Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Cultural differences in positive psychotic experiences assessed with the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences-42 (CAPE-42): a comparison of student populations in the Netherlands, Nigeria and Norway

Abstract

Background

Previous studies have suggested that culture impacts the experience of psychosis. The current study set out to extend these findings by examining cultural variation in subclinical positive psychotic experiences in students from The Netherlands, Nigeria, and Norway. Positive psychotic experiences were hypothesized to (i) be more frequently endorsed by, and (ii) cause less distress in Nigerian vs. Dutch and Norwegian students.

Methods

Psychology students, aged 18 to 30 years, from universities in the Netherlands (n = 245), Nigeria (n = 478), and Norway (n = 162) were assessed cross-sectionally with regard to the frequency of subclinical positive psychotic experiences and related distress, using the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE-42). Multi-group confirmatory factor analysis and multivariate analysis of covariance were performed to assess measurement invariance of the positive symptom dimension (CAPE-Pos) and compare mean frequency and associated distress of positive psychotic experiences across study samples.

Results

Only CAPE-Pos items pertaining to the dimensions ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’ met assumptions for (partial) measurement invariance. Frequencies of these experiences were higher in the Nigerian sample, compared to both the Dutch and Norwegian samples, which were similar. In addition, levels of experience-related distress were similar or higher in the Nigerian sample compared to respectively the Dutch and Norwegian samples.

Conclusion

Although positive psychotic experiences may be more commonly endorsed in non-Western societies, our findings do not support the notion that they represent a more benign, and hence less distressing aspect of human experience. Rather, the experience of psychotic phenomena may be just as, if not more, distressing in African than in European culture. However, observed differences in CAPE-Pos frequency and distress between samples from different cultural settings may partly reflect differences in the measure rather than in the latent trait. Future studies may therefore consider further cross-cultural adaptation of CAPE-42, in addition to explicitly examining cultural acceptance of psychotic phenomena, and environmental and other known risk factors for psychosis, when comparing and interpreting subclinical psychotic phenomena across cultural groups.

Peer Review reports

Background

The psychosis phenotype appears to become manifest across a spectrum of severity, extending from subtle psychotic experiences to a clinically relevant disorder [1, 2]. Although epidemiological studies have emphasized similarities, rather than differences in the prevalence of psychotic disorders across cultures [3,4,5,6,7,8], there are cultural variations in the manifestation of psychotic experiences [9, 10] with as much as 15–30% of psychosis expression being suggested to be culture-dependent [11]. The sociocultural context in which psychotic symptoms manifest themselves, and the associated degree of preoccupation and distress are important factors determining clinical relevance, but have not been investigated much [12]. Given that subclinical psychotic experiences may predict transition to a clinically relevant psychotic disorder, knowledge of cultural diversity in manifestations of psychotic experiences is important for the proper contextual assessment and treatment of psychotic disorders [13, 14]. Thus, an increasing number of studies explore cultural mechanisms that play a role in the expression of psychopathology [15].

Cultural representations, such as values, beliefs, and attitudes, establish a general understanding of the concept of reality, and influence how members of a particular society respond to psychotic phenomena [16]. The common societal conception of values and beliefs appears to be particularly important for the theme of delusions and hallucinations [17]. Delusions of grandeur, for example, hardly exist in societies where striving for a certain social status is frowned upon [11], and visual and tactile hallucinations are reported more frequently by members of social groups that take unexplainable sensory experiences as evidence of the supernatural or divine [16, 18]. These examples illustrate that the likelihood and quality of psychotic phenomena is partly dependent on an individual’s cultural environment [13, 19, 20]. Moreover, the extent to which hallucinations and delusions are interpreted as appropriate or benign, and socially accepted, may have implications for the distress resulting from these experiences within a certain society (i.e., distress is likely lower when experiences are considered normative or appropriate; [16, 20,21,22,23,24]).

To better comprehend cross-cultural differences, several studies suggest that positive psychotic experiences are not to be interpreted as homogeneous dimensions, but are better represented as a number of symptom clusters that may vary in prevalence across cultures [25,26,27,28,29,30,31]. Indeed, a number of cross-national survey studies have reported considerable variation in the prevalence of particular psychotic experiences across groups of individuals from different parts of the world, in the absence of any relevant differences in the overall prevalence of psychotic disorder [32,33,34,35,36]. Similar findings have been reported with regard to the prevalence of positive symptom clusters across different ethnic groups within certain countries: disparity in symptom prevalence between ethnic groups exceeds disparity in prevalence of psychotic disorder [37,38,39]. This seems to be especially true for perceptual anomalies and paranoia [40,41,42]. Moreover, discrepancies between positive symptom prevalence and prevalence of a diagnosis of psychosis have been shown to be more pronounced in some ethnic groups than in others [10, 37, 39, 43], pointing towards a complex interplay of cultural and social factors (e.g. ethnic minority status) in the manifestation of psychosis [44,45,46,47].

The majority of studies assessing cross-cultural differences in prevalence of psychotic experiences have employed trained clinical interviewers or used highly structured interviews, developed in accordance with definitions and criteria of ICD-10 and/or DSM-(I)V classification systems for psychotic disorder [48]. The Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE-42) [49]–a 42-item self-report questionnaire–on the other hand, was developed specifically to measure psychosis proneness in the general population based on a severity spectrum view of the psychosis phenotype. The CAPE-42 has proven to be a stable, reliable and valid instrument [49]. A recent meta-analysis of studies employing CAPE-42 [25] supported three dimensions of positive psychotic experiences in the general population, namely ‘strange experiences’, ‘delusional ideations’ (including paranoia, grandiosity and paranormal beliefs), and ‘perceptual anomalies’ (i.e., hallucinations). Although this meta-analysis [25] included samples from different parts of the world, the focus was mainly on identifying general underlying symptom structures, rather than comparing symptom prevalence across cultural groups or study samples. Moreover, the reviewed samples were from the so-called Western World, and to date CAPE-42 psychotic experiences have been rarely investigated in African study samples [22, 42].

The current study, therefore, aimed at furthering insight into the cultural variation in positive psychotic experiences as measured by CAPE-42, by comparing prevalence and perceived distress of these experiences across student populations from universities in The Netherlands, Norway (i.e., Western and Northern Europe) and Nigeria (Yoruba homeland). The distinction between reality and fantasy has been described as being more rigid in Euro-American culture in comparison to non-Western societies, and reports of psychotic-like phenomena (e.g. paranormal beliefs, feeling the presence of and/or being controlled by a deceased family member or under the influence of a supernatural force) are therefore more likely to be labelled as pathological in Western settings [50]. A greater degree of cultural acceptance towards such phenomena outside of the Western setting may lead to more frequent endorsement of psychotic experiences [16], as supported previously by a number of cross-national reports on the prevalence of hallucinations and delusions [13, 51,52,53,54,55]. Recently, two large studies [22, 42] demonstrated cultural variation in epidemiological expression of psychotic experiences across respectively 12 and 13 countries, with highest prevalence rates in African samples. In line with these findings, we hypothesized higher prevalence of subclinical CAPE-42 psychotic experiences and lower prevalence of related distress in the Nigerian sample, compared to the Dutch and Norwegian samples. With regard to differences between the latter two samples, in Northern Europe living in the high-latitude band has been associated with higher incidence rates of psychotic disorders (the ‘latitude effect’ [56]). However, this effect possibly represents an artefact of methodological inconsistencies between studies (especially, use of register-based vs. first-contact data [57]), as there is little evidence of actual country variation in prevalence and distress of mental disorders across Europe when these inconsistencies are accounted for (see critical review by [58]). Although findings are thus inconclusive, we did not expect (pronounced) differences in prevalence and distress rates of subclinical positive psychotic experiences between Dutch and Norwegian samples, based on the assumption that participants from these countries would hold similar beliefs regarding the distinction between reality and fantasy, despite other cultural differences. In sum, we thus expected reports of hallucinations, strange experiences, and delusional ideations to be more prevalent in the Nigerian sample and, as theorized previously [16, 22, 50], the average distress related to these experiences to be relatively lower in the Nigerian sample, compared to both the Dutch and Norwegian samples. The lower prevalence of distress was based on the idea that these experiences are more likely labelled as appropriate or benign within African cultures, and hence met with more constructive attitudes [16, 20,21,22,23,24, 42, 50, 59, 60].

