- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Childhood trauma and current depression among Chinese university students: a moderated mediation model of cognitive emotion regulation strategies and neuroticism
BMC Psychiatry volume 22, Article number: 90 (2022)
Abstract
Background
Childhood trauma (CT) is considered as a highly risk factor for depression. Although the pathway of CT to depression, especially the mediating or moderating effects of cognitive emotion regulation strategies (CERS) or neuroticism, have investigated by several studies, the results were inconsistent and there is a paucity of full models among these interactive factors. This study aims to examine the relationships among CT, adaptive / maladaptive CERS, neuroticism, and current depression symptoms in university students.
Methods
We recruited 3009 freshman of 2019, aged averagely 18.00 (SD = 0.772) years, from universities in Hunan province in 2019. A moderated mediation model was built to examine the relationships among CT, CERS, neuroticism, and current depression using the SPSS PROCESS 3.5 macro. We conducted bootstrapping of regression estimates with 5000 samples and 95% confidence interval.
Results
Results revealed that the significant mediating effects of adaptive CERS (β = 0.012; 95% CI: 0.006 to 0.018) and maladaptive CERS (β = 0.028; 95% CI: 0.016 to 0.040) between CT and depression were observed, accounting for 5.69% and 13.52% of the total effect respectively. Then, moderated mediation analyses results showed that neuroticism simultaneously moderated the direct effect of CT on current depression (β = 0.035; 95% CI: 0.001 to 0.009), and the indirect effects of CT on current depression through adaptive CERS (adaptive CERS – current depression: β = − 0.034; 95% CI: − 0.007 to − 0.001) and maladaptive CERS (maladaptive CERS – current depression: β = 0.157; 95% CI: 0.017 to 0.025). However, the moderating effects of neuroticism in the indirect paths from CT to adaptive CERS (β = 0.037; 95% CI: 0.000 to 0.014) and maladaptive CERS (β = − 0.001; 95% CI: − 0.006 to 0.005) were not significant.
Conclusions
This study provides powerful evidences through a large university students sample for the mediating role of adaptive / maladaptive CERS and the moderating role of neuroticism between CT and current depression. This manifests that cognitive emotion regulation may be a vital factor for people who suffered from CT and current depression. Furthermore, the influence of neuroticism in this process cannot be ignored.
Background
CT and depression
According to the World Health Organization (WHO), more than one third of the world’s population has experienced childhood trauma (CT). It is estimated that about one in five women and one out of 13 men have suffered from sexual abuse in childhood, and about 25% of adults have undergone physical abuse in childhood. In addition, patients with mental disorders, 28.9% of them reported having experienced CT, and the effects of CT may persist throughout the course of life [1]. The CT was considered as a particularly potent risk factor for the onset, symptomatic severity, and course of depression, which has been demonstrated by various previous studies [2, 3]. Depression is a major human blight, resulting in tremendous personal and social burden. Furthermore, meta-analytic evidence has revealed that experiencing any type of maltreatment may increase more than a two-fold risk for depression in adulthood [4]. The greater childhood adversity is related to more severe depression [5], more chronic depression [6], and a longer time to remission [7].
The Vulnerability-stress model [8] provides theoretical support for the close connection between childhood trauma and depression. In the study of the etiology of depression, researchers have found that an individual suffering from negative life events is prone to depression. However, when facing the same circumstance, individuals react differently: some people would experience depression, while others do not. This phenomenon makes people realize that there are certain variables that determine stress conditions who will be depressed. Researchers have also begun to use vulnerability to study the etiology of depression. Studies have shown that depression may occur after exposure to acute or chronic life stress, especially in people who have experienced childhood trauma exposure [9].
In the large number of existing studies on childhood trauma and depression, we can find that the factors of affecting the relationship between childhood trauma and depression can be divided into two categories: psychosocial and neurobiology. The psychosocial factors include cognition [10, 11], emotion regulation [12], and personality factors [13] etc. Although previous studies have established the basic relationship between childhood trauma and depression, the specific mechanism underlying CT and depression is still unclear.
Psychological vulnerability affecting the relationship between CT and current depression
Cognitive vulnerability
Beck’s Cognitive Theory [14] pointed that cognition is a crucial influence factor in the occurrence and development of depression. Cognitive emotion regulation strategies (CERS) are the cognitive response of an individual consciously or unconsciously trying to change the size and / or type of their own emotional experience and the event itself when faced with an emotion-eliciting events [15]. Garnefski et al. [16] divided CERS into two categories: adaptive and maladaptive. A meta-analysis of emotion regulation strategies shows that compared with other disorders, emotion-related disorders are more closely related to emotion regulation strategies, and compared with adaptive strategies, maladaptive strategies are more closely and more consistent with psychopathology [17]. Cognitive theories put maladaptive appraisal processes at the core of depression and anxiety [18]. Recently, many studies had revealed that higher depression was related to more use of maladaptive CERS, and less use of adaptive CERS (e.g. positive reappraisal) [19, 20]. Furthermore, failure in regulating the emotion has been found to be closely related to the occurrence of psychological problems [21].
