Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Deaf and hard-of-hearing children and adolescents’ mental health, Quality of Life and communication

Abstract

Mental health problems and lower Quality of Life (QoL) are more common in deaf and hard-of-hearing – (D)HH – children than in typically hearing (TH) children. Communication has been repeatedly linked to both mental health and QoL. The aims of this study were to compare mental health and QoL between signing deaf and hard-of-hearing (DHH), hard-of-hearing (HH) and TH children and to study associations between mental health/QoL and severity of hearing loss and communication. 106 children and adolescents (mean age 11;8; SD = 3.42), 59 of them DHH and 47 HH, and their parents reported child mental health and QoL outcomes. Parents also provided information about their children's communication, hearing loss and education while their children's cognitive ability was assessed. Although (D)HH and their parents rated their mental health similar to their TH peers, about twice as many (D)HH children rated themselves in the clinical range. However, (D)HH children rated their QoL as similar to their TH peers, while their parents rated it significantly lower. Associations between communicative competence, parent-reported mental health and QoL were found, whereas severity of hearing loss based on parent-report had no significant association with either mental health or QoL. These results are in line with other studies and emphasise the need to follow up on (D)HH children's mental health, QoL and communication.

Peer Review reports

Background

For decades, mental health problems in deaf and hard-of-hearing "(D)HH" children and adolescents have been of clinical and research interest. In this paper, mental health problems are defined as the presence of symptoms of mental health disorders (e.g. low mood, problems with attention, etc.) as well as mental health disorders based on diagnostic classification, i.e. the combination and severity of symptoms combined with clinically significant loss of function. In the following sections, the term "HH" refers to hard-of-hearing children with a preference for spoken language; "DHH" to signing deaf and hard-of-hearing children and "(D)HH" to both groups, while the term "children" will describe both children and adolescents in this paper.

Mental health

Elevated prevalence of mental health problems has been reported for (D)HH children; twice to four times as high as their typically hearing (TH) peers depending on other risk factors [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10]. These findings are mainly based on parent and teacher-report, whereas (D)HH children do not rate themselves significantly higher than their TH peers on mental health problems [2, 3, 11]. A systematic review on psychopathology [11] reports more symptoms of depression, anxiety, aggression and behavioural disorders in (D)HH children than in TH children. Good communication skills, early detection of hearing loss (HL), early intervention, good cognitive abilities, good peer relationships for girls and good sports skills for boys have been identified as potential protective factors for mental health problems [1, 5, 10, 11]. Aetiology of HL, additional disabilities, intellectual impairment and low language abilities have been established as risk factors for mental health problems [5, 8, 12]. As many as 25% to 40% of (D)HH children have additional disabilities [4, 5, 13]. Degree of HL has not been found to have an effect on mental health problems [11]. Late or incorrect diagnosis of mental health problems in (D)HH people is common due to the complexity and overlap of cultural, linguistic and clinical factors [14,15,16,17]. A recent study on psychiatric disorders and reasons for referral to generic Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services (CAMHS) showed that 18.1% of the (D)HH children in Norway were referred compared to 5% of the TH population [18]. Norwegian DHH children were also referred earlier than their TH peers.

Quality of life

Comparing studies on Quality of life (QoL) is challenging due to the variety of definitions of the concept. The World Health Organisation (WHO) defines QoL as “an individual's perception of their position in life in the context of the culture and value systems in which they live and in relation to their goals, expectations, standards and concerns” (World Health Organization, 1995, p. 1405 [19]). Quality of Life (QoL) in (D)HH children, especially those using cochlear implants (CI), has been a point of interest for researchers from several fields. A systematic review on QoL after paediatric CI implantation [20] concludes that few of these studies are based on generic measures and primarily on small sample sizes, precluding other overall findings on QoL. The same authors, therefore, recommend strict inclusion criteria and generic QoL instruments for future studies. Another systematic review [21] finds that (D)HH children score significantly lower on validated QoL measures. Methodological issues with this systematic review, including misinterpretation of the results of two of the included studies, have been discussed by Aanondsen, Jozefiak, Heiling et al. [22]. Some studies report no significant differences between TH and (D)HH children in QoL [23,24,25], whereas recent Norwegian studies on (D)HH children [3] and children with CI [26, 27] report significantly lower overall QoL (parent and self-report). The subjectiveness of QoL as a concept [28], differences between parent- and self-report, and the importance of multiple perspectives have been described [21, 22, 29,30,31,32,33,34]. Age [31, 35], degree of HL [23,24,25], and communication [26, 27, 36] have all been associated with QoL in (D)HH children. Significant negative associations between age and QoL for TH [37] and (D)HH children [31, 35] have been confirmed, whereas negative associations between degree of HL and QoL have not [23,24,25].

Communication

In this paper, the term “communication” will refer to language skills and communicative competence, whereas “language” refers to all natural languages, i.e., both spoken and sign languages independent of modality and country unless otherwise specified; when referring to other studies the respective authors’ terms are used for vocabulary, pragmatic skills, social communication, etc.

Mode and level of communication have been suggested as factors affecting mental health and QoL in (D)HH children. The historical debate about sign versus spoken language for (D)HH children has been reignited by technological advances such as CIs [38], and bilingual/bicultural approaches have been questioned again [39]. Even though CIs have improved speech and spoken language outcomes, (D)HH children experience delays in language development [26, 40,41,42]. Sign languages have been acknowledged as natural languages [43], leading to a shift viewing (D)HH people in a socio-cultural instead of a disability perspective [44]. There is currently no consensus on interventions [38] due to variability in language outcomes [45], small sample sizes [38], the lack of studies on functional language, and focus on vocabulary, speech perception and production [39, 46]. As only about 5% have (D)HH parents [47], most (D)HH children learn sign language from non-native signers, which can cause delays [48]. DHH children of DHH parents reach language milestones as quickly as TH peers [47,48,49]. Successful communication between parents and (D)HH children is essential for language, cognitive and socio-emotional development independent of modality [40, 50].

