Skip to main content

Further validation of the Cognitive Biases Questionnaire for psychosis

Abstract

Background

Cognitive biases are recognized as important treatment targets for reducing symptoms associated with severe mental disorders. Although cognitive biases have been linked to symptoms in most studies, few studies have looked at such biases transdiagnostically. The Cognitive Bias Questionnaire for psychosis (CBQp) is a self-reported questionnaire that assesses cognitive biases amongst individuals with a psychotic disorder, as well as individuals with other severe mental disorders. The current study aims to validate a French version of the CBQp and to explore transdiagnostic cognitive biases in individuals with psychotic disorders, individuals with depression, and in healthy controls.

Methods

The CBQp was translated into French following a protocol based on international standards. Discriminant validity and internal consistency were determined for total score and each subscale score. Confirmatory factor analyses were performed to test construct validity. Finally, cluster analyses were conducted to investigate cognitive biases across diagnostic groups.

Results

Our results were similar to those of the original authors, with the one-factor solution (assessment of a general thinking bias) being the strongest, but the two-factor solution (assessing biases within two themes relating to psychosis) and the five-factor solution (assessment of multiple distinct biases) being clinically more interesting. A six-cluster solution emerged, suggesting that individuals with similar diagnoses score differently on all cognitive biases, and that individuals with different diagnoses might have similar cognitive biases.

Conclusions

The current findings support the validity of the French translation of the CBQp. Our cluster analyses overall support the transdiagnostic presence of cognitive biases.

Peer Review reports

Background

A transdiagnostic approach to mental health research has been recommended by The National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) [1] and is becoming increasingly popular amongst researchers. The current categorical diagnostic system is designed to facilitate the communication of information regarding epidemiology, clinical descriptions, pathogenesis, treatment options, and prognosis and outcome among treatment providers, patients, families, and the public [2]. Regarding schizophrenia and depression, Mellsop & al [2]. consider that the current classification fails to meet its objectives. Categorical approaches are widely criticized by both researchers and health professionals [2,3,4] for a plethora of reasons.

First, diagnostic criteria currently used to differentiate psychiatric disorders are solely based on symptomatology; however, identical symptoms are included as core criteria for different psychiatric disorders diagnoses. Indeed, one can observe a depressed mood in individuals with major depressive disorder, but also in those diagnosed with psychotic depression, bipolar disorder, schizoaffective disorder, or schizophrenia. Importantly, many of the negative symptoms of schizophrenia (e.g., social withdrawal, anhedonia) are considered symptoms of depression in mood disorders. Second, some diagnoses seem to fit even less in a single category. For instance, criteria for schizoaffective disorder diagnosis are among the most criticized criteria as they encompass the same symptoms as bipolar disorder, and only differ in the duration of psychotic symptoms in relation to the mood symptoms. Third, given the plurality of possible symptom profiles for a given diagnosis, it is possible for two individuals to share a diagnosis, yet exhibit highly different symptoms [5, 6]. Fourth, severe psychiatric disorders do not have a single etiology. For example, a recent genome-wide association study revealed more than 200 common risk variants for schizophrenia [7]. Moreover, studies suggest that severe psychiatric disorders may be genetically linked [8] and that many genes linked to risk for psychiatric disorders may not be diagnostically specific in their effect. Indeed, meta-analyses showed that variants on a single gene (the 5-HT2A receptor) are linked to three different disorders (schizophrenia, bulimia, and anorexia nervosa). For instance, overlapping genes on chromosome 13q (termed G30 and G72) may be associated with both schizophrenia and bipolar disorder [9], and several popular candidate genes (e.g., serotonin transporter, dopamine transporter, dopamine 2 receptor) are significantly associated with a wide variety of psychiatric disorders or psychiatrically relevant traits [10, 11]. Studies also suggest that different psychiatric disorders may be precipitated by similar environmental factors [12, 13]. Exposure to childhood adversity is a good example, as it is linked to higher rates of multiple observed disorders [14]. Risks factors for severe psychiatric disorders are thus not related to the manifestation of a particular disorder, but rather to the likelihood of developing a severe mental disorder in general. Moreover, traditional classification systems that frame mental disorder diagnoses as independent entities fail to consider high rates of observed comorbidity [15] like depression and anxiety [16], or schizophrenia and social anxiety [17]. Finally, responses to various treatments appear to be specific to the symptoms that are targeted and not to the diagnosis itself, with symptoms intensity being a better predictor of treatment needs than the given diagnosis [18]. Empirically-based psychotherapeutic interventions aim to alter the dysfunctional thought patterns and cognitive biases underlying specific symptoms and are not diagnosis-specific [13].

Mathews and MacLeod [19] defined cognitive biases as the tendency to process information in ways that favor certain types of emotional meaning or valence. We can distinguish three categories of cognitive biases: attentional biases, interpretation biases, and memory biases [19, 20]. Interpretation biases are the tendencies to interpret or infer ambiguous information according to a certain emotional meaning or valence [19].

Cognitive biases, especially interpretation biases, are recognized as important treatment targets for reducing symptoms associated with severe mental disorders and Cognitive-oriented psychotherapies are largely based on the assumption that cognitive biases are causally related to symptoms [19]. Indeed, studies show that it is possible to reduce symptoms, including depressive and psychotic ones, as well as prevent relapses by targeting cognitive biases [19].

Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is an evidence-based therapy [21, 22] that focuses on the relationship between cognitions, emotions, and behavior. A recent metaanalysis of CBT randomized controlled trials (RCT) showed that heterogeneity between RCT was low and that CBT remained effective across different conditions [23].

