups at the centro-parietal electrodes (Table 7). The pair-wise comparison of any two groups disclosed that the ERP amplitudes in group L were much smaller than in group H and the control group at the centro-parietal areas. No significant differences were found between group H and the control group (Table 7).

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to 1) investigate changes of the ERP old/new effects in the continuous word recognition in OCD patients in contrast to normal controls and, 2) explore the potential correlation of BoDV-1 infection and memory effects. The behavioral data showed a significant effect of the old word recognition on reaction times and target hit rates with less performance than for new words. But this effect elicited no differences between OCD patients and normal controls or between the OCD subgroups and the normal controls. This clarifies the fact that the groups did not differ in their tradeoff between response processing favoring one or the other parameter [27]. It supports the notion that group differences in ERP measures are related to the pathophysiological basis of OCD.

ERP findings in OCD patients

The ERPs revealed two major findings. First, the early effect of word repetition resulted in an increased positive ERP waveform to repeated words in the time range between 250 and 500 ms post stimulus, with a maximum effect at the fronto-central areas. This effect showed no difference between the whole OCD group and the normal controls. Interestingly, group H (with high levels of BoDV-1-specific CICs) had similar early old/new effects with higher amplitudes compared to the control group. However, group L (with low levels of BoDV-1-specific CICs) had no early old/new effect at this time window. Rugg and colleagues [89, 90] stated that the early old/new effect differed according to whether items were old or new, but was insensitive to accuracy of recognition judgment and the depth of manipulation processing. Moreover, the lack of sensitivity reflected neural activity associated with a form or forms of memory (familiarity) distinct from recollection [90]. Our results suggested that the whole OCD group and group H reflected relatively normal familiarity-derived recognition processing in comparison to the controls while group L alone had a lack of this effect.

The second noteworthy aspect of the ERP findings concerned the memory effect that occurred from approximately 500 ms after the stimulus, which differed from the early old/new effect. The scalp topography was strongly left centro-parietal from 450 to 650 ms and this memory effect is sensitive to depth of processing (Remember or Know), which reflects neural activity associated with the conscious recollection of test items’ study episodes, as shown in other studies [38, 89,90,91]. In our study, the whole OCD group exhibited reduced late old/new effect in the latency of 450–650 ms in contrast to normal controls, but no difference was found in both groups from 650 to 950 ms. Rugg and Curran [38] suggested that ‘no difference’ may be due to small or non-existent in a ‘simple’ word recognition test. One can argue that the decreased old/new effect in the OCD group reflected an impaired conscious recollection of these words compared to healthy controls. What’s more, OCD subgroups showed sufficiently distinct ERP waveforms regarding to this late old/new effect. Only at the left centro-parietal areas this effect was found in group H while group L showed none. This suggested that the two OCD subgroups with different BoDV-1 infection activity levels reflected different memory recollection processing. However, one should be cautious about linking the current impaired memory processing in OCD with interpretations regarding neurocircuitry abnormalities and the infection levels of BoDV-1. This will be discussed at the end of this section.

Kim and colleagues [34] investigated the word memory in OCD using an ERP word recognition task study. Their results demonstrated that the controls showed the old/new effect during the 200–500 ms period post-stimulus, while the OCD patients did not. The absence of an old/new effect on ERP in OCD patients was discussed in terms of dysfunction of the fronto-striatal system, which plays an important role in OCD. Furthermore, Kim and colleagues [35] studied cortical source localization of word repetition effects (old/new effects) in patients with OCD by employing the equivalent current dipole model. Their results suggested that OCD patients suffered from the encoding deficits in the word processing, particularly in the left hemisphere.

The maximum recollection-sensitive effect of word repetition was seen obviously over the left electrodes in our study. This was in accord with the results of numerous other studies [89, 92, 93]. Wilding and Rugg [94] further noted that an effect in the asymmetric scalp distribution is due to a generator lateralized to the left hemisphere, and the left hemisphere is important for many aspects of verbal memory. Likewise, neuroimaging data showed that the left hippocampal formation along with regions of left temporal and frontal cortex involves in late old/new effect (recollection) [90]. These findings support the current ideas about the role of the hippocampal formation in episode memory retrieval and provide complementary information about the localization and time course of the cortical correlates of the recollection of recently experienced words.

OCD patients are at disadvantage, when they actively attempt to retrieve preciously encountered information, this is due to the impairment in non-verbal and verbal memory performance that is correlated with the deficits in semantic clustering and organizational strategies [14, 17]. Active organization of verbal or non-verbal information during encoding is thought to rely on a set of higher-level control processes, called ‘executive functions’, that modulate more basic cognitive, motor or memory functions and rely on the integrity of frontostriatal systems [90]. This suggests that failure to initiate strategies spontaneously is a salient characteristic of executive dysfunction in OCD. Behavioral key features of OCD such as checking or rereading might be due to deficits in memory and structuring information [14]. We have also presented electrophysiological evidence that OCD patient group H showed disturbed conscious word recollection and may utilized an alternative mechanism to recognize words, which seemed to be closely related to the infection levels of BoDV-1 (CICs).