Good communication skills independent of modality are associated with better mental health [1, 10, 11], while negative associations have been found for language development and behavioural problems [12, 51]. In addition, negative associations have been found for (D)HH children’s functional language skills in conversation with TH peers and peer problems [52]. Ching, Cupples, Leigh et al. [53] report positive associations between functional auditory performance, use of speech and language skills, psychosocial skills, and QoL and therefore emphasise the importance of functional performance. The same authors also found that better auditory functional performance and pragmatic language skills were associated with better psychosocial abilities and QoL, whereas structural language abilities were not [53].

Overall, several factors complicate the understanding of mental health, QoL and communication in (D)HH children and adolescents. Among these are the low prevalence of moderate to profound HL [54], resulting in small sample sizes, heterogeneity in outcomes for mental health, QoL [20] and communication due to differences in aetiology of HL [55], access to newborn hearing screening, early intervention as well as the likelihood of other disabilities [4, 9]. In addition, mode and level of communication [1, 11], differences in services between countries, choice of instruments (generic vs ad hoc [20], written vs sign language measures [14, 56]) as well as methodological issues such as those reported here previously [22] also contribute to the heterogeneity in findings. Assessing DHH children is challenging, as few validated assessment tools are available in sign language, but necessary as studies have shown that DHH children report more symptoms on measures in Sign Language than on written ones [14, 56].

This study addresses several of the challenges outlined above by assessing mental health problems and QoL in DHH children with validated questionnaires in both written Norwegian and Norwegian Sign Language (NSL). It also provides insight into the mental health and QoL of Norwegian (D)HH children and adolescents. Knowledge of the prevalence of mental health problems can in turn be used to improve early intervention and the organisation of specialised mental health services in Norway.

Aims

The main aim of the present study is to study associations between mental health, QoL, communication, and parent-reported HL in hard-of-hearing (HH) and signing DHH children.

We addressed the following research questions:

  1. 1.

    Are there differences in mental health (self-and parent-report) between (D)HH and TH children?

  2. 2.

    Are there differences in QoL (self-and parent-report) between (D)HH and TH children?

  3. 3.

    A) What is the association between (D)HH children's degree of HL and mental health?

    B) What is the association between (D)HH children's degree of HL and QoL?

  4. 4.

    A) What is the association between (D)HH children's communication (language skills and communicative competence) and mental health?

    B) What is the association between (D)HH children's communication (language skills and communicative competence) and QoL?

Methods

Participants

Hearing loss of > 40 dB affects 1.4 per 1000 infants [57]; this amounts to 266 children aged 6–18 with a HL in central and northern Norway. DHH children aged 6–17 were recruited from A.C. Møller school, a deaf school for central and northern Norway. DHH adolescents aged 15–20 attending Tiller Upper Secondary School (Trondheim, Norway) with NSL as their first or second language were also invited. In total, 86% (59/69) of the DHH children were included. Ninety-six percent (23/24) of the HH children aged 6–15 were recruited from the Norwegian National Support System for Special Education (Statped), and 16% (24/147) of the HH children aged 6–19 from the local audiology department. None of the HH children received their education in Norwegian Sign Language either part- or full time. The overall response rate for the complete sample (DHH and HH children) was 44% (106/240) (see Fig. 1). The parents of all children were also invited to participate.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart for the inclusion of participants

In total, 106 (D)HH children (62.3% girls) participated, with a mean age of 11.8 years (SD = 3.42; range 6 to 19) and a mean nonverbal IQ of 108.0 (SD = 15.46; range = 74–143). One participant with a very low non-verbal IQ was excluded from analysis. Among the DHH children, 45 DHH children completed the SDQ-NOR, whereas 40 completed both the SDQ-NSL and the SDQ-NOR. For the ILC, 55 DHH children completed the ILC-NOR and 48 completed both the ILC-NSL and the ILC-NOR. Seventy-three of the 86 (84.9%) mothers had completed at least 12 years of education, whereas 56 of the 82 (68.3%) fathers had completed at least 12 years of education. Data were collected between November 1, 2016, and March 23, 2018. The majority of the DHH children (73.2%) mainly attended mainstream schools while spending two to six weeks at the deaf school per school year. Almost all HH children (87%) attended mainstream school full-time. The participants' hearing and communication-related information are reported in Tables 1 and 2.

Table 1 Hearing-related characteristics (parent report) for Norwegian DHH and HH children
Table 2 Communicative competence (parent report)

Studies based on Norwegian community samples, employing the same measures as in our study, were used for comparison. Data for the study on the SDQ-NOR were collected in 2002 [58, 59]. The self-report sample included 29426 [58] TH children aged 8 to 19, while the parent-report sample included 8517 parents of TH children [59] aged 10 to 13. Data for the ILC-NOR were collected between 2004 and 2005, for TH children aged 8 to 16, which resulted in 1987 self-reports and 2563 parent-reports [30].