In addition, early adverse experiences can lead to the formation of latent depressive cognition schema, which affects the emotions, behaviors and thinking patterns of depressive patients [22]. For instance, physical abuse in childhood are related to schemas of danger and distrust, while experiencing emotional neglect is related to schemas of value and belonging [23]. CT may interfere with the healthy development of emotional regulation. Some researchers believed that acquiring appropriate emotion regulation skills can be disrupted by repeated interpersonal trauma between the caregiver and the child [24, 25]. Individuals who experienced CT, often struggle to understand their emotions, because they haven’t received effective feedback from the environment. Therefore, they tend to use avoidance and reflection when choosing coping strategies, which can easily lead to adverse adaptation [26].
To sum up, CT may influence depression via cognitive emotional regulation. Some existing studies have also investigated the mediated role of cognitive emotional regulation between childhood trauma and subsequent mental health problems. Both clinical sample study [12, 27] and college student sample study [28] provide evidence for that the negative effects of childhood trauma on later depression are mediated by emotion regulation. Furthermore, some other studies on children and adolescents [29, 30] have shown the mediator of emotion regulation between childhood trauma and psychopathology. As we have seen, the role of CERS as a mediator between CT and current depression symptoms have been supported by many findings.
Personality vulnerability
Zuroff proposed the interaction model of personality vulnerability to depression [31], which pointed out that the interaction between individual personality vulnerability and external stress events can affect depression and cause its exacerbation or persistence. Meanwhile, many studies have focused on the influence of personality traits between CT and depression. Lee and Hovens’ researches have shown that the personality traits, in particular neuroticism and extroversion, mediated the relationships between CT and depression in nonclinical or clinical patients [32, 33]. Likewise, a previous study based on longitudinal population-based twin cohort has revealed that compared to extraversion, levels of neuroticism more strongly predicted the risks for both lifetime and new-onset major depression [34]. Another cross-sectional study based on a sample of adults further revealed that only emotional stability (neuroticism) significantly mediated the influence of childhood abuse on depressive symptoms [32]. Among several personality traits, neuroticism plays a particularly prominent role.
However, besides mediated effect, some studies have pointed out that the neuroticism moderated the relationship between childhood trauma and depression. A study of internet addiction mediating the relationship between childhood trauma and depression among Chinese colleges, suggested that neuroticism moderated the direct path of childhood trauma on depression and the indirect path of internet addiction on depression [35]. Ling Yu [36] and Xi Chang et al. [37] found that neuroticism plays a role in moderating the relationship between stress and depression through longitudinal studies and cross-sectional among Chinese adolescents and college students.
In addition, individuals with neurotic personality are more likely to have negative emotions (such as anxiety, depression, etc.) [38] and the neuroticism is closely related to CERS. Some studies have found that neuroticism is negatively correlated with adaptive CERS and positively correlated with maladaptive CERS in adolescents [39] and adults [40]. Furthermore, those who were exposed to CT had higher levels of neuroticism, which further contributed to depressive symptoms in adulthood [32].
The current study
Although the relationship between CT and depression has been repeatedly verified by large number of studies, the mechanism of how CT affects depression is still unclear. Studies have verified the mediating role of CERS in the relationship between CT and depression in depressive patients [27], college students [28], adolescents [41] and other groups. Moreover, in previous studies, the role of neuroticism is somewhat inconsistent — mediator or moderator. Childhood trauma may affect current depressive symptoms through CERS, and what role does neuroticism play in this process? Under what circumstances does neuroticism affect the relationship between childhood trauma and depression?
Epidemiological studies have consistently found the high prevalence of many common mental disorders, especially the mood, anxiety and substance use disorders in university students [42, 43]. And the college years straddle a distinct development period of the adolescent and young adulthood life stages, charactered by sexual maturity, touched a range of educational and occupational opportunities, and substantial instability (changes in romantic status, peer groups, course selection, career choices et al) [44]. Therefore, the college stage represents a unique period. This study intends to integrate cognitive vulnerability (cognitive emotion regulation strategies) and personality vulnerability (neuroticism), in order to explore the psychological mechanism of the impact of CT on current depression in university students. In this way, it helps to clarify the research on the mechanism of childhood trauma affecting depression in university students. Moreover, it can also provide intervention guidance for promoting the mental health of university students and prevent future psychological problems.
As a result, the first hypothesis of this research is: adaptive and maladaptive CERS mediate the relationship between CT and current depression. CT may interact with neuroticism and affect depression. Accordingly, we proposed the second hypothesis: neuroticism may function as a moderator between CT and current depressive symptoms either separately, or both in the direct effect (CT → current depression) and indirect effect (path a1: CT → adaptive CERS, path a2: CT → maladaptive and path b1: adaptive CERS → current depression, path b2: maladaptive CERS → current depression).
We aim to examine the conceptual model (Fig. 1) in current study, in which, first, adaptive CERS and maladaptive CERS mediated the relationship between CT and current depression; second, the direct and indirect relationships between CT and current depression were moderated by neuroticism. Vulnerability and stress are important concepts in the vulnerability-stress model. Recent life stress usually acts as a trigger and activates the vulnerability, which in turn brought about the onset of depression. Thus, in order to clarify the impact of CT on current depression, we controlled the negative life events over the last 12 months as a covariable. Furthermore, we also controlled age, gender in this study. This research can help clarify the psychological mechanism of CT on depression, provide theoretical guidance for clinical intervention, and promote the mental health of university students.