Methods

Sample

The non-probability sample for this study, to which we had access, consisted of 885 psychology students from the Open University of the Netherlands (n = 245), the University of Lagos, Nigeria (n = 478), and the Universities of Oslo, Trondheim and Bergen, Norway (n = 162). Inclusion criteria were: (i) aged 18–30 years old, (ii) sufficient command of the Dutch, English or Norwegian language, respectively, to understand instructions and give informed consent, and (iii) no current or lifetime history of psychotic disorder. The total sample consisted of 307 males and 576 females, ranging in age from 18 to 30 years (Mean = 23.3, SD = 3.3). Further demographic characteristics of the sample are displayed in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics and unadjusted descriptives of CAPE-Pos by sample, and tests for between-group differences

Study design and procedure

The study employed a cross-sectional survey design, asking participants to fill out a paper (Nigeria) or online self-report questionnaire (Netherlands and Norway). A lecturer of the University of Lagos directly approached Nigerian participants in 2011, handing out surveys during classes and giving participants the opportunity to directly give verbal consent and fill out the questions, which resulted in a 100% return rate. Dutch participants were recruited in 2009, and received course credits for their participation. The survey return rate was 100%. Norwegian participants were recruited through local university email services, electronic advertisements and Facebook in 2010 and, as a consequence, the total sampling frame and survey response rate are unknown. A total number of 246 Dutch, and 162 Norwegian psychology students accessed the online survey webpage, of which respectively 243 (98.8%), and 158 (97.5%) provided a complete survey response (i.e., all demographic and CAPE-42 items answered). A total number of 501 Nigerian psychology students handed in a paper survey document, of which 316 (63.1%) provided a complete response. We followed previous work on CAPE psychotic-like experiences [30], and excluded all participants with ≥ 25% CAPE-42 data missing, thereby eliminating data rows from 24 participants (< 3% of total recruited sample; one Dutch, and 23 Nigerian participants). The final dataset for analysis therefore contained data from N = 885 individuals, of which 771 (87.1%) provided complete demographic and CAPE-42 data, 109 (12.3%) had no more than five missing values on the set of CAPE frequency variables for analysis, and five individuals (0.6%) had more than five but less than 25% missing data points.

Measures

The Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences is a 42-item self-report instrument that measures the lifetime frequencies and distress of positive, negative and depressive subclinical psychotic experiences [63]. CAPE-42 has proven to be an appropriate tool to assess these experiences in clinical and subclinical populations [25], and has been cross-validated with the Structured Interview for Schizotypy (SIS-R) and the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS), demonstrating good stability, reliability and (discriminant) validity [49]. The current study used validated Dutch and English versions of the instrument [64, 65], and a validated Norwegian translation [66]. The Norwegian [66] and English validation [65] of CAPE-42 suggested sufficient internal consistency of positive, negative and depressive experiences, with Cronbach’s alpha values of 0.87, 0.87, 0.82 (Norway), and 0.83, 0.81, 0.84 (Canada), respectively. The Dutch version of CAPE-42 showed acceptable 3-factor model fit (positive, negative, depressive experiences) with RMSEA = 0.05 and TLI = 0.94, though not fully supported by CFI = 0.81 [64].

The positive symptom dimension (hereafter: CAPE-Pos) that is more closely examined in the current study consists of scores on 20 items, asking participants about subclinical positive psychotic experiences (e.g. “Do you ever hear voices when you are alone?”, “Do you ever feel as if you are under the control of some power other than yourself?”, “Do you ever feel as if there is a conspiracy against you?”). The frequency of each experience is rated on a four-point scale, ranging from 1 (never), 2 (sometimes), 3 (often), to 4 (nearly always), and a four-point scale from 1 (not distressed), 2 (a bit distressed), 3 (quite distressed) to 4 (very distressed) measures the degree of associated distress for each experience that is labelled as present (frequency of at least sometimes). The CAPE-Pos scale was initially conceptualized as unidimensional [67], but later studies have suggested either a three-dimensional structure that distinguishes ‘hallucinations’ (4 items), ‘strange experiences’ (7 items), and ‘delusional ideations’ (9 items; [25]), or a five-dimensional structure, in which the delusional ideation dimension is further decomposed into the subdimensions ‘paranoia’ (5 items), ‘grandiosity’ (2 items), and ‘magical thinking’ (2 items; [26]).

Analysis

Data were analysed with R version 3.5.3 [68], using the psych (version 1.8.12 [69]) and lavaan packages (version 0.6–3 [70]), and SPSS Statistics version 25.0 [71]. Data were inspected for anomalies, but outliers were not removed, as extreme scores were argued to indicate actual severe psychotic experiences.

Demographic variability in frequency and distress of CAPE-42 positive psychotic experiences was preliminary explored using multiple regression analyses with weighted mean frequency and distress scores for all CAPE-Pos items as dependent variables and age, gender (1 = male; 2 = female), marital status (1 = not married; 2 = married), and educational level (1 = secondary education; 2 = tertiary education) as independent variables.

Tests for configural, metric, and scalar invariance were performed to investigate the degree of measurement invariance for CAPE-Pos frequency ratings across groups, with (partial) invariance being considered as prerequisite for validly interpreting mean comparisons between samples as reflecting differences in the latent construct or trait rather than the measure [72,73,74]. In line with previous work [64], measurement invariance testing for the distress ratings was omitted, as these values were selectively available only for those experiences labelled as present (frequency of at least sometimes).

Configural invariance was assessed by performing maximum likelihood confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to investigate the fit of one-, three-, and five-dimensional models of CAPE-Pos [25, 26, 67] in the whole sample, and in the Dutch, Nigerian, and Norwegian samples separately. Comparative fit index (CFI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) were computed for absolute goodness of fit exploration (minimally acceptable fit criteria: CFI ≥ 0.90, [75]; RMSEA ≤ 0.08, [76]; SRMR ≤ 0.08, [77]), and Akaike information criterion (AIC) and sample size adjusted Bayesian information criterion (BIC) were used to compare relative goodness of fit (smaller values indicate better fit). Internal consistency for the initial CFA-supported CAPE-Pos dimensions was assessed in each sample using McDonald’s ω [78], and inter-item correlations (IIC) for dimensions consisting of two items.