CBT for psychosis (CBTp) is also effective [24,25,26,27,28,29]) in reducing psychotic symptoms relapse at 12 months and improving functioning. CBTp is recommended by several clinical guidelines [30, 31] for severe mental illness to diminish distress or symptoms linked to psychotic disorders.CBTp and aims to modify, amongst other things, beliefs underlying an individual’s hallucinations and delusions by targeting the cognitive biases at play. Metacognitive training (MCT) [32] also is another evidence-based therapy that targets cognitive biases. Participants learn to modify biases that are linked to psychotic symptoms via trainings following specific modules [33]. Meta-analyses on MCT interventions have demonstrated small-to-moderate effects on positive symptoms [34, 35]. Cognitive bias modification training (CBMT) [36] also aims at modifying cognitive biases, although mostly attentional biases, specific to facial emotion recognition for instance. Other trainings exist, such as the Maudsley review training program (MRTP) [37], a computerized program that aims at decreasing Jumping to Conclusions via reasoning training (RT) [38], or Michael’s game, a card game designed to help people with psychotic disorders find alternative explanations for various situations that vary in paranoid intensity [39].

Interventions or trainings targeting cognitive biases may also exert positive effects on lack of clinical (unawareness of being ill) and cognitive (self-reflectiveness and self-certainty) insight [40, 41]. A recent meta-analysis also suggests that, overall, interventions targeting cognitive biases have a small, positive and statistically significant effect on the reduction of cognitive biases, a moderate significant positive effect on the improvement of psychotic symptoms, and a moderate significant positive effect on the improvement of patients’ insight levels [33].

Although cognitive biases have been linked to symptoms in most studies, few studies have looked at such biases transdiagnostically. Cognitive biases can be assessed through a variety of experimental tasks or self-reported questionnaires. The Beads task [42], for instance, has been extensively used for the Jumping to Conclusions bias [43,44,45].

Several questionnaires have been developed to assess various biases, including the Attributional Style Questionnaire [46], the Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire [47], and the Ambiguous Intentions Hostility Questionnaire [48]. Most have a narrow focus and only target one type of cognitive bias, and as such do not provide a comprehensive assessment of an individual’s cognitive biases.

As reported by Peters and colleagues [49], several questionnaires evaluating the “Beck biases” (i.e., arbitrary interference, selective abstraction, magnification, minimisation, overgeneralisation, and personalisation) [50] are available in the mood disorder literature [51,52,53,54,55,56,57]. Peters and colleagues [49] thought that many of these questionnaires were less appropriate for people with a severe mental disorder or with occupational and social dysfunction, because they refer to work or social circumstances that might be quite different from theirs (e.g., “You noticed recently that a lot of your friends are taking up golf and tennis” [49, 56]. As a result, they developed the Cognitive Bias Questionnaire for psychosis (CBQp), a self-reported questionnaire that assesses cognitive biases (interpretation biases) and involves a wide range of thinking styles commonly observed among individuals with a psychotic disorder, as well as in individuals with other severe mental disorders. The CBQp is easy to use and was designed to be useful in both clinical and research settings. This comprehensive self-reported questionnaire enables the assessment of multiple biases concurrently, including: Catastrophising (predicting negative events in the future), Dichotomous Thinking (all-or nothing thinking), Emotional Reasoning (the use of subjective emotions to form conclusions); Intentionalising (thinking negative scenarios were committed on purpose), and Jumping to Conclusions (taking hasty decisions without having a sufficient amount of evidence).

Peters and colleagues [49]) validated their questionnaire in three populations, that is individuals with psychosis, with depression, and healthy controls. It has shown good internal consistency (α = 0.89) as a single factor and excellent test-retest reliability (α = 0.96). Scores on the CBQp questionnaire have been associated to those obtained on the Psychotic Symptoms Ratings Scales (PSYRATS) [58], the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) [59], and the Beck Anxiety Inventory [60], providing good rationale for its concurrent validity.

Construct validity was investigated by correlating CBQp total and theme scores with the CST [51], and each of the five cognitive biases measured with its equivalent task or questionnaire (scores on the self items themes of the CST for Emotional Reasoning, the Beads Task [60] for Jumping to Conclusion, the Catastrophising Interview [61] for Catastrophising, the number of extreme responses on the Dysfunctional attitudes scale (DAS) [62] for Dichotomous Thinking, and the Ambiguous Intentions and Hostility Questionnaire (AIHQ) [48] for Intentionalising (accidental and ambiguous scenarios only). None of the CBQp individual bias scores were related to a task equivalent, apart from Emotional Reasoning and the CST self-based items. Moreover, the total CBQp score was correlated with the CST. The authors suggested that the demonstration of the construct validity of the individual CBQp biases was, to some degree, hampered by the lack of appropriate measures available in the literature [49].

Healthy controls scored significantly lower on the CBQp relative to the other group s[49].. Interestingly, CBQp total scores did not distinguish individuals with depressive and psychotic disorders, suggesting that these groups may present with similar cognitive biases. Based on their cluster factor analysis (CFA), the questionnaire seemed to assess a general thinking bias (1-factor), but the 2-factor model (assessing biases within two themes relating to psychosis; Anomalous Perception and Threatening Events) was the best fit when the factors were assumed to be related, and the 5-factor model factor (assessment of multiple biases) also showed a reasonable fit. The CBQp has been translated and validated in several languages, namely Flemish [63], Indonesian [64], Japanese [65], and Italian [66] (Pozza & Dettore, 2017). Thus far, the CBQp has yet to be validated in French.