Materials and methods
Participants
We conducted a large sample questionnaire survey for all freshmen (about 3500 students) through convenient sampling in a university in Hunan from October 27th to November 3rd, 2019. All participants were tested during evening self-study time, in exchange for no rewards or course credit. And all of them were given informed consent before completing the measures. Ultimately, 3009 participants (1014 men and 1995 women), mean aged 18 years (SD = 0.772) provided fully completed questionnaires with no missing data, which gave a response rate of 85.97%. The Ethics Committee of the Third Xiangya Hospital of Central South University approved this study.
Measures
Childhood trauma
The Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) [45], including 28 items self-report inventory, was used to evaluate five types of CT experience: emotional abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional neglect and physical neglect. Each item uses a 5-point frequency scale (1 = never to 5 = always). The higher the total score means that the individual may suffer more severe trauma during childhood. So far, CTQ has been introduced in many countries and applied to different groups of people in the world, such as Norwegians in high-risk groups [46], both clinical and nonclinical samples in Swedish [47] and middle school students in China [48]. And all of them have reported a good internal consistency. In this study, the CTQ total score also displayed an acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.662).
Depressive symptoms
The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [49] as a 21-item self-report measure was used to evaluate depression severity. Each item was rated from 0 to 3 and the total scores range from 0 to 63. Furthermore, according to the final scores, depressive symptoms can be categorized into four levels: minimal depression (total score, 0–13); mild depression (total score, 14–19); moderate depression (total score, 20–28); severe depression (total score, 29–63). The Chinese version of BDI-II translated by Yang Wen-hui [50] was adopted in this study. The Chinese versions of the BDI-II have been well validated. In this study, BDI-II (Cronbach’s α = 0.907) also showed good internal consistency.
Adaptive / maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies (CERS)
Cognitive emotion regulation strategies which people tend to choose when they encounter negative life events, were assessed by the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) [16]. This is a 36-item questionnaire, adopting 5-point Likert scale (1 = almost never to 5 = almost always). There are nine subscales in this questionnaire, and each subscale contains four items. Nine subscales represent nine cognitive emotion regulation strategies respectively: rumination, catastrophizing, self-blame, blaming others, putting into perspective, acceptance, positive refocusing, positive reappraisal, and refocusing on planning. The self-blame, rumination, catastrophizing, and blaming others are regarded as maladaptive, whereas acceptance, positive refocusing, refocus on planning, positive reappraisal, and putting into perspective are regarded as adaptive. Moreover, the higher score means that the person uses the strategy more frequently in response to a negative event. CERQ has been validated for different languages and populations, for instance: adolescents aged 14–18 [51] and older aged adults 65–90 [52] in Spanish, university students in Brazilian [53], and adults with recurrent depression [54]. All of them have proved that the CERQ is a valid and reliable tool for assessing cognitive emotion regulation strategies. The Chinese version of CERQ [55] was used in this study. Cronbach’s α for nine subscales were ranged 0.586–0.912, and the full scale’s Cronbach’s α was 0.875 in current research.
Neuroticism
Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) was developed by Eysenck and colleagues, as a measure of personality traits [56]. G,Y.X [57] revised the Chinese version of EPQ in 1992, which consists of 88 items, and included four subscales: extroversion (E), neuroticism (N), psychoticism (P) and lie (L), where neuroticism(N) represented emotional instability and anxiousness. We adopted the Neuroticism Subscale of Eysenck’s Personality Questionnaire (EPQ-N) to assess the neuroticism in present study. EPQ-N consists of 24 items, and each item is responded to using “yes” or “no” (yes = 1, no = 0). Higher scores reflect higher level of neuroticism, which means that individuals are more likely to experience some negative emotions, such as moodiness, worry and anxiety. Cronbach’s α of EPQ-N was 0.895 in this study.
Negative life events
The current study adopted the Adolescent Self-Rating Life Events Check (ASLEC) to assess negative life events during the past 12 months. It contains of the 27 common stressful life events, summarized by the following 6 factors: interpersonal relationship, academic stress, punishment, loss, health adaptation and other factors [58]. The participants first identified if the event had occurred in the past 12 months. If it had, they were required to rate on 5-point scale according to their psychological feelings. (1 = no influence, 2 = mild influence, 3 = moderate influence, 4 = severe influence, and 5 = extremely severe influence). If the event didn’t happen, it’s deemed to have no influence. By calculating the scores of all items, we can get a total life stress score. Higher score indicates higher influence of negative life events. ASLEC was reported a good reliability and validity in many studies [58, 59], the Cronbach’s α in current study was 0.900.