BoDV-1 infection in animal models

BoDV-1 is an evolutionary old pathogen, which is perfectly adapted to highly specialized neurons in mammals, especially in the limbic system [55]. BoDV-1 causes behavioral disturbances in animals, such as depression-like apathy or excitation, spatial discrimination memory deficit, and learning deficiencies which are likely to be the consequence of viral proteins’ interference with neurotransmitter balances in the limbic system [72, 75, 95] or synaptic vesicle recycling [96]. BoDV-1 infected rats have high levels of viral nucleic acid in the prefrontal cortex, and elevated levels of mesocortical dopamine activity [73]. Kamitani and colleagues [77] have reported that glial expression of BoDV-1 phosphoprotein (P-protein) alone, in the absence of infection, could induce behavioral and neurological abnormalities in transgenic mice. The transgenic brains exhibit a significant reduction in brain-derived neurotrophic factor, serotonin receptor expression, as well as a marked decrease in synaptic density. BoDV-1 infected rats have shown hyperactivity and stereotyped behaviors similar to those following neurotoxic or electrolytic lesions in frontal cortex or its catecholamine afferents in rats.

Rat models of neonatal BoDV-1 infection resulting in virus persistence have shown learning and memory deficits [72, 75]. The hippocampus has a predominant role for memory and learning [97,98,99]. Initial viral dissemination was observed on post infection day 28 in the astrocytes of subiculum and later in the hippocampal neurons. At day 65 post infection, the spread went from the CA1 to the CA3 region and was abundant. The intensity of infection was more pronounced at day 120 post infection in the dendrites of CA3 neurons [100]. Intra-axonal spread was shown to be the major route for virus dissemination [101]. The persistent infection of BoDV-1 in animals favors hippocampus and limbic structures as preferential areas of replication [54, 100,101,102]. The pathophysiology is observed as cortical shrinkage and degeneration of granule neurons of dentate gyrus [72, 103]. The spatio-temporal occurrence of BoDV-1 glycoprotein was observed in the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, amygdala and thalamus [104]. However, the over-expression of virus nuclear and phosphoproteins (N- and P-protein) present in the hippocampus of rats with learning deficiencies [75] supported the hypothesis of an interference of those major proteins at glutamate receptor sites, namely the kainate KA1 receptor [69]. Notably, N- and P-protein are the major diagnostic markers indicating an activated infection [47, 48]. The monoclonal antibodies used in the EIAs for the detection of CICs and antigen (pAG) in plasma were shown to recognize conserved conformational epitopes on N-protein and phosphorylated (active) P-protein [105]. In BoDV-1 infected rats, the synaptic plasticity of dendate gyrus neurons has been altered [71]. Persistent infection in the brain did not affect survival and morphology, but the synaptic plasticity of the hippocampal neurons [70, 78, 106, 107]. Learning and memory consolidation are based on the strength of the synaptic plasticity [108]. The evidence from animal models suggests that the synaptic plasticity was shunned by a complex interference of BoDV-1 infection, most probably mediated by major proteins N and P, in hippocampal neurons resulting in the inhibition of learning and memory consolidation.

PKC and CaMKII have an important role in the memory formation [109] and in psychiatric illness [110]. It is also understood that the BoDV-1 P protein needs PKC and CaMKII for its phosphorylation [111, 112] and viral spread [113]. Abrogating the PKC binding site of BoDV-1-P protein resulted in restorage of normal synaptic plasticity [114]. These research reports clearly stated that BoDV-1 P-protein utilizes secondary messenger signaling for its replication and spread. Recently, Betourne and colleagues [78] have shown BoDV-1 P protein alone can impair memory, like Kamitani and colleagues [77] demonstrated earlier via a transgenic mice model. This provided strong evidence that BoDV-1 infection, at least via its pathogenic P-protein, may hinder the formation of memory and memory recollection.