Configural, metric and scalar invariance of CAPE-Pos scores across Dutch, Nigerian, and Norwegian samples were further assessed by performing maximum likelihood multi-group confirmatory factor analysis (MGCFA [79]), in which several invariance models were compared sequentially, each model adding more restrictions and being tested against the less constrained model [73, 74, 80]. Specification of the baseline configural model was based on CFA results. Metric invariance was tested by constraining factor loadings, and comparing fit to the baseline model, in which loadings were allowed to be freely estimated across groups. The scalar invariance model additionally constrained item intercepts, and was tested against the metric invariance model. χ2 difference tests and ΔCFI were used to examine differences between the nested models, with ΔCFI < 0.01 implying that the invariance assumption holds [81]. When full invariance was not supported, modification indices were explored to identify non-invariant items, and constraints for these items were lifted, thus allowing partial invariance [72, 82].

When at least partial scalar invariance was supported, multivariate analyses of covariance (MANCOVAs) were performed to test for mean differences in the frequency and associated distress of CAPE-Pos experiences between Dutch, Nigerian and Norwegian participants. In order to do so, for those CAPE-Pos dimensions meeting criteria for (partial) measurement invariance, raw scores on CAPE-Pos frequency and distress items were transformed into weighted means (i.e. sum of item scores divided by the number of items with valid responses). Weighted means for experience related distress were only calculated when at least one positive psychotic experience within the respective CAPE-Pos dimension was labelled as present (frequency of at least sometimes). Covariates in the MANCOVA models were age, gender, marital status, and educational level. Whenever the MANCOVA tests yielded significant results, pairwise comparisons of estimated marginal means were conducted to explore group differences with Bonferroni corrected statistics, and Hedges’ g values were calculated by dividing mean differences between groups by the pooled and weighted standard deviation [83].

All findings were interpreted against a significance threshold of p <  0.05, and the respective n for each analysis is presented along with model estimates. (MG)CFAs were performed with full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation to account for missing data points [70, 84], but were additionally repeated using listwise deletion (results presented in Appendix) to ensure that findings were not systematically biased by missing data processing.

Results

Sample characteristics and demographic variability in CAPE-Pos frequency and distress

Table 1 presents a demographic description of the Dutch, Nigerian, and Norwegian samples, together with mean frequency and distress scores for CAPE-Pos. Dutch participants were oldest, most often married, and had generally completed tertiary educational level. By contrast, Nigerian participants were youngest, generally not married, and had typically not (yet) completed the tertiary educational level. Women were overrepresented in the Dutch and Norwegian samples, whereas in the Nigerian sample male and female participants were represented equally. Among the Nigerian participants, 68.4, 16.7, and 1.7% considered themselves as belonging to the Yoruba, Igbo, or Hausa ethnic group, respectively.

In the total sample, preliminary multiple regression analysis revealed that CAPE-Pos frequency scores were significantly associated with age, gender, and educational level (F(4,871) = 49.37, p <  0.001; R2 = 0.185): higher CAPE-Pos frequency was linked to younger (vs. older; B(SE) = − 0.04(0.005), p <  0.001), male (vs. female; B(SE) = − 0.201(0.030), p <  0.001) and lower (vs. higher; B(SE) = − 0.076(0.031), p = 0.014) educated participants. CAPE-Pos distress scores were significantly associated with age and gender (F(4,870) = 15.98, p <  0.001; R2 = 0.069): higher CAPE-Pos distress was linked to younger (vs. older; B(SE) = − 0.025(0.005), p <  0.001), and male (vs. female; B(SE) = − 0.106(0.029), p <  0.001) participants.

Confirmatory factor analysis and internal consistency estimates

Fit indices for one-, three-, and five-dimensional models of CAPE-Pos are presented in Table 2. In the total sample, only the five-dimensional model fulfilled all three fit criteria for CFI, SRMR and RMSEA. In the Dutch and Nigerian samples, the five-dimensional model best fit the data: AIC and BIC values were lower compared to those of the three- and one-dimensional models, SRMR and RMSEA reached threshold values, and CFIs were 0.845 (Dutch sample) and 0.902 (Nigerian sample). In the Norwegian sample, none of the tested models reached threshold values for CFI, SRMR and RMSEA, indicating poor or suboptimal fit of previously suggested CAPE-Pos factor structures in the Norwegian sample. Thus, CFA largely supported five-factor structures of CAPE-Pos in the Dutch and Nigerian samples, but not in the Norwegian sample. Repeating CFAs using listwise deletion instead of FIML returned slightly lower CFI-values, but yielded similar results overall (see Table 7 in Appendix).

Table 2 Confirmatory Factor Analysis for the total sample, and for the three study samples separately

Internal consistency estimates for the five CAPE-Pos dimensions–‘hallucinations’, ‘strange experiences’, ‘paranoia’, ‘grandiosity’, and ‘magical thinking’–that were largely supported by CFA in the Dutch and Nigerian samples were, respectively, ω = 0.53, ω = 0.74, ω = 0.30, IIC = 0.58, IIC = 0.40 in the Dutch sample, ω = 0.75, ω = 0.82, ω = 0.63, IIC = 0.36, IIC = 0.09 in the Nigerian sample, and ω = 0.69, ω = 0.68, ω = 0.59, IIC = 0.38, IIC = 0.22 in the Norwegian sample.

Metric and scalar invariance

Fit statistics for different invariance models of CAPE-Pos across study samples are presented in Table 3. In line with CFA results, the five-dimensional configural invariance (baseline) model did not fit the data well, and assumptions of metric and scalar invariance were violated. Modification indices were explored to identify non-invariant items, and it was concluded that only those items assigned to the dimensions ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’ were (largely) invariant across samples. The configural (baseline) model was respecified accordingly, yielding improved fit, and full metric and partial scalar invariance were established across study samples for the redefined, reduced model (see Table 3 for details). Repeating MGCFA using listwise deletion instead of FIML returned slightly lower fit values for all models, but provided similar results regarding metric and scalar invariance between samples (see Table 8 in Appendix).

Table 3 Fit statistics for different invariance models of CAPE-42 positive psychotic experiences across study samples

Mean differences in CAPE-pos frequency and associated distress between groups

Based on results from MGCFA, tests for mean differences in the frequency and associated distress of CAPE-Pos experiences between samples were limited to the CAPE-Pos dimensions ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’. In nearly all MANCOVA models, the assumption of homogeneity of variance was violated (Levene’s tests were significant), and bootstrapping was used to correct the confidence interval for bias. Samples differed significantly with regard to the mean frequency of ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’ (Wilk’s Λ = 0.748, F(4, 1736) = 67.74, p <  0.001; see Table 5). Post-hoc analyses showed that estimated marginal means for the frequency of ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’ were significantly higher in the Nigerian sample (all comparisons p <  0.001), compared to the Dutch and Norwegian samples, which were similar (see Tables 4 and 6).

Table 4 Estimated marginal means for frequency and distress of CAPE-42 strange experiences and paranoia

The multivariate model of CAPE-Pos distress associated with ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’ also revealed significant differences between samples, Wilk’s Λ = 0.869, F(4, 1086) = 19.81, p <  0.001 (See Table 5). Post-hoc comparison of estimated marginal means showed that distress associated with ‘strange experiences’ was significantly higher in the Nigerian sample (all comparisons p <  0.001), compared to the Dutch and Norwegian samples, which were similar. Distress associated with ‘paranoia’ was similar in Dutch and Nigerian participants, and significantly higher compared to Norwegian participants (see Tables 4 and 6).