Statistical analyses
Descriptive analyses were conducted by SPSS 23.0. Research variables (CT, depression, adaptive / maladaptive cognitive emotional regulation strategies, neuroticism, negative life events) were analyzed by Pearson correlation analyses. The moderated mediation analysis was conducted by using the SPSS PROCESS 3.5 macro [60], for the purpose of testing the hypothesized model (Fig. 1). Firstly, we tested whether the association between CT and depressive symptoms was mediated by adaptive and maladaptive CERS using Model 4. Next, Model 59 was used to examine the moderated mediation effect. And we chose non-parametric percentile bootstrap method for bias correction. We conduct bootstrapping of regression estimates with 5000 samples and 95% confidence interval. If the confidence interval did not include 0, the effect is considered significant. The grouping conditions were set to mean and mean ± 1 SD [61]. In addition, all models were controlled for covariates (age, sex, negative life events).
Results
Preliminary analysis
The present study recruited 3009 subjects from university students in Hunan Province. In this study sample, a total of 371 (12.33%) participants were found to have depressive symptoms: 214 (7.11%) participants belong to mild depression; 121 (4.02%) participants belong to moderate depression; and 36 (1.20%) participants belong to severe depression. A total of 943 subjects (31.34%) reported experiencing CT. To be more specific, 620 (20.60%) subjects reported experiencing physical neglect; 376 (12.59%) subjects reported experiencing emotional neglect; 126 (4.19%) subjects reported experiencing physical abuse; 121 (4.02%) subjects reported experiencing emotional abuse; 184 (6.11%) subjects reported experiencing sexual abuse. This information is shown in Table 1.
Means, SDs and correlation matrix for several crucial research variables are presented in Table 2. CT was positively correlated with depression (r = 0.339, p < 0.01), neuroticism (r = 0.321, p < 0.01), maladaptive CERS (r = 0.199, p < 0.01). Also, CT was negatively correlated with adaptive CERS(r = − 0.120, p<0.01). Adaptive CERS was not significantly correlated with neuroticism, negative life events, and depression. Maladaptive CERS was positively correlated with depression (r = 0.408, p < 0.01), negative life events(r = 0.359, p<0.01). Neuroticism was positively correlated with depression (r = 0.654, p < 0.01), negative life events (r = 0.470, p < 0.01). As we expected, university students with higher CT had more use of maladaptive CERS and more severe depression in adulthood.
Mediation analyses
Table 3 showed the results of mediation. The total effect of CT on current depression is estimated as c = 0.206 (95% CI: 0.176 to 0.236), and the direct effect is estimated as c′ = 0.170 (95% CI: 0.141 to 0.199). The paths from CT to adaptive CERS (β = −0.141, 95% CI: − 0.227 to − 0.131) and maladaptive CERS (β = 0.093, 95% CI: 0.059 to 0.132) were both significant. Also, the paths of from adaptive CERS to current depression (β = − 0.084, 95% CI: − 0.082 to − 0.037) and from maladaptive CERS to current depression (β = − 0.299, 95% CI: 0.233 to 0.292) were also significant. The bootstrapping index for an indirect effect (β = 0.012) was significant when adaptive CERS was included as mediating variables since the 95% confidence interval does not include zero (0.006 to 0.018). the bootstrapping index for indirect effect of maladaptive CERS was also significant (β = 0.028,95% CI: 0.016 to 0.040. Therefore, the mediating effect of adaptive CERS and maladaptive CERS on the relationship between CT and current depression were significant. The indirect effect of CT on current depression through adaptive CERS accounted for 5.69% of the total effect. And the indirect effect of CT on depression current through maladaptive CERS accounted for 13.52% of the total effect.
Then we explored the mediation role of specific types of CERS in the relationship between CT and current depression, conducting follow-up parallel mediation analysis. Parallel mediation analysis results showed that mediating effects of acceptance (β = − 0.004, 95% CI: − 0.008 to − 0.001), positive refocusing (β = 0.004, 95% CI: 0.001 to 0.009), positive reappraisal (β = 0.032, 95% CI: 0.021 to 0.044), putting into perspective (β = 0.003,95% CI: 0.0001 to 0.007), blame-others (β = − 0.004, 95% CI: − 0.008 to − 0.001) and catastrophizing (β = 0.034, 95% CI: 0.024 to 0.046) were significant. However, for the remaining types of CERS, the mediating effects were not significant.
Moderated mediation analyses
The results of moderated mediation analyses (Table 4) showed that neuroticism simultaneously moderated the direct effect of CT on current depression (β = 0.035, 95% CI: 0.001 to 0.009), and the indirect effects of CT on current depression through adaptive CERS (adaptive CERS - current depression: β = − 0.034, 95% CI: − 0.007 to − 0.001) and maladaptive CERS (maladaptive CERS - current depression: β = 0.157, 95% CI: 0.017 to 0.025). However, the moderating effects of neuroticism were not significant in the indirect paths from CT to adaptive CERS (β = 0.037, 95% CI: 0.000 to 0.014) and maladaptive CERS (β = − 0.001, 95% CI: − 0.006 to 0.005). The moderated mediation results of specific adaptive CERS (acceptance, positive refocusing, positive reappraisal, putting into perspective) and maladaptive CERS (blame-others, catastrophizing) showed by additional file 1.