BoDV-1 infection in human psychiatric patients

In humans BoDV-1 infection has been finally accepted because of recent cases of associated encephalitis [49,50,51,52,53, 115]. However, a contributory role to psychiatric disorders which display behavior and cognitive disturbances, is still under discussion but has longtime proponents, due to correlative evidence in numerous studies [44,45,46,47,48, 57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64, 66,67,68, 76, 86, 116, 117]. In this study, we demonstrated significant differences in the early and late ERP old/new effects between OCD patient group H and group L. The group H showed impaired old/new effects with higher mean amplitudes, while group L had no word old/new effects in any time window compared to the normal controls. The clinical characteristics of the patient group H and group L, for example, ages, educational years, Y-BOCS scores, and HAMD scores did not differ significantly. This suggested that the levels of BoDV-1 infection, rather than the severity of clinical symptoms [36] and clinical subtypes, were involved in the abnormal patterns of word memory processes in both OCD subgroups. This was the first demonstration that BoDV-1 activity (amount of CICs) could be correlated with disease-relevant and memory-related cognitive changes (impaired old/new effects) in humans. Only those OCD patients with high CIC levels displayed mnestic dysfunction, those with low levels did not, suggesting that BoDV-1 unfolds disease-related effects depending upon the state of infection and not at any time. This hypothesis is in line with fluctuating activity levels corresponding with disease episodes in major depression [48, 66]. Notably, our pre-study screening suggested that a major proportion of OCD patients are at risk of activating Borna disease virus 1 infection.

The increased general amplitude in group H patients could fit well with our previous study [29], where we found that OCD patients with high CIC levels showed hyperactive attention-related information processing (an increased N1 ERP component and larger P3b amplitude) compared to those with low CIC levels and controls. Indeed, functional neuroimaging studies strongly suggested that OCD involves hyperactivity of CSTC networks [118, 119]. It is interesting to know that the subiculum, a subdivision of hippocampus formation which lies in between the entorhinal cortex and CA1 subfield of hippocampus plays an important role in memory recollection [120]. Viral spread in animal models was observed from subiculum to the hippocampus and then to the cortex [100]. The present findings are consistent with such hyperactivity and support the hypothesis this may be modulated by BoDV-1 infection, possibly through interfering of viral components with neurotransmitters (e.g., glutamate and aspartate) and impairment of synaptic plasticity, especially at hippocampus areas.

Conclusion

In conclusion, ERP data from this study have added weighs to the evidence in support of impaired memory information processing in OCD. Additionally, the present study, being the first study to correlate BoDV-1 infection parameters with memory functions in humans, provides a new insight into the suggested relationship of BoDV-1 infection with cognitive dysfunctions in OCD patients. This will motivate further investigations along this line. Follow up studies in OCD patients with continuous vs. discontinuous virus activation could be useful to clarify whether present or absent virus activity correspond to mnestic dysfunction levels. Subsequent studies, however, need to replicate these preliminary findings in a larger sample size and to further investigate the possible mechanisms of neurotropic BoDV-1 infection and possible treatments.

Availability of data and materials

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article and its supplementary information files.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association.: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV, 4th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.