Accordingly, the current study’s objective is to validate a French version of the CBQp and to replicate Peters and colleagues’ [42] findings by exploring transdiagnostic cognitive biases in individuals with psychotic disorders, individuals with mood disorder (depression), and in healthy controls. The study aims to: 1) translate the CBQp in French; 2) determine the validity and reliability of the French version; 3) verify its factorial structure; and 4) explore cognitive biases across diagnostic groups. Akin to Peters and colleagues (2014), we expected the French CBQp to be valid and reliable. Further, we expected that a one-factor or two-factor structure would be the best fit. We also expected that both the psychosis and depression groups would score higher on the CBQp relative to controls and that similar biases would be found across diagnostic groups.

Methods

The study was cross-sectional, with participants answering the questionnaire only once.

Participants

The Cognitive Bias Questionnaire for Psychosis (CBQp) [49] was administered to the control group (N = 663) either in paper format to several undergraduate classes at the University of Montreal or online through a survey website that was shared across several social media networks. Data from 84 participants were excluded due to too many missing answers. Therefore, 579 control participants were retained for statistical analysis. In addition to the controls, 62 participants with either a psychotic disorder (N = 30) or mood disorder (depression; N = 32) responded to the paper version. These participants were recruited through other ongoing studies in Quebec and France. Participants with a psychotic disorder were receiving services at a clinic for psychotic disorders and did not present with substance-induced psychosis. Participants with a mood disorder had been diagnosed with major depressive disorder and were currently on sick leave from work. Participants with a comorbid substance abuse disorder were not excluded, but those with a documented intellectual disability were. All participants provided socio-demographic as well as diagnostic information. The diagnoses were confirmed by a Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (SCID) [67] or a psychiatric evaluation (N = 14) by a psychiatrist specializing in severe mental disorders.

Measures

The CBQp is a 30-item questionnaire designed to assess cognitive biases. The questionnaire presents situations frequently encountered in everyday life, along with three response choices that reflect what an individual might think in these situations. Answers with an absence of bias merit one point, those with a presence of bias with some qualification receive two points, and those with a presence of bias are given a score of three points. The more biased the answers, the higher the score. The subscales are meant to measure: Intentionalising, Catastrophising, Jumping to Conclusion, Dichotomous Thinking and Emotional Reasoning biases. In addition, the questions were inspired by two themes relating to psychosis; Anomalous Perceptions (AP) and Threatening Event (TE).

The CBQp was translated into French from the original English version following a precise translation protocol based on international standards. Initially, we contacted the original authors of the CBQp and obtained their formal authorization to conduct the adaptation of the instrument into French. The CBQp was translated from English to French. The resulting French version was then translated back again to English by two experts. Those back-translations were compared to the original version by a third expert to identify and fix any discrepancies (reverse translation technique) [68]. The resulting French version was then checked by a translator to get the final version: “Le Questionnaire de Biais Cognitifs pour la psychose”. This translation technique was selected because it reduces the researcher’s bias, a translation that draws on the researcher’s culture and understanding. Translators tried to preserve semantic and inferential equivalence as much as possible.

Statistics and procedures

Discriminant validity was determined via mean comparisons between groups for total scores and subscales scores measured. To test construct validity of the French version of the CBQp, confirmatory factor analyses were performed with R [69], according to the methods of Corbière & Larivière [70], who propose a probit model on categorical variables with a diagonally weighted least squares (DWLS) method. We investigated the one-, two-, and five-factor models to determine whether the model theorized by the original authors [49] would be preserved in our French version. A series of one-way ANOVAs were also conducted to compare the effect of diagnosis on the total score and each factorial solution of the CBQp. Since the assumption of homogeneity of variance was not met, ANOVAs were conducted with a Welch test, as well as Games-Howell post hoc procedure. Finally, cluster analyses were conducted to investigate cognitive biases across diagnostic groups (without the healthy controls). The variables used for the cluster analyses were the score for the five cognitive biases (Catastrophising, Dichotomous Thinking, Emotional Reasoning, Intentionalising and Jumping to Conclusions). Scores were standardized prior to the cluster analysis. We conducted a Clustering ensemble to ensure a more rigorous approach to cluster analyses [71, 72], which includes three methods (Hclust: Hierarchical clustering in R, with Ward Linkage [73]; the DB scan [74] and the K-means Clustering [75]). The data was then clustered using the number of times each pair of subjects had been classified together in the same group as a proximity value. The solution based on this last clustering was our final solution. We described each cluster profile and we compared them with chi-squared (for categorical variables) and ANOVAs (for continuous variables) based on diagnosis, age, gender, total cognitive bias score, and each of the cognitive bias scores. Post-hoc analyses with Bonferroni correction were conducted when a between-group difference was found.

Once the final cluster profile was selected, belonging to a particular cluster was used as the dependent variable in a multinomial logistic regression to test if diagnosis and demographics (age and gender) predicted cognitive bias profiles. A multinomial logistic regression model is appropriate instead of running three separate regression models because separate logistic regression models for each indicator variable are estimated simultaneously, allowing for mutually exclusive response categories to be analyzed without the need for overlapping reference categories [76, 77].

Results

The socio-demographic and diagnostic information for each group (healthy control, psychosis group, and depression group) are reported in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic and clinical information for the three groups

Ability to discriminate between diagnostic group and healthy controls

Total scores and subscale scores on the CBQp are shown next to those from Peters and colleagues [49] in Table 2. For all the subscales, our healthy control group consistently scored slightly higher (i.e., more biases) relative to that of Peters and colleagues [49], while our depression group consistently scored slightly lower. Our psychosis group showed comparable results to those in Peters and colleagues’ [49] study.