Table 5 Between-group analysis of frequency and associated distress of CAPE-42 strange experiences and paranoia
Table 6 Pairwise comparisons for frequency and associated distress of CAPE-42 strange experiences and paranoia between samples

Discussion

This study examined cultural variation in manifestations of positive psychotic experiences as measured by CAPE-42 in student populations from universities in the Netherlands, Nigeria (Yoruba homeland) and Norway. Findings showed that only those items of CAPE-Pos pertaining to the dimensions ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’ (largely) met assumptions for measurement invariance across samples, whereas those that were labelled as belonging to dimensions of ‘hallucinations’, ‘grandiosity’, and ‘magical thinking’ in previous studies did not. For those positive psychotic experiences that could be validly compared between samples (i.e., ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’), frequencies were higher in the Nigerian sample compared to the Dutch and Norwegian samples, which were similar in this regard, as hypothesized. However, contrary to expectations, Nigerian participants reported comparable or even higher levels of psychotic experience related distress than did Dutch and Norwegian participants, not fitting the notion of psychotic phenomena as constituting a relatively more normative and hence less distressing aspect of human experience in Nigerian compared to Dutch and Norwegian culture.

Our findings suggest, first of all, that caution may be warranted when comparing frequency and distress of CAPE-Pos experiences between general population samples from different countries using available translations of CAPE-42, as observed differences may partly reflect differences in the measure rather than in the latent construct(s). Measurement non-invariance may be due to a variety of differences between samples, e.g. differences in the applicability of items, the conceptual meaning or understanding of constructs, the extent of social desirability, the nature of personal reference points and extreme item responses, item translations, or due to different administration methods [79]. Previous work [64] has suggested that individuals with psychosis vulnerability may show a tendency of scoring slightly lower on CAPE-42 when the measure is administered online versus on paper, but concluded this to be of negligible consequence for research in general population samples. Nonetheless, considering the CAPE-42 questionnaire was assessed online by Dutch and Norwegian participants, but on paper by Nigerian participants, differences in the administration method may have represented a source of measurement non-invariance. Moreover, CAPE-42 was developed in the Netherlands and it cannot be ruled out that Dutch values, beliefs, and attitudes have influenced the idiom of the questions [22], and may have been understood differently by Norwegian and Nigerian participants presented with translated versions of the instrument. Assessment tools may not always be applicable cross-culturally [85], and our findings may imply that further cross-cultural adaptation of CAPE-42 for non-Western samples may thus be necessary [86] (see also ITC Guidelines for Translating and Adapting Tests [87]).

Despite differences in the measurement of CAPE-42 positive psychotic experiences across cultural settings, similarities were observed for the measurement of ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’, of which the observed higher frequency in Nigerian participants aligns with previous work showing higher psychotic symptom prevalence of non-Western compared to Western societies [13, 22, 33, 42, 53, 54]. As suggested previously [16, 50], a more pliable way of differentiating between reality and fantasy in non-Western societies–expressed, for example, through recognition of a supernatural or divine realm–may give rise to more frequent endorsement of psychotic-like experiences, such as imagery, or altered states of consciousness, than in Euro-American societies, in which distinctions between what is real and what is not tend to be more rigid [50]. Experiences such as communicating with a deceased family member or feeling the presence of a supernatural force may in fact be highly valued and culturally meaningful in non-Western societies [88], and may therefore be more frequently noticed and more easily communicated to others [50]. A higher prevalence of positive psychotic symptoms in African societies may thus, in part, reflect a greater tendency to share culturally sanctioned experiences that signify contact with the supernatural realm or spirit world [38, 50, 60]. In Western societies, on the other hand, negative attitudes towards experiences that could signal dissociation from what is sensible or physically perceivable (i.e., what is considered as ‘real’), may reduce the tendency to report such experiences, out of fear for stigma of mental illness [50].

However, while the Nigerian participants included in our study reported a higher frequency of positive psychotic experiences, they also reported equal or even higher levels of distress due to these experiences compared to Dutch and Norwegian participants, respectively. This does not seem to fit well with the idea of psychotic-like experiences representing a more normative and benign, and hence less distressing aspect of human experience in non-Western societies [16, 20, 22, 50]. Rather, this finding suggests that the experience of psychotic phenomena may be just as, if not more, distressing in African than in European culture. This contrasts with previous reports, describing for instance that hallucinations are considered less troublesome and in general are experienced positively in African culture [20], in addition to suggesting more sympathetic attitudes within the social environment, a lower demand for clinical support, a more transient nature, and favourable course and outcome of psychosis in non-Western societies [85, 89,90,91,92,93], although some studies suggest that symptom severity rather than associated distress is linked to need for care [94]. However, other work suggests that prevalence-distress associations for psychotic symptoms do not necessarily differ across cultural groups [95], and certain culture-specific spiritual or supernatural interpretations of psychotic-like experiences, such as beliefs of possession by higher order malevolent entities (typically: devils, demons) as described for certain social groups in Africa, may actually induce severe distress [96, 97]. Reversely, the experience of psychological distress may be expressed or communicated differently across cultures [98, 99], and the endorsement of psychotic-like symptoms in Africans may in fact reflect a culturally sanctioned expression of distress, in line with the observation of higher rates of brief reactive psychoses following stressful events in non-Western compared to Western cultures [100].

Because the current study did not tap directly into the belief system of participants, it remains unclear exactly in which way and to what extent culturally based beliefs drive the findings on psychotic symptom frequency and distress. Several studies have suggested, for instance, that while on the one hand Nigerians are in general acculturated in the sense that they support attempts to emulate western economic and social developments, they are on the other hand very protective of traditional cultural values and norms [101, 102]. This may explain why caregivers of schizophrenia patients in Nigeria endorse both supernatural (‘traditional’), as well as natural (‘acculturated’) causes as important in the aetiology of the disorder [103], and why Nigerian patients with schizophrenia seek help from both spiritual healers and physicians [104]. Nigerians may thus represent a relatively heterogeneous group in terms of beliefs and attitudes regarding psychotic experiences, with the possibility of contrasting or bicultural belief systems at play. Educational level may play a role, as supernatural beliefs about disease causation are less commonly endorsed by individuals with formal education [103]. This may well be relevant for Nigerian participants in the current study, given that all had completed secondary or tertiary education. Moreover, respondents were psychology students with expectedly higher tendency towards medical and scientific explanations for their experiences, making them more aware of possible psychopathology.

Apart from cultural factors, the participant samples included in the current study may have differed in terms of demographic and exposure to environmental risk factors for psychosis. The Nigerian sample, for instance, consisted of proportionally more men, was significantly younger, lower educated, and less often married than the Dutch and Norwegian samples. Therefore, although statistical analyses were corrected for demographic variability, it cannot be ascertained that between-group differences do not in part reflect different demographic risk profiles for psychosis [8]. In addition, previously identified environmental risk factors for psychosis, such as childhood trauma, cannabis use, and urbanicity may have differed between samples but were not assessed [44]. Whereas lifetime cannabis use is likely more common in Norway, and particularly in The Netherlands, compared to Nigeria (see e.g. [105]), reports of childhood abuse may be more commonly reported by African patients with a psychiatric diagnosis compared to patients from Europe [106, 107]. With regard to urbanicity, it is important to recognize that Nigerian participants were from the city of Lagos, which is considered the fastest growing and largest city of Africa, with an estimated population of over 20 million residents [108]. Moreover, the city has been ranked among the least liveable cities in the world, due to issues regarding political stability, security, healthcare, education, and infrastructure [109]. Urban residency was previously shown to be associated with delusion-like experiences in young Ugandan adults, regardless of age, gender and social class [52]. Thus, although speculative, between-group differences in reported rates of psychotic experiences and associated distress may be partly attributable to differences in exposure to environmental risk rather than differences in cultural background.