Measures

Socio-demographic and hearing-related information

Parents completed a questionnaire about their children’s age and gender, type of education, and their own attendance of sign language classes. Parents also reported on their children’s type and degree of HL, as we did not have access to audiological records,

Communication

Spoken language skills

Auditory performance (speech intelligibility and listening skills) were assessed using Categories of Auditory Performance (CAP; [60]) and Speech Intelligibility Rating (SIR, [61]). The CAP and SIR are validated single-item scales frequently used in research. Interrater reliability for the Danish version was reported as good (CAP: kappa = 0.785; SIR: kappa = 0.848 [1]). The sum of the CAP and SIR was calculated for each child as the Spoken Language Skills Score.

Sign language skills

Sign language skills were assessed using the Norwegian version of the Sign Language Production Scale (SPS) and the Sign Language Understanding Scale (SUS). Dammeyer [1] designed and validated the SPS and SUS as a short screening of sign language skills for research purposes based on the CAP and SIR. The interrater reliability of the Danish version was reported as being good (kappa = 0.944 for SUS and kappa = 0.921 for SPS; [1]). The validity of the Danish version of the SUS based on correlations between the SUS and the sign language receptive skill test reached statistical significance (Spearman rank correlation coefficient = 0.905, p < 0.000;). No corresponding test was available for sign language production. The sum of the SPS and SUS scores, the “Sign Language Skills Score,” was calculated for each child.

Communicative competence

The participants' communicative competence was assessed using the Norwegian version [62] of the Children’s Communication Checklist Second Edition (CCC-2), which was developed by Bishop [63, 64]. The reliability and validity of the Norwegian version have been confirmed [65]. The CCC-2 is completed by parents and/or teachers. It consists of 10 subscales (7 items per subscale): (A) speech, (B) syntax, (C) semantics, (D) coherence, (E) inappropriate initiation, (F) stereotyped language, (G) use of context, (H) non-verbal communication, (I) social relations, and (J) interests. The General Communication Composite (GCC) is based on the sum of the scaled scores for subscales A to H.

Cognitive abilities

The nonverbal intelligence of the participants was assessed using the well-validated Leiter International Performance Scale – Third Edition (Leiter-3) [66]. Compared to the original validation, significantly higher nonverbal IQ scores were found for a TH Scandinavian sample (M = 108.6, SD = 8.4; [61]).

Mental health

The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) is a brief well-validated [67, 68] measure to assess mental health problems and pro-social behaviour. It consists of 25 items, each scored on a 3-point Likert scale (0 = "Not true," 1 = "Somewhat true" and 2 = "Certainly true"). These are grouped into five scales (Emotional Problems, Conduct Problems, Hyperactivity-Inattention, Peer Problems, and Pro-social Behavior) as well as a Total Difficulties score based on the four negative subscales, higher scores indicating more difficulties. For this study, we administered both the parent and the self-report versions of the SDQ in written Norwegian (SDQ-NOR) and in Norwegian Sign Language (SDQ-NSL; [69]); self-reports only for children ≥ 9.

Quality of Life (QoL)

The Inventory of Life Quality in Children and Adolescents – ILC [30, 32] is a validated multi-informant assessment for QoL based on seven items. One item assesses overall QoL, and six items address the child's physical and mental health, school and family functioning, social contact with peers, interests, and recreational activities. [30, 32]. We administered both the parent and the self-report versions of the ILC in written Norwegian (ILC-NOR) and NSL (ILC-NSL; [22]) for this study.

Procedures

The DHH children and their parents received oral/signed and written information about participation. According to the study's survey procedures, written informed consent was obtained for all participants prior to inclusion. I.e. from all parents as well as from adolescents ≥ 16. DHH children ≤ 16, whose parents already had consented to their children’s participation, were also asked to confirm consent verbally or in NSL. DHH children responded to the written and the NSL versions of the ILC and SDQ. A more detailed description of the DHH group can be found here: [22, 69]. The HH children and their parents recruited through the ENT department received information about the study by mail. According to the study's survey procedures, written informed consent was obtained from adolescents ≥ 16 and parents before inclusion. The HH children responded to the paper and pencil version of the SDQ-NOR and ILC-NOR. Parents for both groups completed questionnaires on language, communicative competence, HL, education, and the SDQ and ILC.

Statistical analyses

We handled missing values using multiple imputation; a detailed description of the missing values can be found in Appendix 3. The imputation model included all variables used in at least one of the subsequent analyses. Degree of HL, additional impairment, total score SDQ (parent and self-report: SDQ-NSL and SDQ-NOR), QoL score (parent and self-report: ILC-NSL and ILC-NOR), CCC-2 GCC, and spoken and sign language skills were included. The following variables were also included in the imputation model: group (DHH/HH), gender, age, mother's and father's education, nonverbal IQ, age at detection of HL and cause of HL. We created M = 100 imputed data sets, generally regarded as sufficient [70]. We imputed with no restrictions to the range and no post-imputation rounding, as recommended [71]. Analysis based on multiple imputation provides unbiased estimates under the missing at random assumption, while a complete case analysis would give unbiased estimates only under the more restrictive missing completely at random assumption. Some variables were structurally missing, such as CCC-2 GCC for those who do not speak in complete sentences, SDQ-NSL, ILC-NSL and sign language skills for those not using NSL. First, we imputed all missing values, including these, then we deleted the imputed values in the positions where there were structurally missing.

Associations between HL, communication, mental health and QoL were investigated using linear regression with mental health and QoL as dependent variables. All regression analyses were adjusted for age and gender. Group differences between DHH and TH children were studied using t-tests not assuming equal variances. Wald confidence intervals were calculated for proportions. Two-sided p values < 0.05 were taken to indicate statistical significance, and 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) are reported where relevant. However, p-values between 0.01 and 0.05 should be interpreted with caution due to multiple hypotheses. All analyses were conducted in Stata/SE 17.0 for Windows.