In order to further analyze the direct effect and indirect effects of CT on current depression at different levels of neuroticism, we divided neuroticism into three levels of low, medium, and high according to M - SD, M and M + SD. The results are shown as Table 5. Specifically, the direct impact of CT on depression were all significant in low-neuroticism (B = 0.053, 95% CI: 0.011 to 0.095), in medium-neuroticism (B = 0.081, 95% CI: 0.053 to 0.108) and high-neuroticism (B = 0.109, 95% CI: 0.080 to 0.137) (see Fig. 2). In addition, the indirect impact of adaptive CERS on depression were significant when neuroticism for individual was medium (B = − 0.35, 95% CI: − 0.055 to − 0.015) and high (B = − 0.057, 95% CI: − 0.086 to − 0.029). However, when the level of neuroticism was low, the influence of adaptive CERS on depression was not significant (see Fig. 3). Fig. 4 showed that neuroticism could moderate the association between maladaptive CERS and current depression at any value: Low-neuroticism (B = − 0.050, 95% CI: − 0.086 to − 0.015), medium-neuroticism (B = 0.074, 95% CI: 0.045 to 0.103) and high-neuroticism (B = 0.198, 95% CI: 0.159 to 0.236). The final moderated mediation model was displayed in Fig. 5
Discussion
In current study, we tested the hypothesis with moderated mediation model in which CT was associated with current depression as mediated by CERS (adaptive and maladaptive CERS) and moderated by neuroticism (Fig. 1).
The mediating role of adaptive and maladaptive CERS
Consistent with our hypothesis 1, mediation analyses results showed that the adaptive CERS and maladaptive CERS partially mediated the relationship between childhood traumatic experience and current symptoms of depression.
CT was negatively correlated with adaptive CERS and positively correlated with maladaptive CERS. Some researchers pointed out that CT may cause emotion dysregulation in later life [62, 63]. And failing to regulate the emotion is closely related to the occurrence of psychological problems [21]. Our mediation analysis results showed that the maladaptive CERS significantly mediated the relationship between CT and current depression, which confirmed that CT could affect current depression through using maladaptive CERS. Our results also revealed the significant mediating effect of adaptive CERS, and highlighted the importance of using adaptive CERS. Namely, using adaptive CERS may weaken the influence of CT on current depression. This finding is consistent with previous research [28], which acknowledged that the use of cognitive emotion regulation strategies is an important possible mechanism underlying the negative effect of CT on depressive severity in later life. However, some previous studies have shown that the mediating effect of adaptive CERS was not significant [12, 64]. They believed the indirect effect of adaptive CERS was weaker than maladaptive CERS. The reason for this inconsistent result may be caused by using healthy university students in this study whose depression symptoms are generally milder than those in the clinical group. Clinical groups may have difficulty in implementing adaptive strategies effectively compared with non-clinical groups [15]. Therefore, for non-clinical groups especially university students, the adaptive CERS may show stronger indirect effects among the CT and current depressive symptom.
In our results, the mediating effects of acceptance, positive refocusing, positive reappraisal, putting into perspective, blame-others and catastrophizing were significant, and the mediating effects of the remaining CERS types were not significant. Regarding the mediating role of specific cognitive emotion regulation strategies, the results of many studies are not completely consistent [27, 28, 41]. Aldao and Nolen-Hoeksema [65] found that the degree to which reappraisal is adaptive depends on the frequency of maladaptive strategy used. This may also be one of the reasons why the results of the specific CERS analysis in this study are different from previous studies. Moreover, we have not considered that the frequency of using maladaptive CERS would influence the effect of adaptive CERS. To further investigate the specific CERS’s exact effect in the relationship between early traumatic experience and psychiatric symptoms, more research is required in this field.
The moderating role of neuroticism
Moderated mediation analysis largely supports our research hypothesis. It demonstrated that CT can affect the occurrence and development of depression through using adaptive / maladaptive CRES, and neuroticism plays a moderator role in the direct impact of CT on depression, and the indirect impacts from adaptive and maladaptive to depression.
Through simple slope analysis, we found that compared with low neurotic individuals, high neurotic individuals have a more pronounced increase in the severity of depression as the level of CT increased (Fig. 2). Consistent with one study among Chinese university students, neuroticism moderated the relationship between stress and depression [37]. In the process of CT affecting depression, neuroticism, as an essentially vulnerable factor, interacted with various stressors [31]. For that reason, using adaptive CERS has an obvious reduction effect on depression in high neurotic people than in low neurotic people (Fig. 3). Some researchers pointed out that more use of adaptive CERS (e.g. refocusing on planning) is beneficial to resilience and depression [66]. The protective effect of adaptive CERS on depression is more pronounced in highly neurotic people. Then, the depressive severity in the low-neuroticism group decreased with the increase of maladaptive CERS, whereas depressive severity in the high-neuroticism group increased obviously. In other words, low-neuroticism can weaken the effect of maladaptive CERS on depression (Fig. 4). as such, maladaptive CERS was positively related to depressive symptoms. A study of using specific emotion regulation strategies in clinical patients suggested that use of maladaptive CERS was an universal feature of depression and anxiety disorders [67]. This research revealed that maladaptive CERS may increase the level of depression, and this effect is strengthened in people with high neuroticism, and low neuroticism may attenuate this effect.