  2. Swedo SE, Schapiro MB, Grady CL, Cheslow DL, Leonard HL, Kumar A, Friedland R, Rapoport SI, Rapoport JL. Cerebral glucose metabolism in childhood-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46(6):518–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Schmidtke K, Schorb A, Winkelmann G, Hohagen F. Cognitive frontal lobe dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43(9583000):666–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Rauch SL. Neuroimaging and neurocircuitry models pertaining to the neurosurgical treatment of psychiatric disorders. Neurosurg Clin N Am. 2003;14(2):213–23 vii-viii.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Rosenberg DR, MacMillan SN, Moore GJ. Brain anatomy and chemistry may predict treatment response in paediatric obsessive–compulsive disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2001;4(2):179–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Savage CR, Rauch SL. Cognitive deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(10873941):1182–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Cohen LJ, Hollander E, DeCaria CM, Stein DJ, Simeon D, Liebowitz MR, Aronowitz BR. Specificity of neuropsychological impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison with social phobic and normal control subjects. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1996;8(8845706):82–5.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Savage CR, Baer L, Keuthen NJ, Brown HD, Rauch SL, Jenike MA. Organizational strategies mediate nonverbal memory impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45(10202579):905–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Gottwald J, de Wit S, Apergis-Schoute AM, Morein-Zamir S, Kaser M, Cormack F, Sule A, Limmer W, Morris AC, Robbins TW, et al. Impaired cognitive plasticity and goal-directed control in adolescent obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Med. 2018;48(11):1900–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Oh S, Jung WH, Kim T, Shim G, Kwon JS. Brain Activation of Patients With Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder During a Mental Rotation Task: A Functional MRI Study. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:659121.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Purcell R, Maruff P, Kyrios M, Pantelis C. Neuropsychological deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison with unipolar depression, panic disorder, and normal controls. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(9596044):415–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Veale DM, Sahakian BJ, Owen AM, Marks IM. Specific cognitive deficits in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Med. 1996;26(6):1261–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Bedard MJ, Joyal CC, Godbout L, Chantal S. Executive functions and the obsessive-compulsive disorder: on the importance of subclinical symptoms and other concomitant factors. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;24(6):585–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Deckersbach T, Otto MW, Savage CR, Baer L, Jenike MA. The relationship between semantic organization and memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychother Psychosom. 2000;69(2):101–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Savage CR, Deckersbach T, Wilhelm S, Rauch SL, Baer L, Reid T, Jenike MA. Strategic processing and episodic memory impairment in obsessive compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology. 2000;14(10674806):141–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Henin A, Savage CR, Rauch SL, Deckersbach T, Wilhelm S, Baer L, Otto MW, Jenike MA. Is age at symptom onset associated with severity of memory impairment in adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder? Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(11136649):137–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Cabrera AR, McNally RJ, Savage CR. Missing the forest for the trees? Deficient memory for linguistic gist in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Med. 2001;31(6):1089–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Bernardes ET, Saraiva LC, MM ES, Hoexter MQ, Chacon P, Requena G, Miguel EC, Shavitt RG, Polanczyk GV, Cappi C, et al. Cognitive performance in children and adolescents at high-risk for obsessive-compulsive disorder. BMC Psychiatry. 2020;20(1):380.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Muller J, Roberts JE. Memory and attention in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: a review. J Anxiety Disord. 2005;19(1):1–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Segalas C, Alonso P, Real E, Garcia A, Minambres A, Labad J, Pertusa A, Bueno B, Jimenez-Murcia S, Menchon JM. Memory and strategic processing in first-degree relatives of obsessive compulsive patients. Psychol Med. 2010;40(12):2001–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Radomsky AS, Rachman S. Memory bias in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Behav Res Ther. 1999;37(10402686):605–18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Radomsky AS, Rachman S, Hammond D. Memory bias, confidence and responsibility in compulsive checking. Behav Res Ther. 2001;39(11419612):813–22.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Tolin DF, Abramowitz JS, Brigidi BD, Amir N, Street GP, Foa EB. Memory and memory confidence in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2001;39(11480832):913–27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Konishi M, Shishikura K, Nakaaki S, Komatsu S, Mimura M. Remembering and forgetting: directed forgetting effect in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2011;7:365–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Moritz S, Jaeger A. Decreased memory confidence in obsessive-compulsive disorder for scenarios high and low on responsibility: is low still too high? Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2018;268(3):291–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Towey JP, Tenke CE, Bruder GE, Leite P, Friedman D, Liebowitz M, Hollander E. Brain event-related potential correlates of overfocused attention in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychophysiology. 1994;31(6):535–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Johannes S, Wieringa BM, Nager W, Rada D, Dengler R, Emrich HM, Munte TF, Dietrich DE. Discrepant target detection and action monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2001;108(2):101–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Miyata A, Matsunaga H, Kiriike N, Iwasaki Y, Takei Y, Yamagami S. Event-related potentials in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998;52(5):513–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Dietrich DE, Zhang Y, Bode L, Münte TF, Hauser U, Schmorl P, Richter-Witte C, Godecke-Koch T, Feutl S, Schramm J, et al. Brain potential amplitude varies as a function of Borna disease virus-specific immune complexes in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(6):515 (519-520).