Ethics

Before inclusion, written informed consent was obtained from the parents and adolescents ≥ 16 and oral/signed informed consent from the children < 16. Study approval was given by the Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics (reference number: 2015/1739/REK midt).

Results

Mental health in DHH, HH and TH children

No significant differences were found between DHH and HH children for self-reported (ß.-0.672; CI: -4.36 to 3.01; p:0.715) and parent-reported mental health (ß -0.569; CI: -3.20 to 2.06; p: 0.668). DHH children reported significantly more mental health symptoms on the SDQ-NSL than TH children on the SDQ-NOR. For the SDQ-NOR, no significant differences were found between DHH and TH children and HH and TH children on both self- and parent-report (Table 3).

Table 3 Comparison of DHH, HH and TH children's mental health (means and SD) based on multiply imputed data

Eighteen point two percent of the DHH children rated themselves in the clinical range on the SDQ-NSL, 16.4% on the SDQ-NOR and 16.2% of the HH children compared to 8.7% of the TH children [58]; see Table 4. Prevalence based on complete case analysis can be found in Appendix 1.

Table 4 Prevalence of mental health problems in DHH, HH and TH children based on multiply imputed data (MI). Estimates and 95% confidence intervals (CI) based on Wald

QoL in DHH, HH and TH children

No significant differences were found between DHH and HH children for self-reported (ß 0.393; CI: -1.33 to 2.12; p:0.651) and parent-reported QoL (ß 1.03; CI: –0.92 to 2.98; p: 0.297). Parents of DHH and HH children reported significantly lower QoL than parents of TH children: see Table 5.

Table 5 Comparison of DHH, HH and TH children's QoL (means and SD) based on multiply imputed data

Associations between mental health, QoL and HL in DHH and HH children

Small and non-significant regression coefficients were found for degree of HL on mental health (see Table 6) and QoL (see Table 7) for both self- and parent-reports.

Table 6 Linear regression with mental health as dependent variable, HL, language skills or communicative competence added one at a time as covariates based on multiply imputed data
Table 7 Linear regression with QoL as dependent variables, HL, language skills or communicative competence added one at a time as covariates based on multiply imputed data

Associations between mental health, QoL and communication in DHH and HH children

Significant regression coefficients were found for communicative competence (CCC-2) and parent-reported mental health (see Table 6), and parent-reported QoL (see Table 7). For spoken and sign language skills, however, non-significant regression coefficients were found for spoken and sign language skills on mental health (see Table 6) for both self-and parent-report. For QoL a significant regression coefficient was found for sign language on the ILC-NOR (see Table 7), but not for sign language on the ILC-NSL.

Discussion

In the present study, no mean differences were found between DHH, HH and TH children for self– or parent-reported mental health on the SDQ-NOR. On the SDQ-NSL, however, DHH children reported significantly more mental health problems than TH children, and more than twice as many DHH children scored in the borderline range on the SDQ-NSL compared to the SDQ-NOR. Both DHH and HH children were about twice as likely to rate themselves in the clinical range for mental health problems compared to TH children, indicating clinically significant differences in mental health. As the data for the TH children were collected 15 years prior to our study, this could cause concerns regarding the validity of our comparison as an increase of emotional problems, especially in teenage girls has been reported both nationally [72, 73] as well as globally [74]. There are, however, other studies such as the one by Pitchforth, Fahy, Ford et al. [75] on a large national British sample that did not find an increase in mental health problems based on the SDQ total score for children and adolescents aged 4 to 16 between 1995 and 2014. As the SDQ total score is based on both internalising and externalising problems and studies such as the one by Pitchforth, Fahy, Ford et al. [75] do not report a significant increase, differences in prevalence for mental health problems found in this study are not likely to be explained solely by an increase in emotional problems in general.

Parents of DHH and HH children reported significantly lower QoL than parents of TH children, while DHH and HH children did not rate themselves significantly differently from their TH peers. Degree of HL was not associated with either mental health or QoL, whereas parent-rated communicative competence was positively associated with parent-rated mental health and QoL.

The lack of differences in mental health between DHH and HH children [2, 7, 76] and the lack of association between degree of HL and mental health are in accordance with previous studies [1, 11, 77,78,79]. The elevated prevalence is also in accordance with several international studies [1, 2, 6, 9, 11] and two recent Norwegian [3, 18] studies, although some report a prevalence of four times as high.

DHH children being more likely to rate themselves in the borderline range for mental health problems on the SDQ-NSL is in accordance with other studies that have found DHH children to under-report symptoms on written measures [14, 56]. Parental support, including sign language tuition, is quite extensive in Scandinavia, which may result in better mental health in (D)HH children, as suggested by other authors [4]. This is likely to have had a preventive effect on mental health [5, 11, 80].