In a nutshell, our findings broaden previous researches by exploring the mediating roles of adaptive CERS and the moderating role of neuroticism. This result validates the interaction model of personality vulnerability to depression [31]. Neuroticism, as an individual’s specific vulnerability quality, interacts with the experience of CT to affect the current depressive symptoms. Specifically, highly neurotic individuals exposed to childhood trauma are high-risk groups of depression and CERS intervention can be used for high-risk groups. In high-risk groups, adaptive CERS plays a protective role in the process of childhood trauma affecting depression, while maladaptive CERS plays a risk role. Furthermore, we also found that low neuroticism plays a protective role in the process of maladaptive CERS effecting depression. For that reason, the influence of neuroticism should be fully considered when intervening in CERS, so that a more appropriate interventional method can be selected and the effects can be estimated in an accurate manner.
Strengths and limitations
This study validates a new moderated mediation model, demonstrating the moderating role of neuroticism, providing evidence for the mechanism of CT on depression, and correct theoretical guidance for our clinical intervention. In addition, we have controlled the impact of recent stressors, which makes our results more rigorous.
The study also has several limitations. First, this study assessed measurements concurrently as a cross-sectional study. However, in order to better determine the causality, it is necessary to conduct further longitudinal studies on children who experience early life trauma. Especially, the prospective studies about serially assessing changes in emotion regulation ability and mental health outcomes would be informative. Second, we did not investigate and discuss individual differences of using CERS. Individuals do not use a single cognitive emotional regulation strategy. The reality is usually a combination of several strategies. Third, this study was only for university students, and the sample representation was limited.
Conclusion
This study provides powerful evidences through a large university students sample for the mediating role of adaptive / maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies and the moderating role of neuroticism between CT and current depression. Specifically, CT can directly affect current depression, and it can also affect depression through adaptive CERS and maladaptive CERS. Furthermore, different levels of neuroticism would affect the impact of CT on current depression and the impacts of adaptive CERS and maladaptive CERS on current depression. This manifests that cognitive emotion regulation may be a vital factor for people who suffered from CT and current depression, and the influence of neuroticism in this process cannot be ignored.
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Change history
16 May 2022
A Correction to this paper has been published: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-03960-w
Abbreviations
- CT:
-
Childhood trauma
- CERS:
-
Cognitive emotion regulation strategies
- CTQ:
-
Childhood Trauma Questionnaire
- BDI-II:
-
Beck Depression Inventory-II
- CERQ:
-
Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire
- EPQ:
-
Eysenck Personality Questionnaire
- EPQ-N:
-
Neuroticism Subscale of Eysenck’s Personality Questionnaire
- ASLEC:
-
Adolescent Self-Rating Life Events Check
- SD:
-
Standard Deviation
- CI:
-
Confidence Interval
- UCI:
-
Upper Confidence Interval
- LCI:
-
Lower Confidence Interval
- SPSS:
-
Statistical Package for Social Sciences
References
Kessler RC, McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, et al. Angermeyer MJTBjop. Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO world mental health surveys. Br J Psychiatry. 2010;197(5):378–85.
Gibb BE, Chelminski I, Zimmerman MJD. Childhood emotional, physical, and sexual abuse, and diagnoses of depressive and anxiety disorders in adult psychiatric outpatients. Depress Anxiety. 2007;24(4):256–63.
Horan JM, Widom CS. Cumulative childhood risk and adult functioning in abused and neglected children grown up. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;27(3):927–41.
Li M, D'arcy C, Meng X. Maltreatment in childhood substantially increases the risk of adult depression and anxiety in prospective cohort studies: systematic review, meta-analysis, and proportional attributable fractions. Psychol Med. 2016;46(4):717–30.
Rhebergen D, Lamers F, Spijker J, De Graaf R, Beekman A. Penninx BJPm. Course trajectories of unipolar depressive disorders identified by latent class growth analysis. Psychol Med. 2012;42(7):1383–96.
Klein DN, Kotov R. Course of depression in a 10-year prospective study: evidence for qualitatively distinct subgroups. J Abnorm Psychol. 2016;125(3):337.
Fuller-Thomson E, Battiston M, Gadalla TM, Brennenstuhl S. Bouncing back: remission from depression in a 12-year panel study of a representative Canadian community sample. Soc Psych Psych Epid. 2014;49(6):903–10.
Zubin J, Spring B. Vulnerability: a new view of schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1977;86(2):103.
Mandelli L, Petrelli C, Serretti A. The role of specific early trauma in adult depression: a meta-analysis of published literature. Childhood trauma and adult depression. Eur Psychiat. 2015;30(6):665–80.
Lara ME, Klein DN. Psychosocial processes underlying the maintenance and persistence of depression: implications for understanding chronic depression. Clin Psychol Rev. 1999;19(5):553–70.
Rose DT, Abramson LY. Developmental predictors of depressive cognitive style: Research and theory. Developmental Perspectives Depression. 1992(pp. 323–349).
Hopfinger L, Berking M, Bockting CL, Ebert DD. Emotion regulation mediates the effect of childhood trauma on depression. J Affect Disorders. 2016;198:189–97.
Yao GY, Du AL, Yang SC, Gao HL. Mediating effects of personality on the relationship between childhood psychological abuse and depression of undergraduates. Chin J Clin Psycho. 2011;19(01):66–8.