Although reported frequencies of positive psychotic experiences were comparable between Dutch and Norwegian participants, our data suggest that experiences of paranoia caused less distress for Norwegian than for Dutch students. Although this finding agrees with previous work showing that psychological distress in general tends to be lower in Norwegian students compared to students from other Western countries [110], our study is the first to compare psychosis proneness-related distress between Dutch and Norwegian adults, and further investigations are required to test the robustness of this result, and elucidate possible involvement of cultural factors.

Strengths and limitations

This study adds to the existing literature on subclinical psychotic experiences in community-based samples, by assessing measurement invariance and differences in frequency and distress of CAPE-42 positive psychotic experiences across student samples from different cultural settings. In particular, the current study responds to the dearth of CAPE-42 data for non-Western study samples (see [25] for recent overview), by exploring CAPE-42 positive psychotic experiences in African students. Although the current study thereby provides new insights in the extended psychosis phenotype across sociocultural groups, a number of limitations require consideration when interpreting results, and deserve attention in future studies.

First, as pointed out above, the Nigerian study sample differed significantly from the Dutch and Norwegian samples regarding demographic characteristics, and possibly also regarding exposure to environmental (e.g. trauma, cannabis use, urbanicity [44]) and other risk factors for psychosis, such as co-occurring phenomena like anxiety and depression [111, 112]. Therefore, although findings were statistically adjusted for demographic characteristics, caution is warranted when attributing between-group differences to cultural factors. Second, higher educated individuals were overrepresented in all samples, and the Dutch and Norwegian samples consisted largely of women. In addition, recruitment procedures may have led to selective underrepresentation of certain groups in our study sample, and results thus possibly lack accuracy regarding generalization to the population level (see e.g. [113, 114] for potential limitations of non-probability sampling). Third, although we assumed similarities between Dutch and Norwegian participants with respect to their cultural belief system in context of distinctions between reality and fantasy, they cannot be interpreted as culturally equal. Fourth, the current study did not distinguish between self-reported ethnic group membership of Nigerian participants. Fifth, (partial) measurement invariance of CAPE-Pos across samples was only established for items pertaining to ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’, thus limiting between-group comparisons to these dimensions of CAPE-Pos. In addition, because reports of experience related distress were selectively available–only for those experiences labelled as present–measurement invariance testing for the distress dimension was omitted, in line with previous work [64]. As discussed above, differences in administration, language, and interpretation of CAPE-42 between samples all represent potential sources of measurement non-invariance in our study, and subject to further investigation in future cross-cultural studies using CAPE-42. Sixth, internal consistency of CAPE-Pos subdimensions supported by CFA varied from ω = 0.30 to ω = 0.82, and findings must be interpreted in this context. Seventh, the current study did not include an infrequency scale (e.g. [115]) or other measures for identifying inconsistent survey responders, thus the possibility of dishonesty, survey fatigue, or other response biases affecting our data cannot be ruled out. Eighth and lastly, this study focused on the positive dimension of psychotic experiences, because it was considered most sensitive to cross-cultural differences in belief systems [17, 50]. However, recent work by [116] suggested a multidimensional nature of the extended psychosis phenotype in which positive and negative psychotic experiences, disorganization, mania and depression complement a general transdiagnostic psychosis factor (i.e., a factor that is relevant across a range of mental disorders). Future cross-cultural studies may thus consider a more comprehensive assessment of the extended psychosis phenotype that takes this multidimensionality into account.

Conclusion

Our findings show cultural variation in the extended psychosis phenotype, and support previous work suggesting that culture may profoundly affect various dimensions of psychometric measures of psychosis proneness [16]. The results emphasize the importance of investigating cross-cultural variants in symptom definition, and behavioural and symptomatic manifestations of psychosis [117]. Awareness of the sociocultural context in which psychotic experiences occur is required in order to adequately interpret these experiences and respond appropriately [12, 14, 15, 118,119,120]. Prevention, screening, identification, and treatment of psychosis in non-Western cultural settings may require involvement of and collaboration between traditional healers and health professionals, as already operationalized in certain African countries for HIV/AIDS and related illnesses [121,122,123].

Future studies are advised to further investigate and minimize sources of measurement non-invariance, explicitly examine cultural acceptance towards psychotic phenomena, and assess environmental and other known risk factors for psychosis when comparing and interpreting subclinical psychotic phenomena across different cultural groups.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

AIC:

Akaike information criterion

BIC:

Bayesian information criterion

BPRS:

Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale

CAPE-42:

Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences

CAPE-Pos :

CAPE positive symptom dimension

CFA:

Confirmatory factor analysis

CFI:

Comparative fit index

DSM-(I)V:

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV / V

FIML:

Full information maximum likelihood

ICD-10:

International Classification of Diseases-10

MANCOVA:

Multivariate analysis of covariance

MGCFA:

Multi-group confirmatory factor analysis

RMSEA:

Root mean square error of approximation

SIS-R:

Structured Interview for Schizotypy

SPSS:

Statistical Package for the Social Sciences

SRMR:

Standardized root mean square residual

References

  1. Johns LC, Van Os J. The continuity of psychotic experiences in the general population. Clin Psychol Rev. 2001. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00103-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. van Os J, Hanssen M, Bijl RV, Ravelli A. Strauss (1969) revisited: a psychosis continuum in the general population? Schizophr Res. 2000. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00224-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Ayuso-Mateos JL. Global burden of schizophrenia in the year 2000: version 1 estimates: WHO; 2006. https://www.who.int/healthinfo/statistics/bod_schizophrenia.pdf. Accessed 18 May 2019

  4. Barrio C, Yamada A, Atuel H, Hough RL, Yee S, Berthot B, et al. A tri-ethnic examination of symptom expression on the positive and negative syndrome scale in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr Res. 2003. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00223-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bhugra D. The global prevalence of schizophrenia. PLoS Med. 2005. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0020151.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Charlson FJ, Ferrari AJ, Santomauro DF, Diminic S, Stockings E, Scott JG, et al. Global epidemiology and burden of schizophrenia: findings from the global burden of disease study 2016. Schizophr Bull. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sby058.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Jablensky A, Sartorius N, Ernberg G, Anker M, Korten A, Cooper JE, et al. Schizophrenia: manifestations, incidence and course in different cultures a World Health Organization ten-country study. Psychol Med Monogr Suppl. 1992. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0264180100000904.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Messias EL, Chen CY, Eaton WW. Epidemiology of schizophrenia: review of findings and myths. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2007.04.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. McLean D, Thara R, John S, Barrett R, Loa P, McGrath J, et al. DSM-IV ‘criterion a’ schizophrenia symptoms across ethnically different populations: evidence for differing psychotic symptom content or structural organization? Cult Med Psychiatry. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11013-014-9385-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Vega WA, Lewis-Fernandez R. Ethnicity and variability of psychotic symptoms. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2008. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-008-0037-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Stompe T, Karakula H, Rudaleviciene P, Okribelashvili N, Chaudhry HR, Idemudia EE, et al. The pathoplastic effect of culture on psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia. World Cult Psychiatry Res Rev. 2006;1(3/4):157–63.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Lim C, Chong SA, Keefe RS. Psychosocial factors in the neurobiology of schizophrenia: a selective review. Ann Acad Med Singap. 2009;38(5):402–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Bauer SM, Schanda H, Karakula H, Olajossy-Hilkesberger L, Rudaleviciene P, Okribelashvili N, et al. Culture and the prevalence of hallucinations in schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry. 2011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.06.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Cheng A. Case definition and culture: are people all the same? Br J Psychiatry. 2001. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.179.1.1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kirmayer LJ, Ryder AG. Culture and psychopathology. Curr Opin Psychol. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Laroi F, Luhrmann T, Bell V, Christian W, Deshpande S, Fernyhough C, et al. Culture and hallucinations: overview and future directions. Schizophr Bull. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbu012.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Suhail K, Cochrane R. Effect of culture and environment on the phenomenology of delusions and hallucinations. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2002. https://doi.org/10.1177/002076402128783181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Olugbile O, Zachariah MP, Kuyinu A, Coker A, Ojo O, Isichei B. Yoruba world view and the nature of psychotic illness. Afr J Psychiatry. 2009. https://doi.org/10.4314/ajpsy.v12i2.43733.