The (D)HH children in this study did not rate their QoL as significantly different from TH peers, which was in line with studies from other countries [23, 24, 29], but in contrast to two Norwegian studies [3, 26]. Overgaard, Oerbeck, Wagner et al.’s study [3] only included self-report measures for adolescents ≥ 13, the (D)HH children in our study were younger. As age has been found to be negatively associated with QoL, this could explain the difference. A closer look at the mean scores of Overgaard, Oerbeck, Wagner et al. [3]’s study shows that the self-reported mean scores of the DHH children were in the average range according to the Norwegian norms, indicating that the observed difference is statistically but not clinically significant. The parents in our study rated QoL of their DHH and HH children as significantly lower than parents of TH children. Parents of DHH children scored their children’s QoL as close to the “below average” range according to Norwegian norms, indicating a clinically significant difference [30]. The difference in ratings between parents and children as informants has been observed in several studies [22, 29, 34, 81]. Possible explanations for this might be that parents experience the impact of their child's HL as more severe [31], that some aspects of QoL (school, family, friends) are less observable for parents [34] or that communication problems may prevent parents from a better insight into their children's subjective experience [29]. The difference in perspectives, which also is reported in TH children, emphasises the need to use self-reported QoL as the authentic QoL report, to evaluate (D)HH children's QoL, QoL being a subjective concept per definition. Parent-report should be used as supplemental information only [28].

Significant associations between communicative competence, mental health and QoL were found for parent-reported mental health and QoL. A closer look at other studies that have found associations between (D)HH children's communication skills and mental health showed that these studies were based on either parent- and/or teacher-report, but not self-report [1, 6, 8]. Our findings are, therefore, in line with these. Spoken and sign language skills not showing significant associations with parent-reported mental health was more surprising as we used the same assessment tools as Dammeyer [1] and a similar sample. A possible explanation could be the statistics used, where Dammeyer [1] dichotomised SDQ-total scores and looked at the (D)HH children with high language skills (maximum score only); or generally high performance, especially on spoken language skills for (D)HH children in his study. The significant positive associations between parent-reported sign language skills and self-reported QoL are in accordance with two studies [26, 27] that find weak to moderate significant correlations between language skills and QoL. When adolescents perceive to understand most of their parents' expressive communication, they report better QoL [36]. Several authors have noted the lack of studies based on pragmatic/functional language [39, 46] use, while others have shown that (D)HH children with age-appropriate vocabulary still are delayed in pragmatic language development [82]. This may explain the variations in findings [10, 80] and emphasises the need for validated tools for pragmatic language and communicative abilities for (D)HH children.

Strengths and limitations

A major strength of the present study is the use of generic assessment tools (SDQ and ILC) also validated in NSL. Further strengths are the multi-informant perspective, a representative sample for DHH children for our region and information on HL, additional disabilities, cognitive abilities, communication, age at detection and type of school. A major limitation is the small sample size that prevented us from studying relevant subdomains within mental health, QoL and communicative competence, and affects statistical precision and generalisability. Another limitation is the low participation of HH children recruited from the ENT unit, which affects the representativeness of the HH group. A further limitation is related to the comparison of the TH samples, as the studies on the SDQ and the ILC for these samples were carried out 10 to 15 years prior to our study as already discussed regarding the SDQ. For the comparison of the ILC this does not pose a problem as a recent Norwegian study confirmed that QoL-reports in adolescents have been stable across the past 13 years [73]. Further, the spoken and sign language measures are simple one-scale items that do not capture the complexity of pragmatic language. The CCC-2 is not designed or validated for (D)HH children but was used as the best solution at the time of data collection to collect data on pragmatic language and compensate for single-item scales of the CAP and SIR. As parents were the only raters to report on both communication and mental health/QoL, this might have affected the associations found in this study. An objective assessment of communication could have compensated for this.

Conclusion

Although (D)HH children and their parents, on average, rate their mental health as similar to their TH peers, about twice as many rate themselves in the clinical range for mental health problems. Even though prevalence is lower than in international studies, it still emphasises the need for specialised mental health services and validated assessment tailored to this group to ensure early referral and correct assessment. (D)HH children do not rate their QoL as lower than their TH peers, in contrast to their parents. This emphasises the importance of using parent-reported QoL as supplementary information only. The associations between communicative competence, mental health and QoL capture the complexity of understanding and supporting (D)HH families, the need to consider overall development and interdisciplinary collaboration. The need for validated assessment of pragmatic language skills (spoken and sign) for Norwegian (D)HH children has also become apparent for clinical and research purposes. International longitudinal interdisciplinary studies and systematic reviews should be conducted to handle challenges with small sample sizes, cross-sectional studies, and lacking consensus.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to the sensitivity of information and the small sample size. They are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

References

  1. Dammeyer J. Psychosocial development in a Danish population of children with cochlear implants and deaf and hard-of-hearing children. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2010;15(1):50–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Stevenson J, Kreppner J, Pimperton H, Worsfold S, Kennedy C. Emotional and behavioural difficulties in children and adolescents with hearing impairment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;24(5):477–96.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Overgaard KR, Oerbeck B, Wagner K, Friis S, Øhre B, Zeiner P. Youth with hearing loss: Emotional and behavioral problems and quality of life. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;145: 110718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Mejstad L, Heiling K, Svedin CG. Mental health and self-image among deaf and hard of hearing children. Am Ann Deaf. 2009;153(5):504–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Fellinger J, Holzinger D, Pollard R. Mental health of deaf people. The Lancet. 2012;379(9820):1037–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Fellinger J, Holzinger D, Sattel H, Laucht M, Goldberg D. Correlates of mental health disorders among children with hearing impairments. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009;51(8):635–341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Stevenson J, Pimperton H, Kreppner J, Worsfold S, Terlektsi E, Kennedy C. Emotional and behaviour difficulties in teenagers with permanent childhood hearing loss. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2017;101:186–95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Theunissen SC, Rieffe C, Kouwenberg M, Raeve LJI, Soede W, Briaire JJ, et al. Behavioral problems in school-aged hearing-impaired children: The influence of sociodemographic, linguistic, and medical factors. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;23(4):187–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. van Gent T, Goedhart AW, Hindley PA, Treffers PDA. Prevalence and correlates of psychopathology in a sample of deaf adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48(9):950–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. VanOrmer JL, Rossetti KG, Zlomke KR. The Development of Behavioral Difficulties in Hard-of-Hearing and Deaf Youth. Child Fam Behav Ther. 2019;41(4):179–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Theunissen SC, Rieffe C, Netten AP, Briaire JJ, Soede W, Schoones JW, et al. Psychopathology and its risk and protective factors in hearing-impaired children and adolescents: A systematic review. JAMA Pediatr. 2014;168(2):170–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Stevenson J, McCann D, Watkin P, Worsfold S, Kennedy C, Hearing Outcomes Study T. The relationship between language development and behaviour problems in children with hearing loss. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(1):77–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Roberts S, Wright B, Moore K, Smith S, Allgar V, Tennant A, et al. Translation into British Sign Language and validation of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Health Serv Del Res. 2015;3(2).