Table 2 Comparison between the French validation and the original study

Overall, as can be observed in Table 3, the psychosis group scored higher than one or both comparison groups for each subscale and total score. No significant differences were found between psychosis and depression groups for subscales Anomalous Perception, Dichotomous Thinking and Emotional Reasoning, but the differences remained throughout between the psychosis and the control groups. No differences were noted between the depression and control groups.

Table 3 Effect of diagnosis on total score and each subscore of the CBQ(p)

Confirmatory factor analysis

Healthy control group

Confirmatory factor analysis revealed that the one-factor model explained 86.6% of the saturated model, with a Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) of .035, and a Percent Confidence Interval RMSE of (.030, .039). The two-factor model explained 88.2% of the saturated model, with a CFI of .882, a RMSEA of .033 and a Percent Confidence Interval RMSE of (.028, .037). The five-factor model explained 89.5% of the saturated model, with a RMSEA of .031 and a Percent Confidence Interval RMSE of (.026, .036; see Table 4).

Table 4 Goodness of Fit for the CFA, comparison between the French version and the original study

Clinical groups

Regarding the factorial validation with the clinical groups (pooled together), the one-factor model explained 92.0% of the saturated model, with a RMSEA of .081 and a Percent Confidence Interval RMSE of (.063, .098). The two-factor model explained 92.6% of the saturated model, with a RMSEA of .078 and a Percent Confidence Interval RMSE of (.059, .095). The five-factor model explained 92.2%of the saturated model, with a RMSEA of .081 and a Percent Confidence Interval RMSE of (.063, .098; See Table 4).

Cognitive biases profiles

The first cluster analysis, the Hierarchical clustering, suggested a six- to seven-cluster solution (see Fig. 1a). The second cluster analysis, the DB-scan, favored a solution with only one group (see Fig. 1b), thus it was not useful for our objective. The third cluster analysis, the K-means clustering, suggested four to six profiles (see Fig. 1c.). The six-profile solution emerged from the final cluster analysis, the Clustering ensemble (see Fig. 1d). For a visual presentation of the cognitive biases’ profiles, see Fig. 2. For descriptive information for each profile, see Table 5.ANOVAs showed that the association between total score and profiles was significant, F(5,56) = 118.28, p < .001, as for the association between each cognitive biases scores and profiles (Intentionalising: F(5,56) = 23.83, p < .001; Catastrophising: F(5,56) = 36.13, p < .001); Dichotomous Thinking: F(5,56) = 17.63, p < .001); Emotional Reasoning: F(5,56) = 21.36, p < .001); Jumping to Conclusions: F(5,56) = 33.67, p < .001). See S.2 to S.8 in Supplementary Materials for the post hoc analysis of each ANOVA. Bonferroni correction was applied to post hoc analyses (p < .003). Cognitive bias scores for each profile will be presented by profiles, with the profiles with highest total score presented first. Scores results are reported as (Mean ± SD).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Cluster analyses a. Hierarchical clustering in R, with Ward Linkage b. DB scan c. K-means Clustering d. Clustering ensemble

Fig. 2
figure 2

Cognitive biases Profiles

Table 5 Demographics, diagnosis, and CBQ(p) scores for the six profiles

Profile 6 had higher total score (61.38 ± 4.97) and individual cognitive bias score than every other profile. Most differences were significant at p < .05 and survived the Bonferroni correction.

Profile 2 had a higher total score (54.88 ± 1.55) than Profiles 1, 3, 4, and 5 (ps < .003). Profile 2 had higher results for every cognitive biases, with Intentionalising being his lowest score (8.88 ± 0.64). Intentionalising was still significantly higher for Profile 2 than for Profiles 1, 3 (ps < .05) and 4 (p < .003).

Profile 5 had a total score of 47.43 ± 1.22, significantly higher than Profiles 1 and 4, but significantly lower than Profiles 2 and 6 (ps < .003). The total score of Profile 5 was not significantly different from that of Profile 3 (p > .05). The highest score was for Emotional Reasoning (11.30 ± 0.89).

Profile 3 had a total score of 44.63 ± 2.57, lower than Profiles 2 and 6 (p < .003), but higher than Profile 4 (p < .003), and similar to Profile 5 (p > .05). The highest score for Profile 3 is Jumping to Conclusion.

Profile 1 had lower total score (38.33 ± 1.91) than every other profile (p < .003) except for Profile 4 (p > .05). The highest score of Profile 1 is Jumping to Conclusion, but visually looking at the data, it is still lower than for every other profile (significantly lower than Profiles 2, 3 and 6, ps < .003).

Profile 4 had a total score of 37.75 ± 2.87, similar to profile 1 (p > .05), and significantly lower than every other profile (p < .003). Visually, the highest scores seem to be for Catastrophizing and Jumping to Conclusion.

An ANOVA showed a significant association between age and profile, F(5.55) = 4.63, p = .001 (see Table 5). Post hoc analysis indicated a difference between Profile 1 and every other profile. However, only the difference between Profile 1 and both the Profiles 2 and 6 survived the Bonferroni correction, with Profile 1 being older (45.61 ± 9.35 years old) relative to Profile 2 (30.57 ± 5.62; p = .009 years) and Profile 6 (30.50 ± 4.72; p = .016). No significant differences were observed between other profiles based on this variable, ps > .05 (see S.1 in Supplementary Materials).