  19. Campbell M, Sibeko G, Mall S, Baldinger A, Nagdee M, Susser E, et al. The content of delusions in a sample of south African Xhosa people with schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatry. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1196-3.

  20. Luhrmann TM, Padmavati R, Tharoor H, Osei A. Hearing voices in different cultures: a social kindling hypothesis. Top Cogn Sci. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Chadwick P, Birchwood M. The omnipotence of voices: a cognitive approach to auditory hallucinations. Br J Psychiatry. 1994. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.164.2.190.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Fonseca-Pedrero E, Chan RC, Debanne M, Cicero D, Zhang LC, Brenner C, et al. Comparisons of schizotypal traits across 12 countries: results from the international consortium for Schizotypy research. Schizophr Res. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2018.03.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Kalra G, Bhugra D, Shah N. Cultural aspects of schizophrenia. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2012. https://doi.org/10.3109/09540261.2012.708649.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Krabbendam L, Myin-Germeys I, Bak M, Van Os J. Explaining transitions over the hypothesized psychosis continuum. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2005. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1614.2005.01541.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Mark W, Toulopoulou T. Psychometric properties of ‘community assessment of psychic experiences’: review and meta-analyses. Schizophr Bull. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbv088.

  26. Schlier B, Jaya ES, Moritz S, Lincoln TM. The community assessment of psychic experiences measures nine clusters of psychosis-like experiences: a validation of the German version of the CAPE. Schizophr Res. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.10.034.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Therman S, Ziermans TB. Confirmatory factor analysis of psychotic-like experiences in a general population sample. Psychiatry Res. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.12.023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Wigman JT, Vollebergh WA, Jacobs N, Wichers M, Derom C, Thiery E, et al. Replication of the five-dimensional structure of positive psychotic experiences in young adulthood. Psychiatry Res. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.09.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Wigman JT, Vollebergh WA, Raaijmakers QAW, Iedema J, van Dorsselaer S, Ormel J, et al. The structure of the extended psychosis phenotype in early adolescence-a cross-sample replication. Schizophr Bull. 2011. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbp154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Yung AR, Nelson B, Baker K, Buckby JA, Baksheev G, Cosgrave EM. Psychotic-like experiences in a community sample of adolescents: implications for the continuum model of psychosis and prediction of schizophrenia. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1080/00048670802607188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Ziermans T. Working memory capacity and psychotic-like experiences in a general population sample of adolescents and young adults. Front Psychiatry. 2013. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00161.

  32. Katz MM, Marsella A, Dube KC, Olatawura M, Takahashi R, Nakane Y, et al. On the expression of psychosis in different cultures: schizophrenia in an Indian and in a Nigerian community. Cult Med Psychiatry. 1988. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00051973.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Nuevo R, Chatterji S, Verdes E, Naidoo N, Arango C, Ayuso-Mateos J. The continuum of psychotic symptoms in the general population: a cross-national study. Schizophr Bull. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbq099.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Sartorius N, Jablensky A, Korten A, Ernberg G, Anker M, Cooper JE, et al. Early manifestations and first-contact incidence of schizophrenia in different cultures: a preliminary report on the initial evaluation phase of the WHO collaborative study on determinants of outcome of severe mental disorders. Psychol Med. 1986. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291700011910.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Sartorius N, Shapiro R, Kimura M, Barrett, K. WHO international pilot study of schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1972; doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291700045244.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. International Pilot Study of Schizophrenia, World Health Organization. Report of the International Pilot Study of Schizophrenia. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1973. p. 427. Available from: http://www.who.int/iris/handle/10665/39405. [cited 2019 May 22]

    Google Scholar 

  37. Johns LC, Nazroo JY, Bebbington P, Kuipers E. Occurrence of hallucinatory experiences in a community sample and ethnic variations. Br J Psychiatry. 2002. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.180.2.174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. King M, Nazroo J, Weich S, McKenzie K, Bhui K, Karlson S, et al. Psychotic symptoms in the general population of England. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2005. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-005-0900-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Werbeloff N, Dohrenwend B, Yoffe R, van Os J, Davidson M, Weiser M. The association between negative symptoms, psychotic experiences and later schizophrenia: a population-based longitudinal study. PLoS One. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119852.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Heuvelman H, Nazroo J, Rai D. Investigating ethnic variations in reporting of psychotic symptoms: a multiple-group confirmatory factor analysis of the psychosis screening questionnaire. Psychol Med. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718000399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Schwartz RC, Blankenship DM. Racial disparities in psychotic disorder diagnosis: a review of empirical literature. World J Psychiatry. 2014. https://doi.org/10.5498/wjp.v4.i4.133.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Wüsten C, Schlier B, Jaya ES, Fonseca-Pedrero E, Peters E, Verdoux H, et al. Psychotic experiences and related distress: a cross-national comparison and network analysis based on 7141 participants from 13 countries. Schizophr Bull. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sby087.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Viswanath B, Chaturvedi SK. Cultural aspects of major mental disorders: a critical review from an Indian perspective. Indian J Psychol Med. 2012. https://doi.org/10.4103/0253-7176.108193.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. van Os J, Kenis G, Rutten BP. The environment and schizophrenia. Nature. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09563.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Veling W, Susser E. Migration and psychotic disorders. Expert Rev Neurother. 2011. https://doi.org/10.1586/ERN.10.91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Morgan C, Charalambides M, Hutchinson G, Murray R. Migration, ethnicity, and psychosis: toward a sociodevelopmental model. Schizophr Bull. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbq051.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Veling W, Susser E, Selten JP, Hoek HW. Social disorganization of neighborhoods and incidence of psychotic disorders: a 7-year first-contact incidence study. Psychol Med. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714002682.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Pull CB, Cloos JM, Pull-Erpelding MC. Clinical assessment instruments in psychiatry. In: Maj M, Gaebel MW, Lopez-Ibor JJ, Sartorius N, editors. Psychiatric diagnosis and classification: Wiley; 2002. p. 177–218. https://doi.org/10.1002/047084647X.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Konings M, Bak M, Hanssen M, van Os J, Krabbendam L. Validity and reliability of the CAPE: a self-report instrument for the measurement of psychotic experiences in the general population. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2006. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00741.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Al-Issa I. The illusion of reality or the reality of illusion: hallucinations and culture. Br J Psychiatry. 1995. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.166.3.368.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Gureje O, Olowosegun O, Adebayo K, Stein DJ. The prevalence and profile of non-affective psychosis in the Nigerian survey of mental health and wellbeing. World J Psychiatry. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2051-5545.2010.tb00269.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Lundberg P, Cantor-Graae E, Rukundo G, Ashaba S, Ostergren P. Urbanicity of place of birth and symptoms of psychosis, depression and anxiety in Uganda. Br J Psychiatry. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051953.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Murphy HB, Wittkower ED, Fried J, Ellenberger H. A cross-cultural survey of schizophrenic symptomatology. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 1963. https://doi.org/10.1177/002076406300900401.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Ndetei DM, Vadher A. A comparative cross-cultural study of the frequencies of hallucination in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1984. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1984.tb01247.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Novick D, Haro JM, Hong J, Brugnoli R, Lepine JP, Bertsch J, et al. Regional differences in treatment response and three year course of schizophrenia across the world. J Psychiatr Res. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.03.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Saha S, Chant DC, Welham JL, McGrath JJ. The incidence and prevalence of schizophrenia varies with latitude. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2006. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00742.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Jongsma HE, Turner C, Kirkbride JB, Jones PB. International incidence of psychotic disorders, 2002–17: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(19)30056-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Wittchen HU, Jacobi F. Size and burden of mental disorders in Europe-a critical review and appraisal of 27 studies. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2005. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2005.04.012.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Jenkins JH. Conceptions of schizophrenia as a problem of nerves: a cross-cultural comparison of Mexican-Americans and Anglo-Americans. Soc Sci Med. 1988. https://doi.org/10.1016/0277-9536(88)90155-4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  60. Olfson M, Lewis-Fernandez R, Weissman MM, Feder A, Gameroff MJ, Pilowsky D, et al. Psychotic symptoms in an urban general medicine practice. Am J Psychiatry. 2002. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.8.1412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Boonstra N, Wunderink L, Sytema S, Wiersma D. Improving detection of first episode psychosis by mental health care services using a self report questionnaire. Schizophr Res. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.02.254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Bukenaite A, Stochl J, Mossaheb N, Schafer MR, Klier CM, Becker J, et al. Usefulness of the CAPE-P15 for detecting people at ultra-high risk for psychosis: psychometric properties and cut-off values. Schizophr Res. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.02.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. van Os J, Verdoux H, Hanssen M. CAPE-42 [Internet]. Maastricht: University of Maastricht; 1999. Available from: http://cape42.homestead.com