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. de Groot CM, Torello MW, Boutros NN, Allen R. Auditory event-related potentials and statistical probability mapping in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clin Electroencephalogr. 1997;28(3):148–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Taylor JR, Olichney JM. From amnesia to dementia: ERP studies of memory and language. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2007;38(1):8–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C. The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:123–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Mecklinger A, Bader R. From fluency to recognition decisions: A broader view of familiarity-based remembering. Neuropsychologia. 2020;146:107527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Kim M-S, Kim YY, Kim EN, Lee KJ, Ha TH, Kwon JS. Implicit and explicit memory in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder: an event-related potential study. J Psychiatr Res. 2006;40(16083910):541–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Kim YY, Yoo SY, Kim MS, Kwon JS. Equivalent current dipole of word repetition effects in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain Topogr. 2006;18(3):201–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Zhang Y, Feutl S, Hauser U, Richter-Witte C, Schmorl P, Emrich HM, Dietrich DE. Clinical correlates of word recognition memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder: an event-related potential study. Psychiatry Res. 2008;162(3):262–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Kim YY, Roh AY, Yoo SY, Kang DH, Kwon JS. Impairment of source memory in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder: equivalent current dipole analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2009;165(1–2):47–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Rugg MD, Curran T. Event-related potentials and recognition memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(6):251–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Cubitt B, Oldstone C, de la Torre JC. Sequence and genome organization of Borna disease virus. J Virol. 1994;68(3):1382–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Briese T, Schneemann A, Lewis AJ, Park YS, Kim S, Ludwig H, Lipkin WI. Genomic organization of Borna disease virus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1994;91(10):4362–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Amarasinghe GK, Bao Y, Basler CF, Bavari S, Beer M, Bejerman N, Blasdell KR, Bochnowski A, Briese T, Bukreyev A, et al. Taxonomy of the order Mononegavirales: update 2017. Arch Virol. 2017;162(8):2493–504.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Ludwig H, Bode L. Borna disease virus: new aspects on infection, disease, diagnosis and epidemiology. Rev Sci Tech. 2000;19(1):259–88.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Guo Y, He P, Sun L, Zhang X, Xu X, Tang T, Zhou W, Li Q, Zou D, Bode L, et al. Full-length genomic sequencing and characterization of Borna disease virus 1 isolates: Lessons in epidemiology. J Med Virol. 2020;92(12):3125–37.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Bode L, Xie P, Dietrich DE, Zaliunaite V, Ludwig H. Are human Borna disease virus 1 infections zoonotic and fatal? Lancet Infect Dis. 2020;20(6):650–1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Bode L, Durrwald R, Rantam FA, Ferszt R, Ludwig H. First isolates of infectious human Borna disease virus from patients with mood disorders. Mol Psychiatry. 1996;1(3):200–12.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Nakamura Y, Takahashi H, Shoya Y, Nakaya T, Watanabe M, Tomonaga K, Iwahashi K, Ameno K, Momiyama N, Taniyama H, et al. Isolation of Borna disease virus from human brain tissue. J Virol. 2000;74(10):4601–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Bode L, Ludwig H. Borna disease virus infection, a human mental-health risk. Clin Microbiol Rev. 2003;16(3):534–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Bode L, Reckwald P, Severus WE, Stoyloff R, Ferszt R, Dietrich DE, Ludwig H. Borna disease virus-specific circulating immune complexes, antigenemia, and free antibodies–the key marker triplet determining infection and prevailing in severe mood disorders. Mol Psychiatry. 2001;6(4):481–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Schlottau K, Forth L, Angstwurm K, Hoper D, Zecher D, Liesche F, Hoffmann B, Kegel V, Seehofer D, Platen S, et al. Fatal Encephalitic Borna Disease Virus 1 in Solid-Organ Transplant Recipients. N Engl J Med. 2018;379(14):1377–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Korn K, Coras R, Bobinger T, Herzog SM, Lucking H, Stohr R, Huttner HB, Hartmann A, Ensser A. Fatal Encephalitis Associated with Borna Disease Virus 1. N Engl J Med. 2018;379(14):1375–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Liesche F, Ruf V, Zoubaa S, Kaletka G, Rosati M, Rubbenstroth D, Herden C, Goehring L, Wunderlich S, Wachter MF, et al. The neuropathology of fatal encephalomyelitis in human Borna virus infection. Acta Neuropathol. 2019;138(4):653–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Niller HH, Angstwurm K, Rubbenstroth D, Schlottau K, Ebinger A, Giese S, Wunderlich S, Banas B, Forth LF, Hoffmann D, et al. Zoonotic spillover infections with Borna disease virus 1 leading to fatal human encephalitis, 1999–2019: an epidemiological investigation. Lancet Infect Dis. 2020;20(4):467–77.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Tappe D, Portner K, Frank C, Wilking H, Ebinger A, Herden C, Schulze C, Muntau B, Eggert P, Allartz P, et al. Investigation of fatal human Borna disease virus 1 encephalitis outside the previously known area for human cases, Brandenburg, Germany - a case report. BMC Infect Dis. 2021;21(1):787.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Gosztonyi G, Ludwig H. Borna disease of horses. An immunohistological and virological study of naturally infected animals. Acta Neuropathol. 1984;64(3):213–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Gosztonyi G, Ludwig H, Bode L, Kao M, Sell M, Petrusz P, Halasz B. Obesity induced by Borna disease virus in rats: key roles of hypothalamic fast-acting neurotransmitters and inflammatory infiltrates. Brain Struct Funct. 2020;225(5):1459–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Bode L, Guo Y, Xie P: Molecular epidemiology of human Borna disease virus 1 infection revisited. Emerg Microbes Infect. 2022;11(1):1335-8.