Chi-squared tests showed that the association between gender and profile was not significant, X2 (61) = 8.05, p = .153. However, visually, we can observe a greater proportion of women in Profile 4 (80.0%, adjusted residual = 1.9) and a greater proportion of men in Profile 6 (83.3%, adjusted residual = 2.2; See Table 5). However, the association between diagnosis and profiles was significant, X2 (62) = 11.69, p = .039. Profile 1 had a significantly greater proportion of individuals with depression than other profiles (72.2%, adjusted residuals = 2.2). Only the difference with Profile 2 survived to post hoc analysis (p < .05), but did not survive the Bonferroni correction (see S.2 in Supplementary Material). Although not significant, we can visually observe that Profile 4 also had a greater proportion of individuals with depression (66.7%, adjusted residuals = 1.5), and that Profiles 2, 3, and 6 have a greater proportion of individuals with psychosis (respectively, 75%, adjusted residuals = 1.5; 70%, adjusted residual = 1.4; and 83.3%, adjusted residuals = 1.7). We can see that Profile 4 had a similar proportion of men and women (see Table 5).

Since the psychosis group and the depression group were not balanced in terms of age and gender, we performed stratifications before running the multinomial regression. However, there were too few individuals in each cell for interpretation, and the results were not significant, p > .05. Therefore, we ran the multinomial regression without stratifications. Among demographic variables (diagnosis, age, and gender), only age continued to be statistically different among the Profiles when variables are considered together, p < .01.

Discussion

The current study aimed at replicating Peters and colleagues’ [49] study with a French sample. Compared to Peters and colleagues’ results [49], our control group showed higher scores, our depression group showed lower scores, and our psychosis group showed similar scores. It is not surprising that our control group had higher scores since most participants in this group were recruited from university courses. That is, several studies indicate that university students have a higher prevalence of depressive symptoms than the general population. A survey conducted by the National College Health Assessment with 43,780 students indicated that 44% of respondents have experienced symptoms of depression in the last 12 months [78]. According to a survey of 10,000 students from the student association Fédération des Associations Étudiantes du Campus de l’Université de Montréal (FAÉCUM), 22% reported moderate-to-severe depressive symptoms or severe depression, 5.8% reported symptoms of exhaustion, and 7.8% reported having seriously thought about killing themselves in the past 12 months [79]. Furthermore, our depression group had lower scores relative to those from Peters and colleagues’ [49] study. This may be explained by the fact that individuals in our depression group were recruited through another study where participants had been on sick leave due to depression and were now returning to work. Although some participants still presented with severe depressive symptoms, most had experienced a reduction in symptoms. Furthermore, our results reflect the transdiagnostic character of cognitive biases; an individual’s score does not only depend on their diagnosis, but also on their current condition and level of distress. In their meta-analysis about attributional biases and the capacity to discriminate internal and external events, Brookwell, Bentall & Varese [80] suggest that the cognitive biases they studied were not diagnostic-specific, but could be accounted for by the presence of hallucinatory experiences. Future studies should study cognitive bias profiles in relation to specific symptoms, including symptom severity, rather than diagnoses.

Based on prior studies [49, 63, 65], we expected that the one-factor structure would prevail over the two- or five-factor structure, for both the control and clinical groups. This hypothesis was confirmed for the control group, suggesting that biases tend to co-exist. For example, an individual with a strong Catastrophising bias is likely to also have a strong Intentionalising bias. This does not mean that the questionnaire measures a single concept, but rather, that they are tightly connected. For the clinical group, all three models were acceptable, although the small sample resulted in less-than-ideal RMSEA.

The three-step cluster analyses aimed to determine profiles of individuals based on their cognitive biases, assessed by the CBQp and to compare the resulting clusters on sociodemographic (i.e., age, gender) and clinical (i.e., diagnosis) characteristics. Consistent with our hypothesis, more than two clusters emerged, suggesting that individuals with similar diagnoses do not score the same on all cognitive biases, and that individuals with different diagnoses might have similar cognitive biases. The clusters indicated that although some people seem to score similarly across biases, suggesting a tendency to have many biases together (as the one-factor model suggests), others have a strong tendency for presenting with some biases above all others.

All profiles were significantly different in terms of cognitive biases, yet not surprising since clustering was performed on those specific variables. It is interesting to notice that individuals with some of the highest bias scores are younger (Profiles 2 and 6), whereas those with some of the fewest biases (Profile 1) are older. It is possible that these biases tend to decrease as individuals age; however, it is also plausible that this finding could be better explained by the fact that Profile 1 had a greater percentage of individuals with depression, and that our depression group was older than our psychosis group. Because age was the only variable that still predicted profiles after the multinomial regression, a larger sample will be needed to truly determine whether diagnosis or gender are significant predictors of clusters.

Our results suggest that, although some biases might be more present in individuals with specific symptoms (e.g., Jumping to Conclusions is more typical in profiles with more individuals with psychosis) or from a specific gender (e.g., Emotional Reasoning is more prevalent in profiles with more women), these are not specific to these individuals and can be found in other profiles as well.

Limitations

Several limitations of the present study must be acknowledged. First, the healthy control group was homogeneous and included only university students – different results might be obtained with a different sample. Another limitation is that some diagnoses were not confirmed by a structured clinical interview. For the validation of the factorial structure of the questionnaire for the clinical group, a larger sample would be needed to confirm our results. Furthermore, for the cluster analysis, there were significant demographic differences between groups, with individuals in the depression group being older than those of the psychosis group. Individuals in the depression group were mostly female whereas individuals in the psychosis group were mostly males. Although this represents a clinical reality, it may have biased our results. Because our sample was too small to use stratifications, we can only conclude about the effect of age on the profiles.