  64. Vleeschouwer M, Schubart CD, Henquet C, Myin-Germeys I, van Gastel WA, Hillegers MH, et al. Does assessment type matter? A measurement invariance analysis of online and paper and pencil assessment of the community assessment of psychic experiences (CAPE). PLoS One. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084011.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Brenner K, Schmitz N, Pawliuk N, Fathalli F, Joober R, Ciampi A, et al. Validation of the English and French versions of the community assessment of psychic experiences (CAPE) with a Montreal community sample. Schizophr Res. 2007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.06.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Busch AN, Jacobs N. Is there a continuum of psychotic symptoms in the Norwegian population? Validation of the Norwegian translation of the CAPE. Heerlen: Open University of the Netherlands; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Stefanis NC, Hanssen M, Smirnis NK, Avramopoulos DA, Evdokimidis IK, Stefanis CN, et al. Evidence that three dimensions of psychosis have a distribution in the general population. Psychol Med. 2002. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701005141.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing, R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna; 2018. Available from: http://www.R-project.org/. [version 3.5.3; cited 2019 May 09]

  69. Revelle W. Psych: procedures for personality and psychological research. Evanston: Northwestern University; 2018. Available from: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych. [version 1.8.12; cited 2019 May 17]

    Google Scholar 

  70. Rosseel Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02.

  71. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 25.0. IBM Corp: Armonk; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Byrne BM, Shavelson RJ, Muthen BO. Testing for the equivalence of factor covariance and mean structures: the issue of partial measurement invariance. Psychol Bull. 1989. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.105.3.456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Testing factorial invariance across groups: a reconceptualization and proposed new method. J Manage. 1999. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-2063(99)80001-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Hirschfeld G, von Brachel R. Multiple-group confirmatory factor analysis in R-a tutorial in measurement invariance with continuous and ordinal indicators. Prac Assess Res Eval. 2014;19:7.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Medsker GJ, Williams LJ, Holahan PJ. A review of current practices for evaluating causal models in organizational behavior and human resources management research. J Manage. 1994. https://doi.org/10.1016/0149-2063(94)90022-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociol Methods Res. 1992. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124192021002005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cut-off criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis. Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. 1999. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Dunn TJ, Baguley T, Brunsden V. From alpha to omega: a practical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency estimation. Br J Psychol. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12046.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Chen FF. What happens if we compare chopsticks with forks? The impact of making inappropriate comparisons in cross-cultural research. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Byrne BM, van de Vijver FJ. Testing for measurement and structural equivalence in large-scale cross-cultural studies: addressing the issue of nonequivalence. Int J Test. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1080/15305051003637306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct Equ Modeling. 2002. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. van de Schoot R, Lugtig P, Hox J. A checklist for testing measurement invariance. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.686740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. 1st ed. Orlando: Academic press; 2014.

  84. Allison PD. Handling missing data by maximum likelihood. Paper presented at: SAS Global Forum. Orlando: SAS Institute; 2012.

  85. Mamah D, Musau A, Mutiso VN, Owoso A, Abdallah AB, Cottler LB, et al. Characterizing psychosis risk traits in Africa: a longitudinal study of Kenyan adolescents. Schizophr Res. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.08.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Gjersing L, Caplehorn JR, Clausen T. Cross-cultural adaptation of research instruments: language, setting, time and statistical considerations. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-10-13.

  87. ITC guidelines for translating and adapting tests (second edition). Int J Test. 2018; doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/15305058.2017.1398166.

  88. Bentall R, Boyle M, Chadwick P, Cooke A, Garety P, Gelsthorpe P, et al. Understanding psychosis and schizophrenia. Leicester: The British Psychological Society Division of Clinical Psychology; 2017. p. 175.

    Google Scholar 

  89. Guinness EA. Relationship between the neuroses and brief reactive psychosis: descriptive case studies in Africa. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 1992. https://doi.org/10.1192/S0007125000296761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Jilek WG, Jilek-Aall L. Transient psychoses in Africans. Psychopathology. 1970. https://doi.org/10.1159/000278616.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  91. Stevens J. Brief psychoses: do they contribute to the good prognosis and equal prevalence of schizophrenia in developing countries? Br J Psychiatry. 1987. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.151.3.393.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Castillo RJ. Trance, functional psychosis, and culture. Psychiatry. 2003. https://doi.org/10.1521/psyc.66.1.9.20285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Castillo RJ. Effects of culture on recovery from transient psychosis. Psychiatr Times. 2006;23(14):46–8.

    Google Scholar 

  94. Bak M, Myin-Germeys I, Hanssen MSS, Bijl RV, Vollebergh WAM, Delespaul P, et al. When does experience of psychosis result in a need for care? A prospective general population study. Schizophr Bull. 2003. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Cohen CI, Marino L. Racial and ethnic differences in the prevalence of psychotic symptoms in the general population. Psychiatr Serv. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.201200348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. McCarthy-Jones S, Waegeli A, Watkins J. Spirituality and hearing voices: considering the relation. Psychosis. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1080/17522439.2013.831945.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Niehaus DJ, Oosthuizen P, Lochner C, Emsley RA, Jordaan E, Mbanga NI, et al. A culture-bound syndrome ‘amafufunyana’ and a culture-specific event ‘ukuthwasa’: differentiated by a family history of schizophrenia and other psychiatric disorders. Psychopathology. 2004. https://doi.org/10.1159/000077579.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Aina OF, Morakinyo O. Culture-bound syndromes and the neglect of cultural factors in psychopathologies among Africans. Afr J Psychiatry. 2011. https://doi.org/10.4314/ajpsy.v14i4.4.