  57. Bode L. Human infections with Borna disease virus and potential pathogenic implications. Curr Top Microbiol Immunol. 1995;190:103–30.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Bode L, Zimmermann W, Ferszt R, Steinbach F, Ludwig H. Borna disease virus genome transcribed and expressed in psychiatric patients. Nat Med. 1995;1(3):232–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Tomonaga K. Virus-induced neurobehavioral disorders: mechanisms and implications. Trends Mol Med. 2004;10(2):71–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Deuschle M, Bode L, Heuser I, Schmider J, Ludwig H. Borna disease virus proteins in cerebrospinal fluid of patients with recurrent depression and multiple sclerosis. Lancet. 1998;352(9143):1828–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Iwahashi K, Watanabe M, Nakamura K, Suwaki H, Nakaya T, Nakamura Y, Takahashi H, Ikuta K. Clinical investigation of the relationship between Borna disease virus (BDV) infection and schizophrenia in 67 patients in Japan. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1997;96(6):412–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Chen CH, Chiu YL, Shaw CK, Tsai MT, Hwang AL, Hsiao KJ. Detection of Borna disease virus RNA from peripheral blood cells in schizophrenic patients and mental health workers. Mol Psychiatry. 1999;4(6):566–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Ikuta K, Ibrahim MS, Kobayashi T, Tomonaga K. Borna disease virus and infection in humans. Front Biosci. 2002;7:d470-495.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Bode L, Dietrich DE, Stoyloff R, Emrich HM, Ludwig H. Amantadine and human Borna disease virus in vitro and in vivo in an infected patient with bipolar depression. Lancet. 1997;349(9046):178–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Liu S, Bode L, Zhang L, He P, Huang R, Sun L, Chen S, Zhang H, Guo Y, Zhou J, et al. GC-MS-Based Metabonomic Profiling Displayed Differing Effects of Borna Disease Virus Natural Strain Hu-H1 and Laboratory Strain V Infection in Rat Cortical Neurons. Int J Mol Sci. 2015;16(8):19347–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Dietrich DE, Bode L, Spannhuth CW, Hecker H, Ludwig H, Emrich HM. Antiviral treatment perspective against Borna disease virus 1 infection in major depression: a double-blind placebo-controlled randomized clinical trial. BMC Pharmacol Toxicol. 2020;21(1):12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Ferszt R, Kuhl KP, Bode L, Severus EW, Winzer B, Berghofer A, Beelitz G, Brodhun B, Muller-Oerlinghausen B, Ludwig H. Amantadine revisited: an open trial of amantadinesulfate treatment in chronically depressed patients with Borna disease virus infection. Pharmacopsychiatry. 1999;32(4):142–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Dietrich DE, Bode L, Spannhuth CW, Lau T, Huber TJ, Brodhun B, Ludwig H, Emrich HM. Amantadine in depressive patients with Borna disease virus (BDV) infection: an open trial. Bipolar Disord. 2000;2(1):65–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Gosztonyi G. Natural and experimental Borna disease virus infections–neuropathology and pathogenetic considerations. APMIS Suppl. 2008;124:53–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Gonzalez-Dunia D, Volmer R, Mayer D, Schwemmle M. Borna disease virus interference with neuronal plasticity. Virus Res. 2005;111(2):224–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Heimrich B, Hesse DA, Wu YJ, Schmid S, Schwemmle M. Borna disease virus infection alters synaptic input of neurons in rat dentate gyrus. Cell Tissue Res. 2009;338(2):179–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Rubin SA, Sylves P, Vogel M, Pletnikov M, Moran TH, Schwartz GJ, Carbone KM. Borna disease virus-induced hippocampal dentate gyrus damage is associated with spatial learning and memory deficits. Brain Res Bull. 1999;48(1):23–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Solbrig MV, Koob GF, Fallon JH, Reid S, Lipkin WI. Prefrontal cortex dysfunction in Borna disease virus (BDV)–infected rats. Biol Psychiatry. 1996;40(7):629–36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Jie J, Xu X, Xia J, Tu Z, Guo Y, Li C, Zhang X, Wang H, Song W, Xie P. Memory Impairment Induced by Borna Disease Virus 1 Infection is Associated with Reduced H3K9 Acetylation. Cell Physiol Biochem. 2018;49(1):381–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Dittrich W, Bode L, Ludwig H, Kao M, Schneider K. Learning deficiencies in Borna disease virus-infected but clinically healthy rats. Biol Psychiatry. 1989;26(8):818–28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Mazaheri-Tehrani E, Maghsoudi N, Shams J, Soori H, Atashi H, Motamedi F, Bode L, Ludwig H. Borna disease virus (BDV) infection in psychiatric patients and healthy controls in Iran. Virol J. 2014;11:161.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Kamitani W, Ono E, Yoshino S, Kobayashi T, Taharaguchi S, Lee BJ, Yamashita M, Okamoto M, Taniyama H, Tomonaga K, et al. Glial expression of Borna disease virus phosphoprotein induces behavioral and neurological abnormalities in transgenic mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(15):8969–74.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Betourne A, Szelechowski M, Thouard A, Abrial E, Jean A, Zaidi F, Foret C, Bonnaud EM, Charlier CM, Suberbielle E, et al. Hippocampal expression of a virus-derived protein impairs memory in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115(7):1611–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  79. Buenz EJ, Rodriguez M, Howe CL. Disrupted spatial memory is a consequence of picornavirus infection. Neurobiol Dis. 2006;24(2):266–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Cornelius ADA, Hosseini S, Schreier S, Fritzsch D, Weichert L, Michaelsen-Preusse K, Fendt M, Kroger A. Langat virus infection affects hippocampal neuron morphology and function in mice without disease signs. J Neuroinflammation. 2020;17(1):278.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. De Chiara G, Piacentini R, Fabiani M, Mastrodonato A, Marcocci ME, Limongi D, Napoletani G, Protto V, Coluccio P, Celestino I, et al. Recurrent herpes simplex virus-1 infection induces hallmarks of neurodegeneration and cognitive deficits in mice. PLoS Pathog. 2019;15(3):e1007617.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  82. Squire LR, Knowlton B, Musen G. The structure and organization of memory. Annu Rev Psychol. 1993;44(8434894):453–95.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Meier AH: Deutsche Sprachstatistik, vol. 2. Hildesheim: Band. Olms; 1967.