Research and clinical implication

This study demonstrated the validity of the French version of the CBQp, therefore allowing clinicians and researchers to easily assess cognitive biases in French-speaking participants. It is a simple and efficient questionnaire that can be used evaluate whether participants receiving CBTp, for instance, have experienced changes in their cognitive biases following the therapy. Our findings highlight the interdependence of different cognitive biases and could be indicative of a common cognitive process leading to the development of several biases. Moritz and al [81]. also suggest that cognitive mechanisms for delusions only partially overlap and recommend continuing studying and targeting specific biases via metacognitive training.

According to Garety and Freeman [82], cognitive biases contribute to maintaining psychotic symptoms such as delusions. For instance, a dichotomous style of thinking would seem to favor the maintenance of delusions, while Jumping to Conclusions would favor both the emergence and the maintenance of psychotic symptoms [83]. To date, most studies have focused on a specific cognitive bias, whereas our study suggests that studying or addressing multiple biases together might be useful. From our clinical experience, individuals in group CBT for psychosis for instance, benefit from learning about and recognizing their own cognitive biases, as this helps them take a step back from distressing thoughts and work on strategies to verify and modify their current beliefs.

Conclusion

The current findings support the validity of the French translation of the CBQp. We obtained results similar to the original validation study of the questionnaire. The authors of the original CBQp have recommended using one-, two-, or five-factor structure. While the results of our study are similar, the 5-factor solution is clinically more interesting. Our cluster analyses overall support the transdiagnostic presence of cognitive biases.

Availability of data and materials

The authors do not have ethical approval to share the raw data that support the findings. However, the datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Cuthbert BN, Insel TR. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med. 2013;11(1):126. https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-11-126.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Mellsop GW, Menkes DB, El-Badri SM. Classification in psychiatry: does it deliver in schizophrenia and depression? Int J Ment Heal Syst. 2007;1(1):1–4.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Cuthbert BN. The RDoC framework: facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry. 2014;13(1):28–35. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20087.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Khoury B, Langer EJ, Pagnini F. The DSM: mindful science or mindless power? 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Costello CG. Research on symptoms versus research on syndromes: arguments in favour of allocating more research time to the study of symptoms. Br J Psychiatry. 1992;160(3):304–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Ripke S, Walters JT, O’Donovan MC. Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. In: Mapping genomic loci prioritises genes and implicates synaptic biology in schizophrenia; 2020. MedRxiv.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet. 2013;381(9875):1371–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62129-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Owen MJ, Williams NM, O'Donovan MC. The molecular genetics of schizophrenia: new findings promise new insights. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9(1):14–27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Ueno SI. Genetic polymorphisms of serotonin and dopamine transporters in mental disorders. J Med Investig. 2003;50(1/2):25–31.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Noble EP. D2 dopamine receptor gene in psychiatric and neurologic disorders and its phenotypes. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2003;116(1):103–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Krueger RF, Eaton NR. Transdiagnostic factors of mental disorders. World Psychiatry. 2015;14(1):27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Bentall RP. Madness explained: psychosis and human nature: Penguin UK; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Eaton NR. Transdiagnostic psychopathology factors and sexual minority mental health: evidence of disparities and associations with minority stressors. Psychol Sex Orientat Gend Divers. 2014;1(3):244.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Eaton NR, Rodriguez-Seijas C, Carragher N, Krueger RF. Transdiagnostic factors of psychopathology and substance use disorders: a review. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2015;50(2):171–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kaufman J, Charney D. Comorbidity of mood and anxiety disorders. Depress Anxiety. 2000;12(S1):69–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Michail M, Birchwood M. Social anxiety disorder in first-episode psychosis: incidence, phenomenology and relationship with paranoia. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195(3):234–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. McGorry PD, Hickie IB, Yung AR, Pantelis C, Jackson HJ. Clinical staging of psychiatric disorders: a heuristic framework for choosing earlier, safer and more effective interventions. Aust New Zealand J Psychiatry. 2006;40(8):616–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Mathews A, MacLeod C. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1(1):167–95. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Gotlib IH, Joormann J. Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:285–312.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Hofmann SG, Asnaani A, Vonk IJ, Sawyer AT, Fang A. The efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy: a review of meta-analyses. Cogn Ther Res. 2012;36(5):427–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical; 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Fordham B, Sugavanam T, Edwards K, Stallard P, Howard R, das Nair, R., ... & Lamb, S. E. The evidence for cognitive behavioural therapy in any condition, population or context: a meta-review of systematic reviews and panoramic meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2021;51(1):21–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Bighelli I, Rodolico A, García-Mieres H, Pitschel-Walz G, Hansen WP, Schneider-Thoma J, et al. Psychosocial and psychological interventions for relapse prevention in schizophrenia: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2021;8(11):969–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Turner DT, Burger S, Smit F, Valmaggia LR, van der Gaag M. What constitutes sufficient evidence for case formulation–driven CBT for psychosis? Cumulative meta-analysis of the effect on hallucinations and delusions. Schizophr Bull. 2020;46(5):1072–85.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Norman R, Lecomte T, Addington D, Anderson E. Canadian treatment guidelines; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Burns AM, Erickson DH, Brenner CA. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for medication-resistant psychosis: a meta-analytic review. Psychiatr Serv. 2014;65(7):874–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Hutton P, Taylor PJ. Cognitive behavioural therapy for psychosis prevention: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2014;44(3):449–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Wykes T, Steel C, Everitt B, Tarrier N. Cognitive behavior therapy for schizophrenia: effect sizes, clinical models, and methodological rigor. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(3):523–37. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. (2019). NICEimpact mental health https://www.nice.org.uk/media/default/about/what-we-do/into-practice/measuring-uptake/niceimpact-mental-health.pdf