  14. Cornes AJ, Rohan MJ, Napier J, Rey JM. Reading the signs: Impact of signed versus written questionnaires on the prevalence of psychopathology among deaf adolescents. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006;40(8):665–73.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Øhre B, Von Tetzchner S, Falkum E. Deaf adults and mental health: A review of recent research on the prevalence and distribution of psychiatric symptoms and disorders in the prelingually deaf adult population. Int J Ment Health Deaf. 2011;1(1):3–22.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Heiling K, Eidevall L. Deaf and hard-of-hearing children with mental health problems. Challenges and resources in Europe. Int J Ment Health Deaf. 2011;1(1):37–45.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Hall WC, Levin LL, Anderson ML. Language deprivation syndrome: a possible neurodevelopmental disorder with sociocultural origins. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2017;52(6):761–76.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Oerbeck B, Ohre B, Zeiner P, Pripp AH, Wagner K, Overgaard KR. What can a national patient registry tell us about psychiatric disorders and reasons for referral to outpatient services in youth with hearing loss? Nordic Journal of Psychiatry. 2021:1–7.

  19. World Health Organization. The World Health Organization Quality of Life assessment (WHOQOL): position paper from the World Health Organization. Soc Sci Med. 1995;41(10):1403–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Lin FR, Niparko JK. Measuring health-related quality of life after pediatric cochlear implantation: A systematic review. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2006;70(10):1695–706.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Roland L, Fischer C, Tran K, Rachakonda T, Kallogjeri D, Lieu JE. Quality of Life in Children with Hearing Impairment: Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2016;155(2):208–19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Aanondsen CM, Jozefiak T, Heiling K, Lydersen S, Rimehaug T. Psychometric properties of the Inventory of Life Quality in children and adolescents in Norwegian Sign Language. BMC Psychology. 2021;9(1):89.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Hintermair M. Quality of life of mainstreamed hearing-impaired children-Results of a study with the Inventory of Life Quality of Children and Youth (ILC). Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother. 2010;38(3):189–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Qi L, Zhang H, Nie R, Xiao A, Wang J, Du Y. Quality of Life of Hearing-Impaired Middle School Students: a Cross-Sectional Study in Hubei Province, China. J Dev Phys Disabil. 2020;32(5):821–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Reeh M, Kroner-Herwig B, Kiese-Himmel C. Health-related quality of life in children with hearing loss in their parents’ perspective. Zeitschrift fur Medizinische Psychologie. 2008;17(4):161–8.

  26. Haukedal CL, Lyxell B, Wie OB. Health-Related Quality of Life With Cochlear Implants: The Children’s Perspective. Ear Hear. 2020;41(2):330–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Haukedal CL, von Koss TJ, Lyxell B, Wie OB. Parents’ Perception of Health-Related Quality of Life in Children With Cochlear Implants: The Impact of Language Skills and Hearing. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2018;61(8):2084–98.

  28. Holte A, Barry MM, Bekkhus M, Borge AIH, Bowes L, Casas F, et al. Psychology of Child Well-Being. In: Ben-Arieh A, Casas F, Frønes I, Korbin JE, editors., et al., Handbook of Child Well-Being: Theories, Methods and Policies in Global Perspective. Dordrecht: Springer, Netherlands; 2014. p. 555–631.

  29. Fellinger J, Holzinger D, Sattel H, Laucht M. Mental health and quality of life in deaf pupils. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;17(7):414–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Jozefiak T, Mattejat F, Remschmidt H. Inventory of Life Quality in Children and Adolescents Manual, Norwegian Version. Stockholm, Sweden: Hogrefe; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Pardo-Guijarro MJ, Martínez-Andrés M, Notario- Pacheco B, Solera-Martínez M, Sánchez-López M, Martínez-Vizcaíno V. Self-Reports Versus Parental Perceptions of Health-Related Quality of Life Among Deaf Children and Adolescents. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2015;20(3):275–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Mattejat F, Remschmidt H. Das Inventar zur Erfassung der Lebensqualitaet bei Kindern un Jugendlichen (ILK) [The Inventory of Quality of Life in Children and Adolescents ILC] Bern. Switzerland: Verlag Hans Huber; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Upton P, Lawford J, Eiser C. Parent-child agreement across child health-related quality of life instruments: a review of the literature. Qual Life Res. 2008;17(6):895–913.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Warner-Czyz AD, Loy B, Roland PS, Tong L, Tobey EA. Parent versus child assessment of quality of life in children using cochlear implants. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2009;73(10):1423–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Aanondsen CM, Heiling K, Nøvik TS, Jozefiak T. Quality of Life, family function and mental health of deaf and hard-of-hearing adolescents in mental health services in Norway – a pilot study. Int J Ment Health Deaf. 2018;4(1):3–13.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Kushalnagar P, Topolski TD, Schick B, Edwards TC, Skalicky AM, Patrick DL. Mode of communication, perceived level of understanding, and perceived quality of life in youth who are deaf or hard of hearing. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2011;16(4):512–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Jozefiak T, Larsson B, Wichstrom L. Changes in quality of life among Norwegian school children: a six-month follow-up study. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2009;7:7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Fitzpatrick EM, Hamel C, Stevens A, Pratt M, Moher D, Doucet SP, et al. Sign Language and Spoken Language for Children With Hearing Loss: A Systematic Review. Pediatrics. 2016;137(1).