  99. Kohrt BA, Rasmussen A, Kaiser BN, Haroz EE, Maharjan SM, Mutamba BB, et al. Cultural concepts of distress and psychiatric disorders: literature review and research recommendations for global mental health epidemiology. Int J Epidemiol. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyt227.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  100. Kleinman A. Do psychiatric disorders differ in different cultures? In: Brown JP, Closser S, editors. Understanding and applying medical anthropology: biosocial and cultural approaches. 3rd ed. London and New York: Routledge; 2016. p. 300–10.

    Google Scholar 

  101. Lysonski S, Durvasula S. Nigeria in transition: acculturation to global consumer culture. J Consum Mark. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1108/JCM-07-2013-0626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Gannon M, Pillai R. The Nigerian marketplace. In Understanding global cultures: metaphorical journeys through 29 nations, clusters of nations, continents, and diversity. 4th ed: SAGE Publications, Inc; 2010. p. 311–30. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781452224886.n19.

  103. Igberase O, Okogbenin E. Beliefs about the cause of schizophrenia among caregivers in Midwestern Nigeria. Ment Illn. 2017. https://doi.org/10.4081/mi.2017.6983.

  104. Adeosun II, Adegbohun AA, Adewumi TA, Jeje OO. The pathways to the first contact with mental health services among patients with schizophrenia in Lagos, Nigeria. Schizophr Res Treatment. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/769161.

    Google Scholar 

  105. Hall W, Degenhardt L. Prevalence and correlates of cannabis use in developed and developing countries. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e32812144cc.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Pereda N, Guilera G, Forns M, Gomez-Benito J. The prevalence of child sexual abuse in community and student samples: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.02.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Lochner C, Seedat S, Allgulander C, Kidd M, Stein D, Gerdner A. Childhood trauma in adults with social anxiety disorder and panic disorder: a cross-national study. Afr J Psychiatry (Johannesbg). 2010. https://doi.org/10.4314/ajpsy.v13i5.63103.

  108. Lagos Population. World Population Review. 2019. Available from: http://worldpopulationreview.com/world-cities/lagos-population/. -[cited 2019 May 23]

    Google Scholar 

  109. The Economist Intelligence Unit. The global Liveability report 2017 [internet]. London, New York, Hong Kong: The Economist Intelligence Unit Limited; 2017. p. 10. Available from https://pages.eiu.com/rs/753-RIQ-438/images/Liveability_Free_Summary_2017.pdf. [cited 2018 Nov 10]

    Google Scholar 

  110. Nerdrum P, Rustoen T, Ronnestad MH. Student psychological distress: a psychometric study of 1750 Norwegian 1st-year undergraduate students. Scand J Educ Res. 2006. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313830500372075.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  111. Armando M, Nelson B, Yung AR, Ross M, Birchwood M, Girardi P, et al. Psychotic-like experiences and correlation with distress and depressive symptoms in a community sample of adolescents and young adults. Schizophr Res. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.03.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Fusar-Poli P, Borgwardt S, Bechdolf A, Addington J, Riecher-Rossler A, Schultze-Lutter F, et al. The psychosis high-risk state: a comprehensive state-of-the-art review. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.269.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  113. Lucas SR. Beyond the existence proof: ontological conditions, epistemological implications, and in-depth interview research. Qual Quant. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-012-9775-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Lucas SR. An inconvenient dataset: bias and inappropriate inference with the multilevel model. Qual Quant. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-013-9865-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  115. Huang JL, Bowling NA, Liu M, Li Y. Detecting insufficient effort responding with an infrequency scale: evaluating validity and participant reactions. J Bus Psychol. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-014-9357-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  116. van Os J, Reininghaus U. Psychosis as a transdiagnostic and extended phenotype in the general population. World Psychiatry. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20310.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  117. Alarcon RD. Culture, cultural factors and psychiatric diagnosis: review and projections. World Psychiatry. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2051-5545.2009.tb00233.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  118. Langness LL. Hysterical psychosis: the cross-cultural evidence. Am J Psychiatry. 1967. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.124.2.143.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Bentall RP. Madness explained: psychosis and human nature. London: Penguin UK; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  120. Wykes T, Callard F. Diagnosis, diagnosis, diagnosis: towards DSM-5. J Ment Health. 2010. https://doi.org/10.3109/09638237.2010.494189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Kayombo EJ, Uiso FC, Mbwambo ZH, Mahunnah RL, Moshi MJ, Mgonda YH. Experience of initiating collaboration of traditional healers in managing HIV and AIDS in Tanzania. J Ethnobiol Ethnomed. 2007. https://doi.org/10.1186/1746-4269-3-6.

  122. Burns JK. The social determinants of schizophrenia: an African journey in social epidemiology. Public Health Rev. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391676.

  123. Colvin M, Gumede L, Grimwade K, Maher D, Wilkinson D. Contribution of traditional healers to a rural tuberculosis control programme in Hlabisa, South Africa. Int J Tuberc Lung Dis. 2003;7(9 Suppl 1):S86–91.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Annemarije Busch and Karin Hilbink for their contributions to the data collection in Norway and The Netherlands, respectively.

Funding

No funding to report.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MV designed the study, collected data in the Nigerian sample, performed statistical analysis, interpreted the data and wrote the paper. JL contributed to the analysis, interpretation and writing, and served as mentor and supervisor on all aspects of the project. EA, NJ, and VT facilitated data collection in the different study samples, and JvO, MJ, NJ, and VT made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study. EA, JL, JvO, JR, MJ, NJ, SP, and VT were involved in drafting and revising the manuscript towards a final version, and MJ and VT contributed equally.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Johan Lataster.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The initial study protocol was developed by researchers from Open University in The Netherlands, and was later adapted for use in Norway and Nigeria with permission of the participating institutions. As there is no judicial coverage of ethical review of non-medical research in The Netherlands, faculties of social sciences together developed the ‘code of ethics for research in the social and behavioural sciences’, which was accepted in January 2016 by the deans of social sciences in The Netherlands. From then on, approval of research protocols from an ethics review committee needs to be established prior to the start of a study. The current study was conducted before this date, which made it not obligatory to obtain approval of an IRB/ethics panel. For that reason, the research protocol was not reviewed by such a board, and Dutch ethics committees are prohibited from retroactively granting ethical approval. However, data were collected under responsibility of the researchers, who confirm that the study was carried out in accordance with The Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) for medical research involving humans. It is further emphasized that the Dutch law on Medical Research in Humans (Wet Medisch-wetenschappelijk onderzoek met mensen [WMO]) was not applicable to the current study, because the research question was not medical, did not involve patients, and participants were not randomly assigned to an intervention.

All participants were informed that participation was on a voluntary basis, and gave digital (in case of online survey; The Netherlands, Norway) or verbal informed consent (in case of paper and pencil survey; Nigeria) at study entry.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors confirm that there are no conflicts of interest associated with this publication.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 7 Confirmatory Factor Analysis for the total sample, and for the three study samples separately
Table 8 Fit statistics for different invariance models of CAPE-42 positive psychotic experiences across study samples

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vermeiden, M., Janssens, M., Thewissen, V. et al. Cultural differences in positive psychotic experiences assessed with the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences-42 (CAPE-42): a comparison of student populations in the Netherlands, Nigeria and Norway. BMC Psychiatry 19, 244 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-019-2210-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-019-2210-8

Keywords