  30. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. (2014). Psychosis and schizophrenia in adults: prevention and management https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/cg178

  31. Moritz S, Woodward TS. Metacognitive training in schizophrenia: from basic research to knowledge translation and intervention. Curr Opinion Psychiat. 2007;20(6):619–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Sauvé G, Lavigne KM, Pochiet G, Brodeur MB, Lepage M. Efficacy of psychological interventions targeting cognitive biases in schizophrenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2020;78:101854.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Eichner C, Berna F. Acceptance and efficacy of metacognitive training (MCT) on positive symptoms and delusions in patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis taking into account important moderators. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42(4):952–62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Philipp R, Kriston L, Lanio J, Kuehne F, Haerter M, Moritz S, et al. Effectiveness of metacognitive interventions for mental disorders in adults—a systematic review and meta-analysis (METACOG). Clinical psychology & psychotherapy. 2019;26(2):227–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Steel C, Wykes T, Ruddle A, Smith G, Shah DM, Holmes EA. Can we harness computerised cognitive bias modification to treat anxiety in schizophrenia? A first step highlighting the role of mental imagery. Psychiatry Res. 2010;178(3):451–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Waller H, Freeman D, Jolley S, Dunn G, Garety P. Targeting reasoning biases in delusions: a pilot study of the Maudsley review training Programme for individuals with persistent, high conviction delusions. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2011;42(3):414–21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Ross K, Freeman D, Dunn G, Garety P. A randomized experimental investigation of reasoning training for people with delusions. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(2):324–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Khazaal Y, Favrod J, Azoulay S, Finot SC, Bernabotto M, Raffard S, et al. “Michael's Game,” a card game for the treatment of psychotic symptoms. Patient Educ Couns. 2011;83(2):210–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Andreou C, Wittekind CE, Fieker M, Heitz U, Veckenstedt R, Bohn F, et al. Individualized metacognitive therapy for delusions: a randomized controlled rater-blind study. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2017;56:144–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Favrod J, Branac A, Nguyena A, Nicoliera A, Perreta M, Rexhaja S. Effects of metacognitive training on the awareness of delusions. J Ther Comport Cogn. 2015;25:117–24.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Phillips LD, Edwards W. Conservatism in a simple probability inference task. J Exp Psychol. 1966;72(3):346–54. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0023653.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. McLean BF, Mattiske JK, Balzan RP. Association of the jumping to conclusions and evidence integration biases with delusions in psychosis: a detailed meta-analytic approach. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(344-354):6.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Dudley R, Taylor P, Wickham S, Hutton P. Psychosis, delusions and the “jumping to conclusions” reasoning Bias: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42:652–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Ross RM, McKay R, Coltheart M, Langdon R. Jumping to conclusions about the beads task? Schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of delusional ideation and data-gathering; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Peterson C, Semmel A, Von Baeyer C, Abramson LY, Metalsky GI. & Seligman. M E.

  46. Kinderman P, Bentall RP. A new measure of causal locus: the internal, personal and situational attributions questionnaire. Personal Individ Differ. 1996;20(2):261–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(95)00186-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Combs DR, Penn DL, Wicher M, Waldheter E. The ambiguous intentions hostility questionnaire (AIHQ): a new measure for evaluating hostile social-cognitive biases in paranoia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 2007;12(2):128–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800600787854.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Peters ER, Moritz S, Schwannauer M, Wiseman Z, Greenwood KE, Scott J. Beck. A. .

  49. Beck AT. Depression: clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects: University of Pennsylvania Press; 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Blackburn IM, Jones S, Lewin RJP. Cognitive style in depression. Br J Clin Psychol. 1986;25(4):241–51. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1986.tb00704.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Krantz S, Hammen CL. Assessment of cognitive bias in depression. J Abnorm Psychol. 1979;88(6):611–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.88.6.611.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Wilkinson IM, Blackburn IM. Cognitive style in depressed and recovered; 1981.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Fennell MJV, Campbell EA. The cognitions questionnaire: specific thinking errors in depression. Br J Clin Psychol. 1984;23(2):81–92. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1984.tb00631.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Beck AT, Brown G, Steer RA, Eidelson JI, Riskind JH. Differentiating anxiety and depression: a test of the cognitive content-specificity hypothesis. J Abnorm Psychol. 1987;96(3):179–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.96.3.179.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Lefebvre MF. Cognitive distortion and cognitive errors in depressed psychiatric and low back pain patients. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1981;49(4):517–25. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006x.49.4.517.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Moss-Morris R, Petrie KJ. Cognitive distortions of somatic experiences. Revision. 1997.