  39. Kermit PS. “Hearing” or “Deaf”? Discussing epistemological and methodological issues related to the bioethical discourse on paediatric cochlear implantation. Scand J Disabil Res. 2010;12(2):91–107.

  40. Holzinger D, Hofer J, Dall M. Frühe Prädiktoren der Sprachentwicklung von Kindern mit permanenter Hörstörung. Kindheit und Entwicklung. 2021;30(1):25–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Niparko JK, Tobey EA, Thal DJ, Eisenberg LS, Wang NY, Quittner AL, et al. Spoken language development in children following cochlear implantation. JAMA. 2010;303(15):1498–506.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Yoshinaga-Itano C, Sedey AL, Wiggin M, Mason CA. Language Outcomes Improved Through Early Hearing Detection and Earlier Cochlear Implantation. Otol Neurotol. 2018;39(10):1256–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Stokoe WCJ. Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication system for the deaf. Studies in Linguistics. 1960;8:1–78.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Maxwell-McCaw D, Zea MC. The Deaf Acculturation Scale (DAS): Development and validation of a 58-item measure. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2011;16(3):325–42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Kral A, Kronenberger WG, Pisoni DB, O’Donoghue GM. Neurocognitive factors in sensory restoration of early deafness: a connectome model. Lancet Neurol. 2016;15(6):610–21.

  46. Holzinger D, Dall M, Sanduvete-Chaves S, Saldaña D, Chacón-Moscoso S, Fellinger J. The Impact of Family Environment on Language Development of Children With Cochlear Implants: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Ear Hear. 2020;41(5):1077–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Grønlie SM. Uten hørsel? En bok om hørselshemming. Bergen: Fagbokforlaget; 2005.

  48. Schick B, Marschark M, Spencer PE. Advances in the Sign-Language Development of Deaf Children. New York: Oxford Universirty Press; 2005.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  49. Lu J, Jones A, Morgan G. The impact of input quality on early sign development in native and non-native language learners. J Child Lang. 2016;43:537–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Moeller MP, Schick B. Relations between maternal input and theory of mind understanding in deaf children. Child Dev. 2006;77(3):751–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Hintermair M. Executive functions and behavioral problems in deaf and hard-of-hearing students at general and special schools. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2013;18(3):344–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Fellinger J, Holzinger D, Beitel C, Laucht M, Goldberg DP. The impact of language skills on mental health in teenagers with hearing impairments. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2009;120(2):153–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Ching TYC, Cupples L, Leigh G, Hou S, Wong A. Predicting Quality of Life and Behavior and Emotion from Functional Auditory and Pragmatic Language Abilities in 9-Year-Old Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Children. J Clin Med. 2021;10(22).

  54. Butcher E, Dezateux C, Cortina-Borja M, Knowles RL. Prevalence of permanent childhood hearing loss detected at the universal newborn hearing screen: Systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(7): e0219600.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Brown AS, Cohen P, Greenwald S, Susser E. Nonaffective psychosis after prenatal exposure to rubella. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(3):438–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Cornes AJ, Brown PM. Mental health of Australian deaf adolescents: An investigation using an Auslan version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Deaf Educ Int. 2012;14(3):161–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Caluraud S, Marcolla-Bouchetemblé A, de Barros A, Moreau-Lenoir F, de Sevin E, Rerolle S, et al. Newborn hearing screening: Analysis and outcomes after 100,000 births in Upper-Normandy French region. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2015;79(6):829–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. van Roy B, Groholt B, Heyerdahl S, Clench-Aas J. Self-reported strengths and difficulties in a large Norwegian population 10–19 years: Age and gender specific results of the extended SDQ-questionnaire. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;15(4):189–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. van Roy B, Groholt B, Heyerdahl S, Clench-Aas J. Understanding discrepancies in parent-child reporting of emotional and behavioural problems: Effects of relational and socio-demographic factors. BMC Psychiatry. 2010;10(56).

  60. Archbold S, Lutman ME, Marshall DH. Categories of auditory performance. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol Suppl. 1995;166:312–4.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Allen C, Nikolopoulos TP, Dyar D, O’Donoghue GM. Reliability of a rating scale for measuring speech intelligibility after pediatric cochlear implantation. Otol Neurotol. 2001;22(5):631–3.

  62. Bishop D. The Children's Communication Checklist - Second Edition. Norwegian Version. Järfälla; Sweden: Åtta.45 Tryckeri AB; 2011.

  63. Bishop DV. Development of the Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC): a method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1998;39(6):879–91.