Beck AT. The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. Am J Psychiat. 2008;165(8):969–77.
Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: a transdiagnostic examination. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(10):974–83.
Garnefski N, Kraaij V. The cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2007;23(3):141–9.
Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(2):217–37.
Beck AT. Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders: Penguin; 1979.
Melero S, Orgilés M, Espada J, Morales A. How does depression facilitate psychological difficulties in children? The mediating role of cognitive emotion regulation strategies. Clin Psychol Psychot. 2021;28(2):384–93.
Garnefski N, Teerds J, Kraaij V, Legerstee J, van Den Kommer T. Cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: differences between males and females. Pers Indiv Differ. 2004;36(2):267–76.
Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, et al. Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24(11):2981–90.
Parker G, Gladstone G, Mitchell P, Wilhelm K, Roy K. Do early adverse experiences establish a cognitive vulnerability to depression on exposure to mirroring life events in adulthood? J Affective Disord. 2000;57(1–3):209–15.
Rafaeli E, Bernstein D P, Young J. Schema therapy: Distinctive features. Routledge; 2010. ISBN 9780415462990.
Burns EE, Jackson JL, Harding HG. Child maltreatment, emotion regulation, and posttraumatic stress: the impact of emotional abuse. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma. 2010;19(8):801–19.
Chen Y, Baram TZ. Toward understanding how early-life stress reprograms cognitive and emotional brain networks. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;41(1):197–206.
Grant KE, Lyons AL, Finkelstein J-AS, Conway KM, Reynolds LK, O'Koon JH, et al. Gender differences in rates of depressive symptoms among low-income, urban, African American youth: a test of two mediational hypotheses. J Youth Adolescence. 2004;33(6):523–33.
Huh HJ, Kim KH, Lee HK, Chae JH. The relationship between childhood trauma and the severity of adulthood depression and anxiety symptoms in a clinical sample: the mediating role of cognitive emotion regulation strategies. J Affect Disorders. 2017;213:44–50.
Zhang F, Liu N, Huang C, Kang Y, Zhang B, Sun Z, et al. The relationship between childhood trauma and adult depression: the mediating role of adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies. Asian J Psychiatr. 2020;48:101911.
Choi JY, Oh KJ. Cumulative childhood trauma and psychological maladjustment of sexually abused children in Korea: mediating effects of emotion regulation. Child Abuse Negl. 2014;38(2):296–303.
Kim J, Cicchetti D. Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(6): 706–16.
Zuroff DC, Mongrain M, Santor DA. Conceptualizing and measuring personality vulnerability to depression: comment on Coyne and Whiffen (1995). Psychol Bull. 2004;130(3):489–511.
Lee MA, Song R. Childhood abuse, personality traits, and depressive symptoms in adulthood. Child Abuse Negl. 2017;65:194–203.
Hovens JG, Giltay EJ, van Hemert AM, Penninx BW. Childhood maltreatment and the course of depressive and anxiety disorders: the contribution of personality characteristics. Depress Anxiety. 2016;33(1):27–34.
Kendler KS, Gatz M, Gardner CO, Pedersen NL. Personality and major depression: a Swedish longitudinal, population-based twin study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(10):1113–20.
Hu YQ, Peng LY, Zeng ZH, OuYang XY, Ding DQ, Wang YL, et al. The effect of childhood maltreatment on college Students’Depression: a moderated mediating model. Chin J Clin Psycho. 2020;28(5):5.
Ling Y. Neuroticism as a moderator of the effects on adolescent stress and depressive symptoms:a longitudinal study. Stud Psychol Behavior. 2013;11(3):6.
Xi C, Ling Y, Zhong MT, Liu Y, Zhou Q, Yao SQ, Yi JY. The Moderation of Neuroticism on the Relationship between Stress and Depression in College Students. Chin J Clin Psycho. 2016;24(4):4.
Watson D, Clark LA, Harkness AR. Structures of personality and their relevance to psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103(1):18.
Liu C, Chen L, Chen S. Influence of neuroticism on depressive symptoms among Chinese adolescents: the mediation effects of cognitive emotion regulation strategies. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:420.
Yoon KL, Maltby J, Joormann J. A pathway from neuroticism to depression: examining the role of emotion regulation. Anxiety Stress Copin. 2013;26(5):558–72.
Stikkelbroek Y, Bodden D, Kleinjan M, Reijnders M, Baar A. Adolescent depression and negative life events, the mediating role of cognitive emotion regulation. PLoS One. 2016;11(8):e0161062.
De Girolamo G, Dagani J, Purcell R, Cocchi A, McGorry P. Age of onset of mental disorders and use of mental health services: needs, opportunities and obstacles. Epidemiol Psichiats. 2012;21(1):47–57.
Pedrelli P, Nyer M, Yeung A, Zulauf C, Wilens T. College students: mental health problems and treatment considerations. Acad Psychiatr. 2015;39(5):503–11.
Sussman S, Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: developmental period facilitative of the addictions. Eval Health Professions. 2014;37(2):147–55.
Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D, Ahluvalia T, et al. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the childhood trauma questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27(2):169–90.