  85. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4:561–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Dietrich DE, Schedlowski M, Bode L, Ludwig H, Emrich HM. A viro-psycho-immunological disease-model of a subtype affective disorder. Pharmacopsychiatry. 1998;31(3):77–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Rugg MD, Allan K: Memory retrieval: an electrophysiological perspective. In: The Cognitive Neurosciences. edn. Edited by Gazzaniga MS. Cambridge, USA: MIT Press; 1999: 805–816.

  88. Klem GH, Luders HO, Jasper HH, Elger C. The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl. 1999;52:3–6.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Rugg MD, Walla P, Schloerscheidt AM, Fletcher PC, Frith CD, Dolan RJ. Neural correlates of depth of processing effects on recollection: evidence from brain potentials and positron emission tomography. Exp Brain Res. 1998;123(1–2):18–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Rugg MD, Mark RE, Walla P, Schloerscheidt AM, Birch CS, Allan K. Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit and explicit memory. Nature. 1998;392(6676):595–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Mecklinger A. Interfacing mind and brain: a neurocognitive model of recognition memory. Psychophysiology. 2000;37(11037034):565–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Jennings JR, Wood CC. Letter: The epsilon-adjustment procedure for repeated-measures analyses of variance. Psychophysiology. 1976;13(3):277–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Johannes S, Mangun GR, Kussmaul CL, Munte TF. Brain potentials in developmental dyslexia: differential effects of word frequency in human subjects. Neurosci Lett. 1995;195(8584205):183–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Wilding EL, Rugg MD. An event-related potential study of recognition memory with and without retrieval of source. Brain. 1996;119(Pt 3):889–905.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Carbone KM, Rubin SA, Nishino Y, Pletnikov MV. Borna disease: virus-induced neurobehavioral disease pathogenesis. Curr Opin Microbiol. 2001;4(4):467–75.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Volmer R, Monnet C, Gonzalez-Dunia D. Borna disease virus blocks potentiation of presynaptic activity through inhibition of protein kinase C signaling. PLoS Pathog. 2006;2(3):e19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  97. Degenetais E, Thierry A-M, Glowinski J, Gioanni Y. Synaptic influence of hippocampus on pyramidal cells of the rat prefrontal cortex: an in vivo intracellular recording study. Cereb Cortex. 2003;13(12816894):782–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Ivanco TL, Racine RJ. Long-term potentiation in the reciprocal corticohippocampal and corticocortical pathways in the chronically implanted, freely moving rat. Hippocampus. 2000;10(10791836):143–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Rosenzweig ES, Barnes CA. Impact of aging on hippocampal function: plasticity, network dynamics, and cognition. Prog Neurobiol. 2003;69(12758108):143–79.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Ackermann A, Guelzow T, Staeheli P, Schneider U, Heimrich B. Visualizing viral dissemination in the mouse nervous system, using a green fluorescent protein-expressing Borna disease virus vector. J Virol. 2010;84(10):5438–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  101. Carbone KM, Duchala CS, Griffin JW, Kincaid AL, Narayan O. Pathogenesis of Borna disease in rats: evidence that intra-axonal spread is the major route for virus dissemination and the determinant for disease incubation. J Virol. 1987;61(11):3431–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  102. Carbone KM, Rubin SA, Sierra-Honigmann AM, Lederman HM. Characterization of a glial cell line persistently infected with borna disease virus (BDV): influence of neurotrophic factors on BDV protein and RNA expression. J Virol. 1993;67(3):1453–60.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. Gonzalez-Dunia D, Watanabe M, Syan S, Mallory M, Masliah E, De La Torre JC. Synaptic pathology in Borna disease virus persistent infection. J Virol. 2000;74(8):3441–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  104. Werner-Keiss N, Garten W, Richt JA, Porombka D, Algermissen D, Herzog S, Baumgartner W, Herden C. Restricted expression of Borna disease virus glycoprotein in brains of experimentally infected Lewis rats. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol. 2008;34(6):590–602.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Bode L. Human bornavirus infection– towards a valid diagnostic system. APMIS Suppl. 2008;124:21–39.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  106. Alonso M, Bekinschtein P, Cammarota M, Vianna MRM, Izquierdo I, Medina JH. Endogenous BDNF is required for long-term memory formation in the rat parietal cortex. Learn Mem. 2005;12(16204202):504–10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  107. Hans A, Bajramovic JJ, Syan S, Perret E, Dunia I, Brahic M, Gonzalez-Dunia D. Persistent, noncytolytic infection of neurons by Borna disease virus interferes with ERK 1/2 signaling and abrogates BDNF-induced synaptogenesis. FASEB J. 2004;18(7):863–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Lee Y-S, Silva AJ. The molecular and cellular biology of enhanced cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(19153576):126–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  109. Izquierdo I, Medina JH. Memory formation: the sequence of biochemical events in the hippocampus and its connection to activity in other brain structures. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1997;68(9398590):285–316.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Van Kolen K, Pullan S, Neefs J-M, Dautzenberg FM. Nociceptive and behavioural sensitisation by protein kinase Cepsilon signalling in the CNS. J Neurochem. 2008;104(17971128):1–13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Schmid S, Mayer D, Schneider U, Schwemmle M. Functional characterization of the major and minor phosphorylation sites of the P protein of Borna disease virus. J Virol. 2007;81(11):5497–507.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  112. Schwemmle M, De B, Shi L, Banerjee A, Lipkin WI. Borna disease virus P-protein is phosphorylated by protein kinase C epsilon and casein kinase II. J Biol Chem. 1997;272(35):21818–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Schmid S, Metz P, Prat CMA, Gonzalez-Dunia D, Schwemmle M. Protein kinase C-dependent phosphorylation of Borna disease virus P protein is required for efficient viral spread. Arch Virol. 2010;155(20333534):789–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Prat CMA, Schmid S, Farrugia F, Cenac N, Le Masson G, Schwemmle M, Gonzalez-Dunia D: Mutation of the protein kinase C site in borna disease virus phosphoprotein abrogates viral interference with neuronal signaling and restores normal synaptic activity. PLoS Pathog. 2009;5(5):e1000425. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1000425.