  57. Haddock G, McCarron J, Tarrier N, Faragher EB. Scales to measure dimensions; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961;4(6):561–71. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Steer RA, Beck AT. Beck anxiety inventory; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Garety PA, Hemsley DR, Wessely S. Reasoning in deluded schizophrenic and paranoid patients. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1991;179(4):194–201.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Vasey MW, Borkovec TD. A catastrophizing assessment of worrisome thoughts. Cogn Ther Res. 1992;16(5):505–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Weissman AMB, Beck AT. Development and validation of the dysfunctional attitudes scale. Presented at the annual meeting of the. Ontario: American Educational Research Association Toronto; 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Bastiaens T, Claes L, Smits D, De Wachter D, van der Gaag M, De Hert M. The cognitive biases questionnaire for psychosis (CBQ-P) and the Davos assessment of cognitive biases (DACOBS): validation in a Flemish sample of psychotic patients and healthy controls. Schizophr Res. 2013;147(2-3):310–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.04.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Erawati E, Keliat BA, Daulima NH. The validation of the Indonesian version of; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Ishikawa R, Ishigaki T, Kikuchi A, Matsumoto K, Kobayashi S, Morishige S, et al. Cross-cultural validation of the cognitive biases questionnaire for psychosis in Japan and examination of the relationships between cognitive biases and schizophrenia symptoms. Cognitive Therapy & Research. 2016;41(2):313–23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-016-9807-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Pozza A, Dettore D. The CBQ-p: a confirmatory study on factor structure and convergent validity with psychotic-like experiences and cognitions in adolescents and young adults. Applied Psychology Bulletin. 2017;280(65):58–69.

    Google Scholar 

  67. First MB, Williams JBW, Karg RS, Spitzer RL. In: Arlington VA, editor. Structured clinical interview for DSM-5—research version (SCID-5 for DSM-5, Research version; SCID-5-RV): American Psychiatric Association; 2015. p. 1–94.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Vallerand RJ. Towards transcultural validation methodology of psychological questionnaires: implications for french research. Can Psychol. 1989;30:662–89. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0079856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Corbière M, Larivière N. Méthodes qualitatives, quantitatives et mixtes dans la recherche en sciences humaines, sociales et de la santé: Presses de l’Université du Québec; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  71. Ronan T, Qi Z, Naegle KM. Avoiding common pitfalls when clustering biological data. Sci Signal. 2016;9(432):re6-re6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Strehl A, Ghosh J. Cluster ensembles---a knowledge reuse framework for combining multiple partitions. J Mach Learn Res. 2002;3(Dec):583–617.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Gordon AD. Classification. 2nd ed: Chapman and Hall/CRC; 1999.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  74. Campello RJ, Moulavi D, Sander J. Density-based clustering based on hierarchical density estimates. In Pacific-Asia conference on knowledge discovery and data mining. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer; 2013. p. 160–72.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Forgy, E. (1965). Cluster analysis of multivariate data: efficiency vs. interpretability of.

  76. Agresti A. An introduction to categorical data analysis: John Wiley & Sons; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Anderson CJ, Rutkowski L. Multinomial logistic regression. Best practices in quantitative methods. 2008:390–409.

  78. Esmaeelzadeh S, Moraros J, Thorpe L, Bird Y. The association between depression, anxiety and substance use among Canadian post-secondary students. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2018;14:3241.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Fédération des associations étudiantes du campus de l’Université de Montréal (FAÉCUM). (2016). Enquête sur la santé psychologique étudiante. Retrieved from http://www.faecum.qc.ca/ressources/documentation/avis-memoires-recherches-et-positions-1/enquete-sur-la-sante-psychologique-etudiante.

  80. Brookwell ML, Bentall RP, Varese F. Externalizing biases and hallucinations in source-monitoring, self-monitoring and signal detection studies: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Med. 2013;43(12):2465–75.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Moritz S, Veckenstedt R, Hottenrott B, Woodward TS, Randjbar S, Lincoln TM. Different sides of the same coin? Intercorrelations of cognitive biases in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 2010;15(4):406–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800903399993.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Garety PA, Freeman D. Cognitive approaches to delusions: a critical review of theories and evidence. Br J Clin Psychol. 1999;38(2):113–54. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466599162700.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Garety PA, Kuipers E, Fowler D, Freeman D, Bebbington PE. A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychol Med. 2001;31(2):189–95. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701003312.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Crystal Samson and Tania Lecomte contributed to the design and implementation of the research and to the analysis of the results. Crystal Samson, Tania Lecomte and Amélie M. Achim did the questionnaire translation. Crystal Samson, Audrey Francoeur and Nicolas Franck did the data collection. Crystal Samson, Tania Lecomte, Charles-Edouard Giguère, Andy Gilker and Francelyne Jean-Baptiste performed the analysis. Crystal Samson wrote the main manuscript text with input from all authors. Crystal Samson, Tania Lecomte and Veronik Sicard contributed to the writing of the manuscript. Crystal Samson designed the figures and the tables. Veronik Sicard and Tania Lecomte reviewed the figures and the tables. All authors reviewed the manuscript. Tania Lecomte supervised the project and Amélie M. Achim helped supervise the project.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tania Lecomte.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Approval was obtained from the research and ethics board of the University of Montreal. The procedures used in this study follow the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants gave informed consent.

Consent for publication

The authors affirm that participants provided informed consent for publication.

Competing interests

The authors certify that they have NO affiliations with or involvement in any organization or entity with any financial interest (such as honoraria; educational grants; participation in speakers’ bureaus; membership, employment, consultancies, stock ownership, or other equity interest; and expert testimony or patent-licensing arrangements), or non-financial interest (such as personal or professional relationships, affiliations, knowledge or beliefs) in the subject matter or materials discussed in this manuscript.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Samson, C., Achim, A.M., Sicard, V. et al. Further validation of the Cognitive Biases Questionnaire for psychosis. BMC Psychiatry 22, 560 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-04203-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-04203-8

Keywords