  64. Bishop DVM. The Children’s Communication Checklist - 2. London: Psychological Corporation; 2003.

  65. Reindal L, Brøndbo PH, Jozefiak T. Måleegenskaper ved den norske versjonen av The Children’s Communication Checklist - Second Edition (CCC-2). PsykTestBarn. 2022;12(1).

  66. Roid GH, Miller LJ, Pomplun M, Koch C. Leiter International Performance Scale Manual (3rd ed.). Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting; 2013.

  67. Goodman R. Psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:1337–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. van Roy B, Veenstra M, Clench-Aas J. Conctruct validity of the five-factor Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) in pre-, early, and late adolescence Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49:1304–12.

  69. Aanondsen CM, Jozefiak T, Heiling K, Rimehaug T. Validation of the Strengths and Difficulties Self-Report in Norwegian Sign Language. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 2019:1–14.

  70. Van Buuren S. Flexible imputation of missing data. 2nd ed. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman and Hall/CRC; 2018.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  71. Rodwell L, Lee KJ, Romaniuk H, Carlin JB. Comparison of methods for imputing limited-range variables: a simulation study. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2014;14(1):1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Institute of Public Health. Quality of Life and Mental Health Among Children and Adolescents in Norway. Public Health Report - Health Status in Norway Oslo: Institute of Public Health; 2016.

  73. Jozefiak T, J LW, Lydersen S. Change Over 11–13 Year Periods in Quality of Life, Emotional Problems and Negative Stressful Life Events Among 13–17 Year Old Students. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2022.

  74. Bor W, Dean AJ, Najman J, Hayatbakhsh R. Are child and adolescent mental health problems increasing in the 21st century? A systematic review. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2014;48(7):606–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Pitchforth J, Fahy K, Ford T, Wolpert M, Viner RM, Hargreaves DS. Mental health and well-being trends among children and young people in the UK, 1995–2014: analysis of repeated cross-sectional national health surveys. Psychol Med. 2019;49(8):1275–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Panter-Brick C, Burgess A, Eggerman M, McAllister F, Pruett K, Leckman JF. Practitioner Review: Engaging fathers – recommendations for a game change in parenting interventions based on a systematic review of the global evidence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(11):1187–212.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Fellinger J, Holzinger D, Gerich J, Goldberg D. Mental distress and quality of life in the hard of hearing. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2007;115(3):243–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Fellinger J, Holzinger D, Dobner U, Gerich J, Lehner R, Lenz G, et al. Mental distress and quality of life in a deaf population. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2005;40(9):737–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Hintermair M. Prevalence of socioemotional problems in deaf and hard of hearing children in Germany. Am Ann Deaf. 2007;152(3):320–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Laugen NJ, Jacobsen KH, Rieffe C, Wichstrom L. Predictors of psychosocial outcomes in hard-of- hearing preschool children. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. 2016;21(3):259–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Chmiel R, Sutton L, Jenkins H. Quality of Life in children with cochlear implants. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol. 2000;109:103–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Yoshinaga-Itano C. The missing link in language learning of children who are deaf or hard of hearing: pragmatics. Cochlear Implants Int. 2015;16(Suppl 1):S53–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Rønning JA, Handegaard BH, Sourander A, Mørch WT. The Strengths and Difficulties Self-Report Questionnaire as a screening instrument in Norwegian community samples. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;13(2):73–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was a collaboration between the Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry at St. Olav's University Hospital and the Regional Centre for Child and Youth Mental Health and Child Welfare (RKBU), Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim. We would like to thank STATPED, Tiller Upper Secondary School and the audiology department at St. Olavs Hospital for allowing us to recruit the participants and all the children, adolescents, and parents participating in this study.

Funding

Open access funding provided by the Norwegian University of Science and Technology. This work was supported by grants from the Regional Hospital Trust in Central Norway, the Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry at St. Olavs Hospital; the Research Department at St. Olavs Hospital; Regional Centre for Child and Youth Mental Health and Child Welfare (RKBU Central Norway); and the Gidske and Peter Jacob Sørensens Research Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CMA, TJ, KH and TR contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation and data collection were performed by CMA. Data analysis was performed by CMA and ST. The first draft of the manuscript was written by CMA, all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chris Margaret Aanondsen.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and  consent to participate

Study approval was given by the Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics (reference number: 2015/1739/REK midt). Written informed consent was obtained from the parents and adolescents ≥16 prior to inclusion, as well as oral/signed informed consent from the children <16.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The first author was involved in the translation of the ILC-NSL and SDQ-NSL but has not gained any economic benefits from these or retained any financial interests in the ILC-NSL or SDQ-NSL. The second author contributed to the publishing of the Norwegian ILC (ILC-NOR) by Hogrefe and receives royalties from this publisher. On behalf of the authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Table 8 Table A Prevalence of mental health problems in DHH, HH and TH children based on complete case analysis (CC) data. Estimates and 95% confidence intervals (CI) based on Wald

Appendix 2

Table 9 Norwegian version of the Sign Language Understanding Scale (SUS) VTF—Vurdering av tegnspråklig forståelseSett et kryss ved nivået, som passer best på barnet
Table 10 Norwegian version of the Sign Language Production Scale (SUS) VTP—Vurdering av tegnspråklig produksjon Sett et kryss ved nivået, som passer best på barnet

Appendix 3

Table 11 Table B1 Missing values handled by multiple imputation

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Aanondsen, C.M., Jozefiak, T., Lydersen, S. et al. Deaf and hard-of-hearing children and adolescents’ mental health, Quality of Life and communication. BMC Psychiatry 23, 297 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-04787-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-04787-9

Keywords