  115. Frank C, Wickel J, Bramer D, Matschke J, Ibe R, Gazivoda C, Gunther A, Hartmann C, Rehn K, Cadar D, et al. Human Borna disease virus 1 (BoDV-1) encephalitis cases in the north and east of Germany. Emerg Microbes Infect. 2021;11(1):1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  116. Zaliunaite V, Steibliene V, Bode L, Podlipskyte A, Bunevicius R, Ludwig H. Primary psychosis and Borna disease virus infection in Lithuania: a case control study. BMC Psychiatry. 2016;16(1):369.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  117. Liu X, Bode L, Zhang L, Wang X, Liu S, Zhang L, Huang R, Wang M, Yang L, Chen S, et al. Health care professionals at risk of infection with Borna disease virus - evidence from a large hospital in China (Chongqing). Virol J. 2015;12(1):39.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  118. Rauch SL, Dougherty DD, Cosgrove GR, Cassem EH, Alpert NM, Price BH, Nierenberg AA, Mayberg HS, Baer L, Jenike MA, et al. Cerebral metabolic correlates as potential predictors of response to anterior cingulotomy for obsessive compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50(11704072):659–67.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Rauch SL, Savage CR, Alpert NM, Dougherty D, Kendrick A, Curran T, Brown HD, Manzo P, Fischman AJ, Jenike MA. Probing striatal function in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a PET study of implicit sequence learning. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1997;9(4):568–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Eldridge LL, Engel SA, Zeineh MM, Bookheimer SY, Knowlton BJ. A dissociation of encoding and retrieval processes in the human hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2005;25(15800182):3280–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Claudia Richter-Witte, Sebastian Feutl and Philip Schmorl for data collecting, Patrizia Reckwald, Dominik Rada and Robert Stoll for excellent technical supports, and Dr. T. Münte for proofreading.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. This study was supported by the funds (HiLF 1) from the Hannover Medical School, Germany.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, design, and methodology: YZ. Clinical data collection: YZ. Virological data collection and data analysis: LB, HL. Formal analysis and investigation: YZ. Writing—original draft preparation: YZ. Writing—review and editing: YZ, HME, DED, AAPA, LB. Final version: YZ, AAPA, LB. Funding acquisition: YZ. All authors read and approved the final manuscript, except HME who passed away in September 2018 and approved a pre-final version with identical data sets and interpretation.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yuanyuan Zhang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent for participation

The local ethical committee at the Hannover Medical School, Hanover, Germany, approved the study (see Additional file 1: study history and disclaimer). All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. LB and HL report a common patent application on differential BoDV-1 diagnosis (first publication on 11th April, 2019) by the German Patent and Trade Mark Office.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Supplemental file 1.

Study history and disclaimer.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhang, Y., Alwin Prem Anand, A., Bode, L. et al. Word recognition memory and serum levels of Borna disease virus specific circulating immune complexes in obsessive–compulsive disorder. BMC Psychiatry 22, 597 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-04208-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-04208-3

Keywords