Dovran A, Winje D, Overland SN, Breivik K, Arefjord K, Dalsbo AS, et al. Psychometric properties of the Norwegian version of the childhood trauma questionnaire in high-risk groups. Scand J Psychol. 2013;54(4):286–91.
Gerdner A, Allgulander C. Psychometric properties of the Swedish version of the childhood trauma questionnaire-short form (CTQ-SF). Nord J Psychiatry. 2009;63(2):160–70.
Zhao XF, Zhang YL, Li LF, Zhou YJ. Evaluation on reliability and validity of Chinese version of childhood trauma questionnaire. Chin J Clin Rehabil. 2005;9(16):209–11.
Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck depression inventory, (BDI-II) San Antonio. Psychol Assoc. 1996. https://www.nctsn.org/measures/beck-depression-inventory-second-edition. Accessed 20 Aug 2020.
Yang WH, Liu SL, Zhou T. Reliability and validity of Chinese version of the Beck depression inventory-II in Chinese adolescents. Chin J Clin Psycho. 2014;02.
Chamizo-Nieto MT, Rey L, Sánchez-Álvarez N. Validation of the spanish version of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire in adolescents. Psicothema. 2020;32(1):153–9.
Carvajal BP, Molina-Martínez MÁ, Fernández-Fernández V, Paniagua-Granados T, Lasa-Aristu A, Luque-Reca O. Psychometric properties of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire (CERQ) in Spanish older adults. Aging Ment Health. 2020:1–10.
Schäfer JL, Cibils Filho BR, de Moura TC, Tavares VC, Arteche AX, Kristensen CH. Psychometric properties of the Brazilian version of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Trends Psychiatry Psychotherapy. 2018;40(2):160–9.
McKinnon A, Kuyken W, Hayes R, Werner-Seidler A, Watson P, Dalgleish T, et al. The psychometric properties of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire (CERQ) in a clinical sample of adults with recurrent depression. J Affective Disord. 2020;276:212–9.
Zhu XZ, Auerbach RP, Yao SQ, Abela JR, Xiao J, Tong X. Psychometric properties of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire: Chinese version. Cognit Emot. 2008;22(2):288–307.
Eysenck HJ, Eysenck SBG. Manual of the Eysenck personality questionnaire (junior & adult). Hodder and Stoughton Educational. 1975.
Gong YX. Revised Eysenck personality questionnaire (EPQ-R). Changsha: Map Publishing House; 1992.
Liu XC, Liu LQ, Yang J, Chai FX, Sun LM. Development and psychometric reliability and validity of adolescent self-rating life events checklist. Shandong Arch Psychiatry. 1997;10:15–9.
Xin X, Yao S. Validity and reliability of the adolescent self-rating life events checklist in middle school students. Chin Ment Health J. 2015;5:355–60.
Hayes AF. PROCESS macro (version 3). NY: Guilford Publications; 2017.
Hayes AF, Rockwood NJ. Conditional process analysis: concepts, computation, and advances in the modeling of the contingencies of mechanisms. Am Behav Sci. 2020;64(1):19–54.
Cloitre M, Stolbach BC, Herman JL, Kolk Bvd, Pynoos R, Wang J, Petkova E. a developmental approach to complex PTSD: childhood and adult cumulative trauma as predictors of symptom complexity. J Trauma Stress 2009;22(5):399–408.
Roth S, Newman E, Pelcovitz D, Van Der Kolk B, Mandel FS. Complex PTSD in victims exposed to sexual and physical abuse: results from the DSM-IV field trial for posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress. 1997;10(4):539–55.
Demir Z, Böge K, Fan Y, Hartling C, Harb MR, Hahn E, et al. The role of emotion regulation as a mediator between early life stress and posttraumatic stress disorder, depression and anxiety in Syrian refugees. Transl Psychiat. 2020;10(1):1–10.
Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S. When are adaptive strategies most predictive of psychopathology? J Abnorm Soc Psychol. 2012;121(1):276.
Min JA, Yu JJ, Lee CU, Chae JH. Cognitive emotion regulation strategies contributing to resilience in patients with depression and/or anxiety disorders. Compr Psychiatry. 2013;54(8):1190–7.
D’Avanzato C, Joormann J, Siemer M, Gotlib IH. Emotion regulation in depression and anxiety: examining diagnostic specificity and stability of strategy use. Cogn Ther Res. 2013;37(5):968–80.
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 81501176 to Lifeng Wang) and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central South University (Xiang Wang, grant number 2021zzts0374).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
QC took primary responsibility in the design of this study and developing the initial draft of this manuscript. QT provided guidance in the design of the project and revised this manuscript. XW contributed to the data entry and analysis and revised the manuscript. RY and FN were involved in the data collection. JF, YL and LW commented on and revised this manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
The Ethics Committee of the Third Xiangya Hospital of Central South University approved this study. And this study was also carried out in accordance with ethical guidelines of Central South University. All participants were given informed consent before completing the measures.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
The original version of this article was revised: the numbering of intext citations have been updated.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Chu, Q., Wang, X., Yao, R. et al. Childhood trauma and current depression among Chinese university students: a moderated mediation model of cognitive emotion regulation strategies and neuroticism. BMC Psychiatry 22, 90 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-021-03673-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-021-03673-6